10,000 Matching Annotations
  1. Feb 2018
    1. On 2014 Jan 08, Tom Kindlon commented:

      Numerous exercise abnormalities been found in Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS)

      One curious omission from this interesting review[1] is Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS), which is also sometimes known as Myalgic Encephalomyelitis (ME). An abnormal response to physical activity is an essential part of widely used ME/CFS clinical criteria for adults[2] and children[3]. The most frequently used research criteria for CFS [4] require that patients, along with suffering from chronic debilitating fatigue lasting at least 6 months, have at least 4 out of a list of 8 symptoms, one of which is “postexertional malaise lasting more than 24 hours.”

      There is a growing body of research on abnormal responses to exercise in CFS. A recent review[5] covers the issue in a fairly comprehensive manner - here's a summary: “Exertion induces post-exertional malaise with a decreased physical performance/aerobic capacity, increased muscoskeletal pain, neurocognitive impairment, "fatigue", and weakness, and a long lasting "recovery" time. This can be explained by findings that exertion may amplify pre-existing pathophysiological abnormalities underpinning ME/CFS, such as inflammation, immune dysfunction, oxidative and nitrosative stress, channelopathy, defective stress response mechanisms and a hypoactive hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis.”

      High rates of adverse reactions to graded exercise programs have been reported in patients with CFS – sometimes 50% or greater[6].

      CFS remains a fairly poorly understood condition. There is increasing evidence that CFS is heterogeneous and this heterogeneity could be of relevance to therapeutic programs involving exercise [7,8]. Those interested in researching abnormal responses to physical activity, including dysregulated inflammatory responses, could find much of interest if they chose to study CFS.

      References:

      1) Cooper DM, Radom-Aizik S, Schwindt C, Zaldivar F Jr. Dangerous exercise: lessons learned from dysregulated inflammatory responses to physical activity. J Appl Physiol. 2007 Aug;103(2):700-9. Epub 2007 May 10.

      2) Carruthers BM, Jain AK, De Meirleir KL, Petersn DL, Klimas MD, Lerner AM, Bested AC, Flor-Henry P, Joshi P, Powles ACP, Sherkey JA, van de Sande MI (2003). "Myalgic encephalomyelitis.chronic fatigue syndrome: Clinical working definition, diagnostic and treatment protocols". Journal of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome 11 (1): 7-36.

      3) Jason LA, Porter N, Shelleby E, Bell DS, Lapp CW, Rowe K, & De Meirleir K. (2008). A case definition for children with Myalgic Encephalomyelitis/ chronic fatigue syndrome. Clinical Medicine: Pediatrics, 1, 53-57.

      4) Fukuda K, Straus SE, Hickie I, Sharpe MC, Dobbins JG, Komaroff A. The chronic fatigue syndrome: a comprehensive approach to its definition and study. International Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Study Group. Ann Intern Med. 1994 Dec 15;121(12):953-9.

      5) Twisk FN, Maes M. A review on cognitive behavorial therapy (CBT) and graded exercise therapy (GET) in myalgic encephalomyelitis (ME) / chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS): CBT/GET is not only ineffective and not evidence-based, but also potentially harmful for many patients with ME/CFS. Neuro Endocrinol Lett. 2009;30(3):284-99.

      6) Kindlon T, Goudsmit EM. Graded exercise for chronic fatigue syndrome: too soon to dismiss reports of adverse reactions. J Rehabil Med. 2010 Feb;42(2):184; author reply 184-6.

      7) Jason LA, Corradi K, Torres-Harding S, Taylor RR, & King C. Chronic fatigue syndrome: The need for subtypes. Neuropsychology Review 2005, 15, 29-58.

      8) Kindlon T. Stratification using biological factors should be performed in more CFS studies. Psychol Med. 2010 Feb;40(2):352. Epub 2009 Oct 12.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Jun 23, Prof.Dr.Jogenananda Pramanik commented:

      Delayed diagnosis of MDR-TB and TB with co-infection of HIV – A major concern!

      Tuberculosis (TB) is emerging as an dreaded communicable disease in Malaysia, with the death rate even higher than that of dengue (Ministry of Health, Malay Mail, 2014). Besides being a deadly disease, TB is also an expensive disease which is adding additional economic burden to the health system of the country. Delayed diagnosis of tuberculosis (TB) may lead to an increased period of infectivity in the community, a delay in treatment and a severe form of the disease like MDR-TB and TB with HIV co-infection.(1-7) We appreciate the insightful attempts of Chang CT1, Esterman A.(1) in analyzing the delay in diagnosis of tuberculosis in two perspectives: (A) period between the onset of TB symptoms to any first medical consultation (patients' delay); and (B) period between the first medical consultation to the diagnosis of TB (diagnosis delay). Moreover, we would like to emphasize that there is a need to reduce the time lag between the first inoculations of patient’s sample in solid or liquid culture media till appearance of macroscopically detectable AFB colonies. One of the priorities in the control of tuberculosis is to cure patients with the disease, given that the most effective way to prevent transmission and avoid the appearance of drug-resistant strains is to detect cases of tuberculosis early and treat them appropriately. As a routine practice, all patients suspected of having PTB should submit at least two sputum specimens for microscopic examination in a quality-assured laboratory. These techniques while remaining an important baseline modality of investigations currently, they lack the desired sensitivity or are time consuming. Hence, Nucleic Acid Amplification Tests for the detection of Mycobacterium tuberculosis are useful tools for rapid diagnosis of TB, both pulmonary and extra-pulmonary. These tests are also useful for the rapid screening of patients suspected of MDR-TB. Nucleic Acid Amplification Tests (NAAT) provide rapid results within 24 - 48 hours and has greater PPV (>95%) with AFB smear positive specimens. They have the ability to confirm rapidly the presence of Mycobacterium in 50 - 80% AFB smear negative, culture positive specimens. However, Molecular assays may supplement but cannot replace conventional methods for culture and sensitivity. Moreover their high cost and requirement for sophisticated laboratory infrastructure limit their use in routine diagnosis. Laboratories with advanced infrastructure are limited in most of the peripheral diagnostic laboratories in Malaysia(6-7).

      To balance the lack of sophisticated laboratory infrastructure for performing TB identification and drug sensitivity tests to exclude MDR-TB cases with speed and accuracy, there is an urgent need of inexpensive, non-invasive, robust diagnostic and culture sensitivity test. To achieve this goal an attempt was made to incorporate thyroxine hormone in solid and liquid media for culture of AFB in vitro (8-12) Presently, we planned to evaluate the effectiveness of this thyroxine supplemented culture media for culture of AFB from clinical samples in vitro and also to standardize use of this method in comparison to routine AFB culture procedure. This study may prove to be a useful technique for rapid culture isolation of AFB in vitro.

      References:

      1.Chang CT1, Esterman A.Diagnostic delay among pulmonary tuberculosis patients in Sarawak, Malaysia: a cross-sectional study. Rural Remote Health. 2007 Apr-Jun;7(2):667. Epub 2007 May 11. 2. Alison Rodger, Shabbar Jaffar, Stuar Paynter et al., Delay in the diagnosis of pulmonary tuberculosis, London, 1998-2000: analysis of surveillance data: BMJ 2003; 326 doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.326.7395.909 (Published 26 April 2003)<br> 3. Zahar JR, Azoulay E, Klement E et al., Delayed treatment contributes to mortality in ICU patients with severe active pulmonary tuberculosis and acute respiratory failure. Intensive Care Med 2001; 27: 513–520 4. Wares D.. Delay in diagnosis of tuberculosis: of remaining concern in England and Wales. J Public Health Med 1999; 21: 355–356 5. Aldhubhani AH1, lzham MI, Pazilah I, Anaam MS (2013)Effect of delay in diagnosis on the rate of tuberculosis among close contacts of tuberculosis patients. East Mediterr Health J. 2013 Oct;19(10):837-42. 6. Ying Li1†, John Ehiri2†, Shenglan Tang et al., 3(2013)Research article Factors associated with patient, and diagnostic delays in Chinese TB patients: a systematic review and meta-analysis:BMC Medicine 2013, 11:156 doi:10.1186/1741-7015-11-156 7. Timothy William123, Uma Parameswaran12, Wai Khew Lee4 et al., Pulmonary tuberculosis in outpatients in Sabah, Malaysia: advanced disease but low incidence of HIV co-infection: BMC Infectious Diseases 2015, 15:32 doi:10.1186/s12879-015-0758-6 8. Pramanik J, Lodam AN, Reddy MVR, Narang P and Harinath BC. Increased yield of excretory-secretory antigen with thyroxine supplementation in in vitro culture of tubercle bacilli. Ind. J. tub. 1997; 44: 185-190. 9. Pramanik J, Lodam AN, Badole CM, Reddy MVR, Patond KR and Harinath BC. Detection of tubercular antibody and antigen in sera of bone and joint tuberculosis. Ind. J. Clin. Biochem. 2000; 15 (1): 22–28. 10. Lodam AN, Pramanik J, Reddy MVR and Harinath BC. Diagnostic potential of fractionated Mycobacterium tuberculosis H37Ra excretory-secretory (EST-DE1) antigen in pulmonary tuberculosis. Ind. J. Clin. Biochem. 1997; 12 (1): 71-77. 11. Pramanik.J. BMJ.2003.http://www.bmj.com/rapid-response/2011/10/30/delays-diagnosis-tuberculosislet-us-use-thyroxine-supplemented-culture-med: Delays in diagnosis of tuberculosis? Let us use thyroxin supplemented culture medium for early lab-diagnosis. 12. Pramanik.J. BMJ: 2004.http://www.bmj.com/rapid-response/2011/10/30/early-diagnosis-tuberculosis-reported-third-world-country Early diagnosis of tuberculosis-reported from third world country:A research letter from India.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 01, Christopher Southan commented:

      This is Open Access with full text in the Transactions link. Note also the approaches outlined were used in a 2013 paper on the evolutionary history of BACE1 and BACE2 and their substrates http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24381583


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jun 25, Chandra Boosani commented:

      Many articles from this journal are not found on their website including this one.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jan 11, Brett Snodgrass commented:

      Dear Authors,

      Thank you for the excellent article. Please provide your kind attention to the distinction between the Thebesian veins and the vessels of Wearn.

      1. http://bit.ly/JTWearn

      2. http://bit.ly/vasaThebesii

      3. http://bit.ly/ThebesianByPratt

      4. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22704295

      The vessels described are probably better described as vessels of Wearn as they are (1) not Thebesian veins*, (2) not studied by Thebesius, and (3) they were described by Wearn et al.

      Given that there was the prior intervention of stent placement, it may be possible that these connections are "true fistula" in that they are not normally present. Perhaps angiogenesis formed the fine connections identified by injection of contrast material into the left coronary artery.

      Another hypothesis is that the procedure caused tissue injury which resulted in vasodilation and increased prominence of the normally present vessels of Wearn.

      My opinion is that accurate anatomic terminology is a basic principle underlying good medical science, and I ask others to consider whether the aforementioned definitions are appropriate. If this comment is not helpful, please let me know how it might be improved.

      Comments and suggestions are welcome.

      Thank you very much.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jan 08, Tom Kindlon commented:

      Does the use of the "Role emotional" subscale of the SF-36 help with sensitivity and specificity rates? Can we find out the prevalence rate if this subscale hadn't been used?

      It is to be welcomed that attempts are being made to operationalize the CDC (94) CFS criteria [1], enabling easier comparisons between studies and making it easier for researchers to try to replicate findings. So, for example, having some sort of numerical value on a symptom so that one can say whether a symptom is present or not in a patient seems to be a good idea.

      However, if one is aiming to do this, it would seem preferable to choose methods that have good sensitivity and specificity rates for the condition in question. And it's questionnable whether the methods used in this study have good sensitivity and specificity.

      The authors claim that they "used stringent (i.e., <= 25th percentile population norms on any of the 4 SF-36 scales) to define severe functional impairment". One of the SF-36 subscales in question is the "role emotional" subscale. This involves questions such as: "During the past 4 weeks, have you accomplished less than you would like as a result of any emotional problems?" Does this really capture whether there has been a "substantial reduction in previous levels of .. personal activities"? [Full quote from paper[1]: 1) clinically evaluated, unexplained, persistent or relapsing chronic fatigue that is of new or definite onset [has not been lifelong]; is not the result of ongoing exertion; is not substantially alleviated by rest; and results in substantial reduction in previous levels of occupational, educational, social, or personal activities] Perhaps the other three sub-scales cover this? For example, a better measure of whether the condition is having an effect on somebody's "personal activities" might be got from using the physical functioning subscales which asks about ability to walk distances, bath or dress oneself, etc. If this score is low, it's likely one's ability to do "personal activities" has been impaired.

      Baraniuk[2] used the CDC '94 not operationalized in the same way as this study and found that CFS patients scores did have lower scores on some of the SF-36 subscales - but role emotional was one of the ones that weren't different (the others that weren't different were mental health and general health change). Would it be possible for the authors to calculate the all important overall prevalence rate if those people who only satisfied this part of the "functional impairment" criteria are excluded? This data would be be useful not just in the US but around the world - countries around the world have been depending on the US to undertake such large scale (and expensive) studies on CFS.

      Even before the recent broadening of the criteria, it had been felt by some that the CDC '94 criteria lacked specificity. For example, Kennedy[3] investigated "patients with self-reported symptoms which developed sporadically (sCFS, n=48); after Gulf War service (GW, n=24); and following exposure to organophosphate insecticides (OP, n=25)" all of whom fulfilled the CDC '94 criteria[1]. Based on their findings, they concluded that "differences in simple, easily performed clinical outcome measurements can be observed between groups of patients, all of whom fulfill the CDC-1994 criteria for CFS. It is likely that their response to treatment may also vary. The specificity of the CFS case definition should be improved to define more homogeneous groups of patients for the purposes of treatment and research."

      Perhaps what is required is a totally new set of criteria?

      References:

      [1] Fukuda, K., Straus, S.E., Hickie, I., Sharpe, M.C., Dobbins, J.G., & Komaroff, A. (1994). The chronic fatigue syndrome: A comprehensive approach to its definition and study. Annals of Internal Medicine, 121 (12):953-959. http://www.annals.org/cgi/content/full/121/12/953

      [2] James N Baraniuk, Begona Casado, Hilda Maibach, Daniel J Clauw, Lewis K Pannell and Sonja Hess S. A chronic fatigue syndrome - related proteome in human cerebrospinal fluid. BMC Neurology 2005, 5:22 doi:10.1186/1471-2377-5-22http://www.biomedcentral.com/1471-2377/5/22

      [3] Kennedy G, Abbot NC, Spence V.A, Underwood C, Belch JJF. The specificity of the CDC-1994 criteria for chronic fatigue syndrome: comparison of health status in three groups of patients who fulfil the criteria. Ann Epidemiol 2004; 14: 95–100.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Jan 08, Tom Kindlon commented:

      Questioning the use of the Role Emotional (RE) subscale of the SF-36 questionnaire in the diagnosis of CFS

      As background to my previous comment, I thought I'd point out that if people would like to see what makes up the Role Emotional (RE) subscale of the SF-36, a copy of a sample SF-36 questionnaire can be seen at: https://clinicalresearch.ccf.org/fsgs/docs/WEBdocs/Form36.pdf .It is question 14 i.e. 3 questions with only yes or no as possible answers.

      The cut off point used in the current study is less than or equal to a score of 66 [1], so two "yes" answers (out of the three questions) is the cut off point for functional impairment.

      References:

      [1] Chronic Fatigue Syndrome – A clinically empirical approach to its definition and study. William C Reeves, Dieter Wagner, Rosane Nisenbaum, James F Jones, Brian Gurbaxani, Laura Solomon, Dimitris A Papanicolaou, Elizabeth R Unger, Suzanne D Vernon and Christine Heim BMC Medicine 2005, 3:19 doi:10.1186/1741-7015-3-19 http://www.biomedcentral.com/1741-7015/3/19


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jan 06, Tom Kindlon commented:

      Caution required when extrapolating prevalence rates to the full population

      This editorial [1] says, with regard to the CDC study[2]: "The CDC has now repeated and extended the Wichita study in Georgia, and found a prevalence of between six and ten times greater, with 2.5% of the population suffering from CFS. If this prevalence was both accurate and representative of the USA as a whole, this would suggest that some 7.5 million Americans were sufferers, compared to the previous estimates of 0.7 to 1.2 million."

      Before the 7.5 million figure is quoted, it might be useful to point out that the figure makes a number of assumptions, including that the prevalence rate for those under 18 and over 60 would be similar. However previous studies have suggested this is unlikely to be the case, with prevalence rates for young children in particular being much lower.

      The round figure of 7.5 million would be equivalent to a population of 295,275,591. Using this data the population estimate for 2005 was 296,410,404 (i.e. a similar figure). Using the same data: The population under 18 years was 73,469,580, the population over 60 was 49,791,976 and population aged 18-60 was 173,148,444.

      For a population of those aged over 18 and under 60 of this size, a back of the envelope calculation for CFS prevalence using the prevalence rate of 2.64%[2] would give: (173,148,444*0.0264)= 4,397,971.

      References:

      [1] How common is chronic fatigue syndrome; how long is a piece of string? Peter D White Population Health Metrics 2007, 5:6 doi:10.1186/1478-7954-5-6

      [2] Prevalence of chronic fatigue syndrome in metropolitan, urban, and rural GeorgiaWilliam C Reeves , James F Jones , Elizabeth Maloney , Christine Heim , David C Hoaglin , Roumiana S Boneva , Marjorie Morrissey and Rebecca Devlin. Population Health Metrics 2007, 5:5 doi:10.1186/1478-7954-5-5


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Jan 06, Tom Kindlon commented:

      Reference to obesity a red herring?

      In his editorial, Prof White says: "Georgia may not be representative of the USA as a whole. For instance, we do not know the body mass index (BMI) of the Georgian sample. The Wichita sample of CFS cases contained 43% of subjects with a BMI of 30 or over, representing significant obesity [9]. This compares with 20% in the USA as a whole [13]. Since obesity is associated with fatigue [14], a similar proportion in Georgia might inflate the prevalence of CFS."

      Firstly, just because obesity can cause fatigue is quite a different from obesity causing the syndrome CFS. Using this logic, perhaps we should be saying that prevalence studies on any condition which can involve disabling fatigue (for example multiple sclerosis) may be questionable if there is a higher rate of obesity within the sample population. It is important to consider cause and effect i.e. just because people with a condition may be more obese when they are sampled years after having the illness is not the same as saying they were more obese before getting the illness and this caused them to develop the condition.

      Also the Wichita study[1], to which Prof. White refers, found a relatively low prevalence rate, of 0.235%, for CFS compared to other random-number studies including the one under review. So it seems curious to refer to this study to try to justify a hypothesis that the obesity rate in the Georgia study artificially increased the prevalence rate.

      References:

      [1] Prevalence and Incidence of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome in Wichita, Kansas Michele Reyes, PhD; Rosane Nisenbaum, PhD; David C. Hoaglin, PhD; Elizabeth R. Unger, PhD, MD; Carol Emmons, PhD; Bonnie Randall, MCP; John A. Stewart, MD; Susan Abbey, MD; James F. Jones, MD; Nelson Gantz, MD; Sarah Minden, MD; William C. Reeves, MD, MSPH Arch Intern Med. 2003;163:1530-1536.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2014 Jan 06, Tom Kindlon commented:

      Obesity and arbitrary criteria

      Firstly, I thought I would clarify that I did not make my point about obesity rates based simply on the one study, the Wichita study[1]: the Chicago Study[2] found a prevalence of 0.422% using the same (or very similar) methodology and method of operationalizing the criteria as the Wichita study, producing a much higher score than the 0.235% score found in the Wichita study.

      However this correspondence has caused to me to reflect on the issue: I still remain to be convinced that because the residents of Wichita were more obese than the general population, the prevalence figure for CFS (as defined then) of 0.235% was artificially increased; however perhaps if the new broadened criteria lack sensitivity and specificity, the figures in the latest studies could be artificially inflated because of a higher background obesity rate?

      I think there is an important issue of a lack of sensitivity and specificity with the new method of operationalizing the criteria. As Peter White says, the current criteria are "arbitrary". Whether they are being used by a "jobbing physician", an epidemiologist or a researcher, one of the aims of criteria should be that they have good sensitivity and specificity rates. Perhaps a direction for discourse and research in the future should be trying to arrive at CFS criteria that reach that aim?

      If necessary, having different criteria for different circumstances: for example, have one set of criteria when looking for expensive biological work but perhaps less stringent criteria for use in some clinical settings?

      References:

      [1] Prevalence and Incidence of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome in Wichita, Kansas Michele Reyes, PhD; Rosane Nisenbaum, PhD; David C. Hoaglin, PhD; Elizabeth R. Unger, PhD, MD; Carol Emmons, PhD; Bonnie Randall, MCP; John A. Stewart, MD; Susan Abbey, MD; James F. Jones, MD; Nelson Gantz, MD; Sarah Minden, MD; William C. Reeves, MD, MSPH Arch Intern Med. 2003;163:1530-1536.

      [2]. Jason LA, Richman JA, Rademaker AW, Jordan KM, Plioplys AV, Taylor RR, McCready W, Huang CF, Plioplys S: A community-based study of chronic fatigue syndrome. Arch Int Med 1999, 159:2129-2137.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    4. On 2014 Jan 06, Tom Kindlon commented:

      Caution required when making numerical comparisons between Wessely (1997) and the current study

      In his editorial[1], Prof. White says: "Comorbid psychiatric conditions may have inflated the prevalence. A previous study found an equally high point prevalence of CFS (2.6%), by surveying United Kingdom primary care patients [10]. However, when those patients who also had a comorbid psychiatric disorder were excluded, the prevalence fell to 0.5%."

      Reference to this paper[2] is also made in the editorial's concluding paragraph and in the accompanying Reeves paper[3].

      A close inspection of table 2 of the referenced paper[2] reveals some strange figures (with regard to the estimates for the CDC '94 criteria mentioned above): (i) The Oxford criteria for CFS were found to have a lower prevalence, of 2.2%. Given that the CDC 94 criteria would be seen as more restrictive than the Oxford criteria (e.g. requiring symptoms as well as fatigue lasting six months), this suggests an error with one or both of the figures? (ii) the mean and 95% confidence intervals given for the prevalence rates without co-morbid psychological disorders for CFS (CDC 94) are given as 0.5 (0.1, 0.3) which makes no sense (the confidence intervals should be above and below the mean).

      So these two observations mean that I'm not sure how much faith should be placed with some of the figures given in that study.

      The methodology of the Wessely study was also different, using attendance at primary care physicians to screen for patients, which could lead to skewed data. The random number methodology in the Reeves study seems stronger.

      It should also be remembered that the authors of the Reeves study[3] did exclude many patients with psychological disorders before giving the diagnosis of CFS. So even if one accepts the curious data presented in Table 2 in Wessely et al[2], it seems unlikely we can extrapolate from the drop in the figures found the Wessely study to produce a similar drop in figures found in the current study[3].

      References:

      [1] How common is chronic fatigue syndrome; how long is a piece of string? Peter D White Population Health Metrics 2007, 5:6 doi:10.1186/1478-7954-5-6

      [2] Wessely S, Chalder T, Hirsch S, Wallace P, Wright D. The prevalence and morbidity of chronic fatigue and chronic fatigue syndrome: a prospective primary care study. Am J Pub Health 1997, 87:1449-1455.Available online at:http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=1380968

      [3] Prevalence of chronic fatigue syndrome in metropolitan, urban, and rural Georgia. William C Reeves, James F Jones, Elizabeth Maloney, Christine Heim, David C Hoaglin, Roumiana S Boneva , Marjorie Morrissey and Rebecca Devlin. Population Health Metrics 2007, 5:5 doi:10.1186/1478-7954-5-5


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 May 07, Jeff Kiefer commented:

      DAVID is continuously being used as evidenced by its numerous citations in current biomedical literature http://bit.ly/1FS1JgT. However, the data resources used by DAVID appear to not have been updated since 2009 http://david.abcc.ncifcrf.gov/helps/update.html. The fact that DAVID has not been updated going on 5 years calls into question the current utility of using this tool.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2016 May 26, Jeff Kiefer commented:

      It looks like DAVID has been updated May 2016 https://david-d.ncifcrf.gov/content.jsp?file=release.html.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:

      Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Oct 27, Tom Kindlon commented:

      "Recovery" (from fatigue) does not necessarily mean recovery from CFS (in terms, for example, of activity levels)

      In this paper, some variant of the word "recovered" is used 12 times, generally to describe some of the Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS) patients who had taken part in a cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT) trial for CFS[1].

      Actometer data from this study has subsequently been released[2]. This paper reported that CBT did not cause an increase in physical activity at the end of treatment. Before CBT, the patients did a mean 67.4 units of activity (standard deviation(SD): 21.8); following CBT, patients did 68.8 units of activity (SD: 25.2). The authors report that a previous study [3] found healthy controls had a mean Actometer score of 91 (S.D.=25). There was also no increase in activity levels in two other CBT trials examined[4,5].

      The authors[2] also found that in the three CBT studies, changes in physical activity were not related to changes in fatigue. So "recovery from fatigue" does not necessarily mean recovery from CFS in terms of achieving normal activity levels.

      In another Dutch CBT study[6], 37% of patients were said to be recovered from fatigue, where recovery was defined as no longer scoring on any of the 3 negative factors of the Fatigue Quality List (FQL). However when low scores on other questionnaires were also required to fulfil "full recovery", only 23% of the sample were said to be in "full recovery". Note that only subjective measures were used in that definition of full recovery - a return to a normal level of activity, as measured by an actometer, was not required.

      Incidentally, a paper was subsequently published which also examined language used to describe fatigue by CFS patients and healthy controls[7]. It is slightly different as phrases rather than adjectives were used to describe the fatigue e.g. "Mentally tired after the slightest effort", "Lack the energy to talk to anyone", etc. Factor analyses revealed a five-factor structure for participants with ME/CFS but only a one-factor solution for the control group. The five factors for fatigue in ME/CFS participants were described as: "Post-Exertional", "Wired", "Brain-Fog", "Energy" and "Flu-Like".

      References:

      [1] Prins JB, Bleijenberg G, Bazelmans E, Elving L, de Boo Th, Severens JL, van der Wilt GJ, Spinhoven Ph, van der Meer JWM: Cognitive behaviour therapy for chronic fatigue syndrome: A multicentre randomised controlled trial. Lancet 2001, 357:841-847.

      [2] Wiborg JF, Knoop H, Stulemeijer M, Prins JB, Bleijenberg G. How does cognitive behaviour therapy reduce fatigue in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome? The role of physical activity. Psychol Med. 2010 Jan 5:1 -7. [Epub ahead of print]

      [3] van der Werf SP, Prins JB, Vercoulen JH, van der Meer JW, Bleijenberg G (2000). Identifying physical activity patterns in chronic fatigue syndrome using actigraphic assessment. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 49, 373–379.

      [4] Knoop H, van der Meer JW, Bleijenberg G (2008). Guided self-instructions for people with chronic fatigue syndrome: randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry 193, 340-341.

      [5] Stulemeijer M, de Jong LW, Fiselier TJ, Hoogveld SW, Bleijenberg G (2005). Cognitive behaviour therapy for adolescents with chronic fatigue syndrome: randomised controlled trial. British Medical Journal 330. Published online : 7 December 2004. doi :10.1136/bmj.38301.587106.63.

      [6] Knoop H, Bleijenberg G, Gielissen MFM, Van der Meer JWM, White PD: Is a full recovery possible after cognitive behavioural therapy for chronic fatigue syndrome? Psychother Psychosom 2007, 76:171-176.

      [7] Jason, L.A., Jessen, T., Porter, N., Boulton, A., Njoku, M.G., & Friedberg, F. Examining types of fatigue among individuals with ME/CFS. Disability Studies Quarterly, 2009, 29, 3. Full text at: http://www.dsq-sds.org/article/view/938/1113


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Jul 18, Jan Tunér commented:

      A problem with most studies trying to treat tinnitus with laser light is the lack of proper diagnosis of the patients. Quite a few of these have a somatosensory background (the problem is basicly muscluar). Irradiation into the ear will then have no effect.

      References: Bjorne A, Agerberg G. Reduction in sick leave and costs to society of patients with Ménière´s disease after treatment of temporomandibular and cervical spine disorders: A controlled 6-year cost-benefit study. Cranio. 2003; 21 (2): 136-143. Bernhardt O, Gesch D, Schwahn C, Bitter K et al. Signs of temporomandibular disorders in tinnitus patients and in a population-based group of volunteers: results of the Study of Health in Pomerania. J Oral Rehabil. 2004; 31 (4): 311-319. Levine RA, Abel M, Cheng H. CNS somatosensory-auditory interactions elicit or modulate tinnitus. Exp Brain Res. 2003; 153 (4): 643-648. Tullberg M, Ernberg M. Long-term effect on tinnitus by treatment of temporomandibular disorders: a two-year follow-up by questionnaire. Acta Odontol Scand. 2006; 64 (2): 89- 96.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Dec 18, Ahmed Adeel commented:

      The figures given for PCR-corrected treatment failure with AS/SP do not seem to be consistent in the Abstract and in the Results sections. This needs to be clarified.

      ABSTRACT:"However, when PCR-corrected, 6.5% (5/77) of patients treated with AS/SP maintained parasites from their primary infection."

      RESULTS: "In the AS/SP group, five patients (41.7 %) showed different allelic forms and were classified as reinfection, while seven (58.3) retained the same allelic pattern and were classified as recrudescence of parasites."


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Sep 24, Morten Oksvold commented:

      There is a response to this reply by Kaufmann T et al., which is well worth to read. This response was published in the same number of Cell, but unfortunately hidden as a "linked article" and not searchable in PubMed.

      Title of the response: "Does Bid Play a Role in the DNA Damage Response?".

      Link: http://www.cell.com/cell/fulltext/S0092-8674(07)00842-2


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Aug 21, Morten Oksvold commented:

      Please note that this article represents one of eleven publications which were found to contain false data, after an investigation led by ORI in 2015:

      https://ori.hhs.gov/content/case-summary-dasmahapatra-girija

      This article should therefor not be cited.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Oct 25, FREDERICK DOMANN commented:

      U87MG cells are not what they used to be !!

      The U87MG cells used in this study were obtained from ATCC. A recent (2016) DNA sequencing study by Bengt Westermark revealed that these cells were likely contaminated at some point as their genetic signature did not match the parental tumor of their origin. Below are links to the story and an interview with Dr. Westermark.

      http://www.igp.uu.se/research/neuro-oncology/bengt-westermark/

      http://www.the-scientist.com/?articles.view/articleNo/46929/title/Popular-Tumor-Cell-Line-Contaminated/

      http://stm.sciencemag.org/content/8/354/354re3


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Jan 03, Harri Hemila commented:

      The review by Caruso TJ, 2007 is misleading about the effect of zinc on the common cold

      In their systematic review, Caruso TJ, 2007 identified 14 zinc trials. They used the quality scoring approach so that for the identified trials they gave 1 point for each of 11 quality items when the requirements were satisfied. In 2 tables and 1 figure, Caruso et al. described the distribution of quality scores and the individual quality features of the trials.

      Caruso TJ, 2007 considered that only studies with the full 11 points were valid: “Four studies met all 11 criteria. Three of these studies reported no therapeutic effect from zinc lozenge or nasal spray. One study reported positive results from zinc nasal spray.” On the basis of this 3 vs. 1 comparison (so called “vote counting”) Caruso et al. concluded that “the therapeutic effectiveness of zinc lozenges has yet to be established.” They proposed that the positive findings for zinc could be explained by methodological faults in the trials.

      The approach to evaluate the quality of trials by a set of explicit criteria was initiated by Thomas Chalmers in the 1980s, see Chalmers TC, 1981. Thereafter dozens of quality scales have been developed. However, the approach was not successful and indeed it is discouraged eg in the Cochrane Handbook (sec 8.3.3; Jan 2015), which states that:

      “The use of [quality] scales for assessing quality or risk of bias is explicitly discouraged in Cochrane reviews. While the approach offers appealing simplicity, it is not supported by empirical evidence. Calculating a summary score inevitably involves assigning ‘weights’ to different items in the scale, and it is difficult to justify the weights assigned. Furthermore, scales have been shown to be unreliable assessments of validity and they are less likely to be transparent to users of the review. It is preferable to use simple approaches for assessing validity that can be fully reported (i.e. how each trial was rated on each criterion).”

      One major problem of quality scoring is the focus on reporting in contrast to the scientific quality of the trial. For example, Caruso et al. gave 1 point if there was “measurement of dropout rate” in the trial. This means that a trial might report a high dropout rate, which means low scientific quality, yet the trial would get 1 point from Caruso et al., because the high dropout rate was reported explicitly. Caruso et al. also gave 1 point for “sample size calculation” which is important when a trial is planned, because it can show that the planned trial is too small. However, this is irrelevant after the trial is published, because then the confidence interval reveals the accuracy of the result. Most of Caruso et al.’s remaining 9 quality items have similar problems, see detailed comments in a separate document.

      Although it is important to consider the methods of a trial, there are no simple criteria that decide whether a trial is reliable or not. For example, in a meta-analysis of 276 RCTs, Balk EM, 2002 concluded that “double blinding and allocation concealment, two quality measures that are frequently used in meta-analyses, were not associated with the treatment effect” meaning that valid estimates of treatment effect can be reached without them. Furthermore, Glasziou P, 2007 pointed out that in some cases firm conclusions of treatment benefits can be drawn even without any control groups.

      Caruso TJ, 2007 did not present any numerical results of the trials, simply classifying them as positive (statistically significant effect) or negative (no statistically significant effect), even though such a “vote counting” approach is strongly discouraged eg by the Cochrane Handbook (sec 9.4.11; Jan 2015), which states that:

      “Two problems can occur with vote counting, which suggest that it should be avoided whenever possible. Firstly, problems occur if subjective decisions or statistical significance are used to define ‘positive’ and ‘negative’ studies… Secondly, vote counting takes no account of the differential weights given to each study.”

      Vote counting can lead to false negative conclusions because it ignores the quantitative findings. For example, a large number of placebo-controlled trials on vitamin C and the common cold found non-significant effects on common cold duration, but the results consistently favoured vitamin C. Quantitative pooling of the results shows a statistically highly significant benefit from the vitamin, see Hemilä H, 2013 and Douglas RM, 2005. Vote counting would simply look at the proportion of studies with P<0.05, ignoring the actual mean and SD values, thereby vote counting would lead to false negative conclusions about the effects of vitamin C.

      Caruso TJ, 2007 did not discuss the possibility that the dose of zinc or the lozenge composition might have an effect on trial results, nor did they refer to any of the numerous papers that discussed the possibility that the level of free zinc ion might be an important variable in zinc lozenge trials by Godfrey JC, 1988, Eby GA, 1988, Martin RB, 1988, Eby GA, 1997, Eby GA, 2001, Eby GA, 2004.

      Although Caruso et al. focused on the methodological features that are mostly irrelevant to trial validity, they stated that a “common deficiency [in the zinc trials] was proof of blinding which was lacking in 7 studies. The placebo effect in the treatment of colds was first shown >70 years ago and has since been demonstrated in subsequent studies.” As a justification for this statement, Caruso et al. refer to the Thomas Chalmers review (1975), Chalmers TC, 1975, and the Thomas Karlowski and Thomas Chalmers trial (1975), Karlowski TR, 1975.

      However, when Caruso TJ, 2007 wrote those sentences in their zinc review, they had already been informed that those 2 papers were erroneous, because I had pointed out problems with those 2 papers in a criticism of a previous Caruso TJ, 2005 review on echinacea and the common cold. In a letter-to-the-editor, I wrote that: “The Chalmers review (1975) was shown to be erroneous a decade ago; it has data inconsistent with the original study publications, errors in calculations, and other problems”, see the letter Hemilä H, 2005.

      The Karlowski TR, 1975 trial found statistically significant benefits of vitamin C against the common cold, yet paradoxically Karlowski et al. concluded that “the effects [of vitamin C] demonstrated might be explained equally well by a break in the double blind.” My comments on the Caruso TJ, 2005 review on echinacea and the common cold briefly summarized the problems of the Karlowski trial (1975) as follows: “the [Karlowski] subgroup analysis excluded 105 episodes of common cold (42% of all episodes of cold), even though the 2 subgroups were presented as if they were complementary. There are numerous additional problems with Karlowski’s placebo effect explanation, and, consequently, it is not a valid interpretation to the study results.” see the letter Hemilä H, 2005.

      My letter-to-the-editor, Hemilä H, 2005 referred to studies that documented in detail the problems of the Chalmers review (1975) in Hemilä H, 1995, and the problems of the Karlowski trial (1975) in Hemilä H, 1996. Thus, in their zinc and the common cold review in 2007, Caruso TJ, 2007 still referred to those old erroneous papers by Chalmers and Karlowski although they were aware of my criticisms, since they had read and responded to my letter-to-the-editor.

      See specific comments on the quality items of the Caruso et al. 11 point scoring system in a separate document.

      Another systematic review on zinc lozenges concluded that there is strong evidence that high dose zinc acetate lozenges shorten the duration of colds by 42%, see Hemilä H, 2011.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Aug 27, Bernard Friedenson commented:

      For more recent information please see the article entitled "Mutations in Breast Cancer Exome Sequences Predict Susceptibility to Infection and Converge on the Same Signaling Pathways" This article is available at http://la-press.com/article.php?article_id=5029


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 May 31, David Nunan commented:

      Readers may not be aware of concerns with duplicate data in this paper and another paper (Parrinello G, 2009) by the same group published in the Journal of Cardiac Failure in 2009. Both these papers were also included in a 2012 systematic review published in BMJ Open Heart which was subsequently retracted. A notice of concern was raised with the Journal of Cardiac Failure paper. No such notice has been made for this paper and neither individual papers have been retracted.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:

      Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Apr 12, Konstantinos Fountoulakis commented:

      This is a paper on the efficacy of adjunctive aripiprazole on the reduction of prolactin levels in patients with antipsychotic-induced hyperprolactinaemia (1). In that particular study, adding aripiprazole in stabilized patients with schizophrenia already under haloperidol (mean dosage around 20 mg daily) was not accompanied by an increase in side-effects or destabilization of the patients. However, the unusually low Simpson Angus scale scores for haloperidol treated patients, implies the use of anticholinergics; this could have masked an increase in EPS. If on the contrary no anticholinergics were used, then this could be a special population of patients. The Simpson Angus scale does not rate akathisia, (a major problem with aripiprazole) thus it is rather inadequate for that study. A crude explanation of the lowering of prolactin levels could be that adding aripiprazole reduced the overall antidopaminergic effect of haloperidol but this could had been achieved just by lowering the haloperidol dosage. Another explanation suggests that aripiprazole as a partial dopamine agonist leads to internalization of dopamine receptors (2). However, when spared activated receptors are not available, it is unlikely that aripiprazole could exert its ‘partial agonist’ properties and manifest a more artidopaminergic effect, thus leading to more EPS (3). A preferential agonist action of aripiprazole in the pituitary implies also the presence of spare D2 receptors in the pituitary and it is in contrast with the literature (4). The only remaining explanation is that since it has been consistently shown that aripiprazole possesses a higher affinity to D2 receptors than haloperidol, it also completely replaced haloperidol on the D2 receptor. Shim et al discuss it however they do not follow it explicitly. The magnitude of this replacement should be considered to be almost complete since it has been suggested that aripiprazole is effective when >90% of D2 receptors are occupied (5). Thus it does not seem that the conclusions of Shim et al are justified. In essence what their trial showed is that it is needless to add aripiprazole; the therapist should either reduce the dosage of the original antipsychotic or switch to aripiprazole or to another appropriate agent

      References

      1. Shim JC, Shin JG, Kelly DL, Jung DU, Seo YS, Liu KH, Shon JH, Conley RR: Adjunctive treatment with a dopamine partial agonist, aripiprazole, for antipsychotic-induced hyperprolactinemia: a placebo-controlled trial. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164(9):1404-10
      2. Laruelle M: Imaging synaptic neurotransmission with in vivo binding competition techniques: a critical review. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 2000; 20(3):423-51
      3. Burris KD, Molski TF, Xu C, Ryan E, Tottori K, Kikuchi T, Yocca FD, Molinoff PB: Aripiprazole, a novel antipsychotic, is a high-affinity partial agonist at human dopamine D2 receptors. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2002; 302(1):381-9
      4. Dean B, Pavey G, Scarr E, Goeringer K, Copolov DL: Measurement of dopamine D2-like receptors in postmortem CNS and pituitary: differential regional changes in schizophrenia. Life Sci 2004; 74(25):3115-31
      5. Hamamura T, Harada T: Unique pharmacological profile of aripiprazole as the phasic component buster. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007; 191(3):741-3


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 May 07, Jeff Kiefer commented:

      DAVID is continuously being used as evidenced by its numerous citations in current biomedical literature http://bit.ly/1FS1JgT. However, the data resources used by DAVID appear to not have been updated since 2009 http://david.abcc.ncifcrf.gov/helps/update.html. The fact that DAVID has not been updated going on 5 years calls into question the current utility of using this tool.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2016 May 26, Jeff Kiefer commented:

      It looks like DAVID has been updated May 2016 https://david-d.ncifcrf.gov/content.jsp?file=release.html.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Nov 20, Morten Oksvold commented:

      An investigation committee at Wayne State University (WSU) recommends that 42 articles from Fazlul Sarkar to be retracted (report finished August 31, 2015). This article represents one of them.

      This information was published by Retraction Watch (November 17, 2016) and you can find a link to the full report here:

      http://retractionwatch.com/2016/11/17/details-of-investigative-report-into-sarkar-released-by-aclu/

      This article should therefore no longer be cited.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Feb 01, Jan Tunér commented:

      The treatment time is said to be 10 minutes per patient regardless of wound size. Wound size in the laser group was in the range 4-52 cm2 (average 12 cm2). With an average output power of 4 mW and a treatment time of 600 seconds (10 minutes), an energy of 2.4 joules were emitted from the laser in each session. If the whole wound area was treated, the dose (energy density) for the smallest wounds (4 cm2) must have been 0.6 J/cm2 and for the largest wounds (52 cm2) the dose was 0.046 J/cm2. As the stated dose was 1.96 J/cm2, something seem to be wrong. Further, according the manufacturer of the laser equipment (Irradia AB, Sweden), a GaAs laser with as low power as 4 mW has never been produced and a pulse frequency of 3800 Hz is not available with any of their equipment. Neither has a GaAs laser with a beam divergence of 70 mrad (collimated) been available.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Oct 27, Tim D. Smith commented:

      This paper is a deep and friendly introduction to and comparison of decomposition methods for shape representations of cells. It is, moreover, a pleasure to read; the figures are excellent case studies in clear, evocative, and aesthetic illustration of the data they represent. The software tool described in the article has moved and is, as of this writing, available under a GPL license from Dr. Pincus' website.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Sep 10, Erik Gotfredsen commented:

      Two ways of misspelling. "Delphinium tiantaishanense" in heading, "Delphinium tiantaishan" in abstract the correct (according to botanical litterature) is "Delphinium tianshanicum"


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Dec 29, Keizo Takao commented:

      The raw data of behavioral tests, which are not described in this paper, are shown in the Mouse Phenotype Database (http://www.mouse-phenotype.org/). "ImageLD", "ImageEP", and "ImageFZ", image analysis application softwares used in this article, are now freely available from http://www.mouse-phenotype.org/software.html.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Aug 25, thomas samaras commented:

      Geneticist and statistician Francis Galton calculated the risk of being hit in combat was 33% greater for taller, heavier men. This increased risk makes sense since larger men make bigger targets. Certainly, in attacking enemy trenches, it would seem to be true. In addition, if a soldier is in a defensive position and is being attacked by three enemy soldiers, I'm sure the defender would focus on the larger of the three attackers since he would be more dangerous in hand-to-hand combat.

      Sarna et al. researched Finish athletes who were in WWII. They found that tall basketball players had the highest mortality rate from combat compared to shorter athletes.

      In view of the preceding, it appears that some other factors led to the study results that taller, bigger men had better survival.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Jan 04, Wouter Hendrickx commented:

      I used this great tool before but since december 2015 it seems the domain this tool is hosted on went offline ? I hope this tool will be made available elsewhere.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Aug 23, Ben Goldacre commented:

      This article has two incorrect trial registry IDs associated with it on PubMed: both in the XML on PubMed, and in the originating journal article. The first incorrect ID given is NCT00209795. We believe the correct ID, which we have found by hand searching, is NCT00209794.

      The second ID given is NCT88523834. This trial ID does not appear in clinicaltrials.gov. We have contacted the corresponding author on the study to ask them for the correct trial ID on 18/08/2016, but received no reply. We have also searched manually for this trial on clinicaltrials.gov and found no matching study. We therefore believe that this trial may not have not been registered on ClinicalTrials.gov.

      This comment is being posted as part of the OpenTrials.net project<sup>[1]</sup> , an open database threading together all publicly accessible documents and data on each trial, globally. In the course of creating the database, and matching documents and data sources about trials from different locations, we have identified various anomalies in datasets such as PubMed, and in published papers. Alongside documenting the prevalence of problems, we are also attempting to correct these errors and anomalies wherever possible, by feeding back to the originators. We have corrected this data in the OpenTrials.net database; we hope that this trial’s text and metadata can also be corrected at source, in PubMed and in the accompanying paper.

      Many thanks,

      Jessica Fleminger, Ben Goldacre*

      [1] Goldacre, B., Gray, J., 2016. OpenTrials: towards a collaborative open database of all available information on all clinical trials. Trials 17. doi:10.1186/s13063-016-1290-8 PMID: 27056367

      * Dr Ben Goldacre BA MA MSc MBBS MRCPsych<br> Senior Clinical Research Fellow<br> ben.goldacre@phc.ox.ac.uk<br> www.ebmDataLab.net<br> Centre for Evidence Based Medicine<br> Department of Primary Care Health Sciences<br> University of Oxford<br> Radcliffe Observatory Quarter<br> Woodstock Road<br> Oxford OX2 6GG


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Aug 18, Paul Brookes commented:

      I did not discover the following; these comments were sent to me by an anonymous correspondent, and I agree with their assessment of the data, so am posting here using my own name even though this is not a paper I have either read, or am familiar with the field of...

      There appears to be duplication of several bands in Supplemental Figure 1. Specifically, the anti-EGFP blot in Figure S1A, the anti-Myc blot in Figure S1B (rotated by 180 degrees), and the left two lanes of the anti-GFP blot in Figure S1C are all remarkably similar considering their allegedly different sample origins.

      In Figure 2 of the main manuscript, there also appear to be several undisclosed splicing events. In the anti-myc blot (lower panel) of Figure 2B, enhanced contrast seems to suggest that several images were used to compile this image. Furthermore in Figure 2D, left panel, the anti-EGFP blot lower panel) contains a splicing seam in between the 1st and second lanes.

      Finally, bringing the story full-circle, in the right panel of Figure 2D, the bands in the anti-GST blot (middle panel) appear to resemble those discussed above WRT Figure S1, but flipped horizontally and stretched vertically.

      FYI, another paper from the same group Schwamborn JC, 2007 has also been flagged by the same person, and I have/will left a comment there too. I am reliably informed that the DFG (German equivalent of the NIH) is aware of these data problems, as are the journals involved. However more than 6 months has now passed with no actions taken.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2015 Jul 02, Leonid Schneider commented:

      This paper has now been retracted, June 19th, 2015: http://www.jbc.org/content/290/25/15391


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Nov 01, Dale D O Martin commented:

      An updated list of new post-translationally myristoylated proteins including the anti-apoptotic protein Mcl-1 and the causative agent of Huntington Disease, Huntingtin, can be found here: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21965604 Herein, we used a post-translational myristoylation assay, that was dependent on caspase cleavage, to verify previously identified substrates and to identify new post-translationally myristoylated proteins.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Feb 07, Dale D O Martin commented:

      We have since shown that caspase-cleavage of HTT releases an autophagy-inducing domain that requires its post-translational myristoylation. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24459296

      Videos can be seen here: http://hmg.oxfordjournals.org/content/early/2014/01/22/hmg.ddu027/suppl/DC1


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2014 Sep 30, Dale D O Martin commented:

      Post-translational myristoylation of both Bap31 and CD-IC2 have recently been confirmed endogenously in Thinon et al found here http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25255805


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Feb 01, Jan Tunér commented:

      In this study, a 6 mW HeNe laser or a placebo HeNe laser was used to treat leg ulcers twice weekly for 12 weeks. The stated dose was 4 J/cm2. The area of the ulcers varied from 3 cm2 to 32 cm2. To achieve the stated dose, the treatment time of each session of therapy would thus have had to vary from 33 minutes to 6 hours. It is questioned that the patients really would have been treated for 6 hours. The method by which the dose was calculated is therefore questionable. No indication is given of the method of treatment. If a scanning laser with an unexpanded beam was used, the power density would have been about 0.15 W/cm2. If the beam was expanded to a diameter capable of illuminating the whole area of the ulcer at once, the power density when treating the largest ulcer would have been about 0.00019 W/cm2, which is close to the level of moonlight. Unless more parameters are accounted for, it is impossible to evaluate this study. The possibility of performing a double blind study with red light is also questionable. The authors have been reluctant to answer questions on this paper.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Jul 02, Pedro Silva commented:

      The code used to locate the minimum-energy-crossing points in this work is publicly available as:

      Silva, Pedro (2015): Program for the computation of MECP using the Firefly Quantum Chemistry program. figshare. http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.1471648


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Dec 02, Stephen Strum commented:

      This is a landmark publication and should be required reading of all those caring for patients with cancer that involves bone mets. In my medical oncology practice I rarely see attention paid to bone integrity and what is called the the vicious cycle of bone resorption & tumor cell proliferation (VC). The implications of how we assess patients at diagnosis and during the course of their care as it relates to VC and the associated increase in cytokines is of immense prognostic and even diagnostic value. I continue to see men with prostate cancer who never have any lab assessment that relates to bone resorption markers (BRMs). This paper beautifully portrays the importance of bone integrity & the underlying mechanisms of action.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Feb 08, Mwidimi Ndosi commented:

      My research student Hannah Cumpstey picked this up in the methods section: "A p-value >0.05 was accepted as being statistically different" (Akbari et al, 2007, P.633).

      In the results section however, the interpretation of the null hypothesis significance testing seem to follow the conventional significance level, where a p=0.03 is interpreted as significant and p=0.95 not significant (p.634).

      Could the authors please comment, just to reassure other readers?


      Akbari A, Moodi H, Ghiasi F, Sagheb HM, Rashidi H. (2007) Effects of vacuum-compression therapy on healing of diabetic foot ulcers: randomized controlled trial. J Rehabil Res Dev. 44(5): 631-6.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Aug 23, Ben Goldacre commented:

      This trial has the wrong trial registry ID associated with it on PubMed: both in the XML on PubMed, and in the originating journal article. The ID given is NCT00535031. We believe the correct ID, which we have found by hand searching, is NCT00535431.

      This comment is being posted as part of the OpenTrials.net project<sup>[1]</sup> , an open database threading together all publicly accessible documents and data on each trial, globally. In the course of creating the database, and matching documents and data sources about trials from different locations, we have identified various anomalies in datasets such as PubMed, and in published papers. Alongside documenting the prevalence of problems, we are also attempting to correct these errors and anomalies wherever possible, by feeding back to the originators. We have corrected this data in the OpenTrials.net database; we hope that this trial’s text and metadata can also be corrected at source, in PubMed and in the accompanying paper.

      Many thanks,

      Jessica Fleminger, Ben Goldacre*

      [1] Goldacre, B., Gray, J., 2016. OpenTrials: towards a collaborative open database of all available information on all clinical trials. Trials 17. doi:10.1186/s13063-016-1290-8 PMID: 27056367

      * Dr Ben Goldacre BA MA MSc MBBS MRCPsych<br> Senior Clinical Research Fellow<br> ben.goldacre@phc.ox.ac.uk<br> www.ebmDataLab.net<br> Centre for Evidence Based Medicine<br> Department of Primary Care Health Sciences<br> University of Oxford<br> Radcliffe Observatory Quarter<br> Woodstock Road<br> Oxford OX2 6GG


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 May 07, Jeff Kiefer commented:

      DAVID is continuously being used as evidenced by its numerous citations in current biomedical literature http://bit.ly/1FS1JgT. However, the data resources used by DAVID appear to not have been updated since 2009 http://david.abcc.ncifcrf.gov/helps/update.html. The fact that DAVID has not been updated going on 5 years calls into question the current utility of using this tool.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2016 May 26, Jeff Kiefer commented:

      It looks like DAVID has been updated May 2016 https://david-d.ncifcrf.gov/content.jsp?file=release.html.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Jun 30, Swapnil Hiremath commented:

      This is a fascinating attempt at resolving heterogeneity in this field of contrast nephropathy. The ACT study, done later by Berwanger et al in Circulation 2011 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21859972, proved conclusively that NAC is not useful. However, this paper, by trying to resolve the cause of the heterogeneity is a fascinating read beyond just the summary outcome measure. As Prof Stoto would say, while doing a systematic review, try to think, 'What is the Story?'


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Dec 11, Ibrahim Masoodi commented:

      We have found Fecal Lactoferrin an effective biomarker too.This co relates well the UC activity and severity


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:

      Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 May 04, Raphael Levy commented:

      This post is co-authored by Raphael Levy and David Mason.


      Note: We contacted Chad Mirkin and EMD Millipore for comments. Chad Mirkin replied but prefers to keep his comments for the peer reviewed literature rather than blogs. EMD Millipore has provided a response (reproduced below) and is keen to further engage in the discussion. They wrote that they "look forward to responding to [the questions you pose at the end of the post] after your blog is posted so other researchers who may have the same questions can follow our discussion online."


      To image proteins in cells, biologists have powerful tools based on the Green Fluorescent Protein (GFP) for which Osamu Shimomura, Martin Chalfie and Roger Y. Tsien obtained the 2008 Nobel Prize in Chemistry. RNA molecules play crucial roles in cells such as coding, decoding, regulation, and expression of genes, yet they are much more difficult to study. SmartFlares are nanoparticle-based probes for the detection and imaging of RNA in live cells. Could they become the GFP of the RNA world?

      Read more at https://raphazlab.wordpress.com/2015/03/11/how-smart-are-smartflares/


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2017 Sep 15, Raphael Levy commented:

      A new paper confirms that spherical nucleic acids / nano-flares / SmartFlares do no detect mRNA. SmartFlares fail to reflect their target transcripts levels

      See also the Guest post by the authors of the above study at my blog.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 May 12, David Gortler commented:

      This trial supports the findings of the older CARDS trials which also showed that "lower is better" when it comes to LDL, and there is additive benefit to driving LDL levels beyond 70mg/dL (the current NECP goal) to even as low as 40mg/dL. Since this publication, it has been additionally shown that elevation of hsCRP is associated with increased risk of type 2 diabetes development in patients with all levels of metabolic syndrome.

      In type 1 and type 2 diabetes mellitus, hemoglobin A1c significantly correlates with hsCRP levels and future cardiovascular risk indicating that diabetes has inflammatory fundamentals. Also, hsCRP levels increase with the stage of beta-cell dysfunction and insulin resistance. Non-diabetic drugs that have been shown to reduce hsCRP concentrations include aspirin, statins, cyclooxygenase-2 inhibitors, and fibrates.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Nov 20, Morten Oksvold commented:

      An investigation committee at Wayne State University (WSU) recommends that 42 articles from Fazlul Sarkar to be retracted (report finished August 31, 2015). This article represents one of them.

      This information was published by Retraction Watch (November 17, 2016) and you can find a link to the full report here:

      http://retractionwatch.com/2016/11/17/details-of-investigative-report-into-sarkar-released-by-aclu/

      This article should therefore no longer be cited.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Oct 23, MARK VRABEL commented:

      For the latest evidence on ondansetron, granisetron, other antiemetics, and nonpharmacologic interventions for CINV, see the Oncology Nursing Society Putting Evidence Into Practice http://www.ons.org/Research/PEP/Nausea and http://esource.ons.org/ProductDetails.aspx?sku=INPU0638


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Aug 23, Ben Goldacre commented:

      This trial has the wrong trial registry ID associated with it on PubMed: both in the XML on PubMed, and in the originating journal article. The ID given is NCT0037. We believe the correct ID, which we have found by hand searching, is NCT00371982.

      This comment is being posted as part of the OpenTrials.net project<sup>[1]</sup> , an open database threading together all publicly accessible documents and data on each trial, globally. In the course of creating the database, and matching documents and data sources about trials from different locations, we have identified various anomalies in datasets such as PubMed, and in published papers. Alongside documenting the prevalence of problems, we are also attempting to correct these errors and anomalies wherever possible, by feeding back to the originators. We have corrected this data in the OpenTrials.net database; we hope that this trial’s text and metadata can also be corrected at source, in PubMed and in the accompanying paper.

      Many thanks,

      Jessica Fleminger, Ben Goldacre*

      [1] Goldacre, B., Gray, J., 2016. OpenTrials: towards a collaborative open database of all available information on all clinical trials. Trials 17. doi:10.1186/s13063-016-1290-8 PMID: 27056367

      * Dr Ben Goldacre BA MA MSc MBBS MRCPsych<br> Senior Clinical Research Fellow<br> ben.goldacre@phc.ox.ac.uk<br> www.ebmDataLab.net<br> Centre for Evidence Based Medicine<br> Department of Primary Care Health Sciences<br> University of Oxford<br> Radcliffe Observatory Quarter<br> Woodstock Road<br> Oxford OX2 6GG


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Feb 26, Bill Noble commented:

      Please note that the particular strategy proposed here for estimating false discovery rates in the context of tandem mass spectrometry has subsequently been shown to be biased. Evidence for this bias, and discussion of alternative protocols, is given in this paper: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26152888


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Nov 01, Dale D O Martin commented:

      It should be noted that N-myristoylation can only occur on N-terminal Glycines, hence the name N-myristoylation. This occurs either co-translationally on the nascent polypeptide following the removal of the initiator Met or it can occur posttranslationally following proteolysis, which exposes a new N-terminal Gly. The latter has only been shown to occur in caspase-cleaved proteins. In this case, the Gly follows an Asp residue where caspase will cleave. The authors here predict internal myristoylation at very unlikely positions. Furthermore, the general consensus sequence for myristoylation is GXXXS/C/T where X is any amino acid, except for large bulky residues, and S/C/T are preferred in position 5 (counting from Gly). The first site they predict is GAAPP and is very unlikely to be myristoylated. Caution should be taken when predicting internal myristoylation sites. Unless it is predicted to be cleaved to expose an N-terminal Gly.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 17, Carl Denef commented:

      An update of this review is available in an on line database at the following URL:

      https://perswww.kuleuven.be/~u0009251/index.htm

      This database is updated


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:

      Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Jan 19, Denise Fernandez-Twinn commented:

      I am an author on this paper, however because my name is listed as Twinn DF, I am not getting the cites for this really important paper. Is there something PubMed can do to change the way my name is listed to Fernandez-Twinn DS. I have other collaborative work with some of the other authors on this paper (Ozanne SE and Samuelson AM), so it would be easy to verify my identity and contribution on this paper. Thank you. Kind Regards, Denise S Fernandez-Twinn


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 17, Raphael Levy commented:

      A key paper in the stripy controversy. Discussed here.

      More broadly, the evidence behind the structure and special properties of “striped” nanoparticles has been challenged by Cesbron Y, 2012. The publication in 2012 of Cesbron Y, 2012 took three years and has been followed by post-publication peer review of the various existing and new stripy articles on my blog, PubPeer, etc.

      A detailed analysis of this body of work is published today in PloS One by Stirling et al; from the abstract: “through a combination of an exhaustive re-analysis of the original data with new experimental measurements of a simple control sample comprising entirely unfunctionalised particles, we conclusively show that all of the STM evidence for striped nanoparticles published to date can instead be explained by a combination of well-known instrumental artefacts, strong observer bias, and/or improper data acquisition/analysis protocols.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Oct 22, DAVID SANDERS commented:

      None


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2013 Oct 25, DAVID SANDERS commented:

      This article twice claims that Alvarado J, 2006 hypothesizes that there are two distinct active sites on the E. coli Ppx.

      "However, the bifunctional aspect of pppGpp hydrolysis was linked to the presence of two active sites in one study Alvarado J, 2006 but was in favor of a common active site in the other."

      "The structural analysis by Alvarado et al. identified domain III as a second PPX active site responsible for pppGpp hydrolysis."Alvarado J, 2006

      However, no such statement is made in Alvarado J, 2006.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 May 12, G L Francis commented:

      This paper sought to find a chemically-defined medium to maintain human embryonic stem cells (hESCs) in long-term culture, necessary for expansion of cell numbers to enable production platforms required to provide differentiated cells for the clinical trial of cell-mediated therapeutics and beyond. Although this paper in now six years old the authors raise some important points still relevant today, however, I believe they misinterpreted a key finding for the relative importance of the components of ‘lipid rich albumin’ by underestimating the contribution of the albumin carrier itself. The studies initially utilized KnockOut™ Serum Replacement (KOSR) and then AlbuMAX®, a chromatographically purified lipid-rich bovine serum albumin (BSA) preparation, which they established was the active component in KOSR, moreover, both products are problematic themselves when developing production methods for human clinical products, due to their animal origins . None the less, the importance of the albumin borne lipid components in sustaining pluripotency markers of hESCs, indicating self-renewal over five passages, was correctly established (Fig. 2 & Fig.3). They extend this finding to claim evidence for the exclusive role of lipids, and attribute no role for the associated albumin carrier protein, in supporting the self-renewal of the hESC lines (Fig 3 A-E). It is the latter conclusion I take odds with, and in fact believe the results also support another conclusion - that while lipid components are essential to the action of the lipid-rich albumin preparations used, albumin promotes the action of its lipid ligands, to maximize the response as seen for both KOSR and AlbuMAX®. Indeed careful examination of the data (Fig 3B and Fig3D) reported allows the conclusion that the presence of intact albumin increases the expression of the pluripotency markers TRA-1-60 and NANAOG by around 25%, and only that of OCT4 is unchanged. Also in this series of experiments the authors could have included another control treatment where they mixed the intact AlbuMAX® with trypsin inhibitor before adding the protease trypsin. Next the authors correctly proved that after the repeated passage (X7) of hESCs they actually retained pluripotency and the capacity to differentiate into ectoderm, endoderm, and mesoderm, by displaying the appropriate markers, GFAP & Sox1, Sox17 & Pdx1 (latter not shown), and cardiac Troponin T, respectively (Fig.4A). Differentiation of hESCs passaged under all conditions resulted in a drastic reduction of pluripotency markers Oct4 and NANAOG and the appropriate increase of differentiation marker for each germ layer of the generated embroid bodies (EBs). However, for the AlbuMAX® + Trypsin media passaged hESCs the resulting embroid bodies displayed for each germ layer the relevant markers, but numerically (by inspection) reduced by 47% GFAP and 25% SOX1 (ectoderm), 10% SOX17 and ??% Pdx1 (endoderm), and 42% cardiac Troponin T (mesoderm) compared to EBs derived from hESCs cultured with intact AlbuMAX®. This difference was not addressed by the authors to my knowledge, and if all experimental variables are controlled and assuming these differences are statistically robust - then possibly a more complex role for albumin in facilitating the subsequent differentiation of these cell lines is indicated. Furthermore, the presence of lipid-rich albumin (i.e. BSA present in 10% Fetal Bovine Serum) during hESC differentiation and EB growth suggests the observed marker differences result from earlier genetic or epigenetic modifications to the hESCs during the serial passaging phase. While the possible involvement of other endogenous or exogenous ligands of albumin were considered to no avail, reasonably the authors could not cover the full spectrum of effects that albumin could promote directly or indirectly in mammalian cells, many of which I have reviewed more recently (2010-PMID: 20373019 [PubMed]).<br> I wish to sincerely thank the authors for the wide range of issues they raised in relation to this topic and I hope my comments are of some value to them or others.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Feb 18, Kristina Hanspers commented:

      The network in figure 2 is available in the "Open Access Publication" collection at WikiPathways: http://www.wikipathways.org/index.php/Pathway:WP2636. This pathway can be downloaded for use in network analysis tools such as Cytoscape and PathVisio.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Sep 27, S Sundar commented:

      We noticed re-induction of hormone sensitivity in more patients and we presented our experience at the ASCO GU conference in 2008. The conference abstract is unfortunately no longer available from ASCO website.

      We have reproduced our submission below for the benefit of clinicians and researchers interested in our manuscript.

      Citation:<br> Cox RA, Sundar S. Re-induction of sensitivity to diethylstilbestrol in docetaxel-treated androgen refractory prostate cancer. In: American Society of Clinical Oncology Genitourinary Symposium. 2008. Abstract No: 172. ....................................

      Control/Tracking Number: 08-AB-20299-ASCOGU Activity: Abstract Submission Current Date/Time: 1/29/2008 10:04:47 AM

      Re-induction of sensitivity to Diethylstilbestrol in Docetaxel treated androgen refractory prostate cancer

      Author Block: S. Sundar, R. Cox; Nottingham City Hospital, Nottingham, United Kingdom

      Abstract:

      Introduction: Diethylstilbestrol (DES) is a standard second line hormone therapy for prostate cancer in the UK. Following introduction of Docetaxel, DES is increasingly being used as third line therapy. We audited our patients (pts) response to DES after treatment with Docetaxel.

      Methods: Pts (n=56) who received Docetaxel within an 18-month period were identified from a pharmacy database and the sub-group (n=16) treated with DES after Docetaxel was identified. PSA Response rate (50% decline) to Docetaxel was 60%. All pts had androgen refractory disease and had castrate levels of testosterone (LHRH-agonist therapy/orchidectomy). Pts with a history of ischaemic heart disease, stroke, pulmonary embolism (PE) or thrombosis were not offered DES. Pts were treated with DES 1mg daily with aspirin 75mg daily for thrombotic prophylaxis.

      Results: 16 pts subsequently received DES on disease progression following Docetaxel. (1 pt unable to tolerate DES due to nausea was excluded from analysis). Median age of pts given DES after Docetaxel was 70yrs (52-79). 12 of 15 (80%) responded to DES 'after' Docetaxel, with 6 (50%) having a >50% reduction in PSA. Significantly 7 pts were treated with DES 'prior' to Docetaxel. 6 of these 7 pts (86%) who were previously refractory to DES 'prior' to Docetaxel responded to retreatment with DES 'following' Docetaxel chemotherapy. Currently, 6 pts remain on therapy after a median follow up of 2.5 months. 5 pts who progressed, did so after a median treatment duration of 4 months. 1 responding patient stopped after 2 months due to a PE. DES was otherwise very well tolerated. 1 patient stopped treatment after 1 week due to nausea. There were no other documented cardiovascular, cerebrovascular or thrombotic events.

      Conclusions: DES is an option for patients with androgen refractory prostate cancer who have relapsed after treatment with Docetaxel. Previous work (Shamash et al, Br.J.Cancer.2005) found re-induction of hormone sensitivity following failure of Chlorambucil and Lomustine chemotherapy. Further investigation of this interesting phenomenon of re-induction of DES sensitivity could provide further insight into molecular mechanisms underlying androgen refractory prostate cancer. : Author Disclosure Information: S. Sundar, None; R. Cox, None. ..........................................................................


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Jun 28, Erick H Turner commented:

      Below are DOIs and links to FDA review documents for the 8 older antidepressant drugs whose reviews are not available on the FDA website Drugs@FDA.

      doi:10.6083/M4542M8J Bupropion SR http://digitalcommons.ohsu.edu/fdadrug/21

      doi:10.6083/M41C1VJT Fluoxetine http://digitalcommons.ohsu.edu/fdadrug/22

      doi:10.6083/M48W3C0Z Mirtazapine http://digitalcommons.ohsu.edu/fdadrug/20

      doi:10.6083/M4WM1C2X Nefazodone http://digitalcommons.ohsu.edu/fdadrug/23

      doi:10.6083/M4HD7TC4 Paroxetine http://digitalcommons.ohsu.edu/fdadrug/26

      doi:10.6083/M4CN72MB Sertraline http://digitalcommons.ohsu.edu/fdadrug/27

      doi:10.6083/M4RV0MCN Venlafaxine http://digitalcommons.ohsu.edu/fdadrug/24

      doi:10.6083/M4N58K2D Venlafaxine XR http://digitalcommons.ohsu.edu/fdadrug/25

      Because they were not acquired directly from the FDA but rather indirectly through colleagues who had obtained them via FOIA requests, the order of the 'hard copy' pages may have been shuffled in some cases. For a 'cleaner' copy, one might need to place a new FOIA request with the FDA (http://www.accessdata.fda.gov/scripts/foi/FOIRequest/requestinfo.cfm).

      As for the 4 newer antidepressant drugs, as stated in the article, those reviews are downloadable from Drugs@FDA. If assistance is needed in navigating to and through that site, the "cookbook" article of mine available at http://www.bmj.com/content/347/bmj.f5992 may be useful.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Oct 28, DAVID SANDERS commented:

      Identical Env deletions to those described in this article ("The Env truncated at 616 exhibits maximum fusogenicity in cell-to-cell fusion assay. By comparison, full tail Env (632) and the Env truncated to residue 601 mediated fusion at 40%.") are analyzed in Taylor GM, 2003, which is not cited by this article and contains an alternative hypothesis that there is a trimeric coiled coil in the cytoplasmic domain. This hypothesis has gained recent support. Löving R, 2012

      Analysis of the effects of Env mutations in Li M, 2001, Yang C, 1997, and Taylor GM, 2003, which is inconsistent with the hypothesis proposed by the authors of this Exp Mol Pathol article, was also ignored by them.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Dec 15, Eduardo Palencia-Herrejón commented:

      Surprised with the Le Gall statement against the involvement of family in "hard decisions"... Perhaps the full text provides an adequate explanation, but the bare statement sounds like paternalistic...


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Jul 02, Pedro Silva commented:

      The code used to locate the minimum-energy-crossing points in this work is publicly available as:

      Silva, Pedro (2015): Program for the computation of MECP using the Firefly Quantum Chemistry program. figshare. http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.1471648


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Oct 26, Graham Coop commented:

      The heritable variation in hotspot usage that we found in this article was later mapped to the gene prdm9, see Baudat et al ( Baudat F, 2010 ) and related articles.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 24, Claudia Paiva commented:

      I think that the possible link between autoimmune thyroiditis and Meniere's disease may be the deposition of antibody+thyroid antigen immune complexes instead of autoimmunity against other antigens. The evidences do not point to hormone levels as the link.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Oct 14, Eric Yarnell commented:

      Authors should be Peng A, Gu Y, Gui DK, Zhu QY, Zhu KY, Lin SY (family and given names have been switched)


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Jun 17, Mike Fay commented:

      This paper shows an improved way to calculate the variance when using multiple imputation with interval censored data. Huang, Lee and Yu show this is better than calculating the variance by using a correction for multiple imputation developed by Rubin for a different situation, that was used in earlier papers.

      Another advantage of this test (not studied in the original paper) is that it retains the type I error fairly well even when the assessment times depend on treatment (see my paper with Joanna Shih Fay MP, 2012).

      Software to calculate this test is available in the interval R package, http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/interval/index.html.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 May 23, Morten Oksvold commented:

      This article was retracted April 27 2017 together with eight other articles from the same group due to data manipulations.

      Please note that the retraction notice is visible in the PDF document only.

      http://www.jbc.org/content/283/17/11176.full.pdf?sid=a8d5dc2c-dd64-4b19-bf5e-c46f7deb9f49

      "This article has been withdrawn by the authors. The authors were recently made aware of issues in Figs. 1A and 2A. In Fig. 1A, the actERK2 panel was manipulated inappropriately, and the GST panel was duplicated in Fig. 1C as the GST panel. In Fig. 2A, the P-Elk panels for ERK2 were duplicated, and the right actERK2 panel for RSK was duplicated in the right actERK2 panel for cFOS. Because the original data are no longer available, in the interest of maintaining accuracy in the published scientific literature, the authors wish to withdraw this article. However, the authors have full confidence in the findings and conclusions of this paper and have replicated the findings in subsequent work."


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jan 03, Alexander Lerchl commented:

      None


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Jan 16, Franz Adlkofer commented:

      None


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2014 Feb 11, Franz Adlkofer commented:

      Two papers from a research team at the Medical University of Vienna (MUV), the one above and a previous one [1], point to a genotoxic potential of radiofrequency electromagnetic fields. Both papers result from the REFLEX project, a multi-national study on biological effects of electromagnetic fields funded by the European Union, which I coordinated [2]. About three years after REFLEX had been completed all of a sudden the claim was circulated that the Vienna results might have been faked. With this allegation the editors of the two peer-reviewed scientific journals should be forced to retract the respective papers. However, they carried out their own investigations, and in both editorial boards the outcome of a thorough scrutiny led to the conclusion that there is no evidence of fraud.

      At the same time, the MUV mandated its Council for Scientific Ethics to investigate in detail how the Vienna REFLEX data were generated. This Council confirmed already at its first meeting and without any investigation the suspected fraud, and recommended the retraction of the two papers. By chance, it turned out that its chairman was a lawyer from the Austrian telecommunication industry. After his replacement the new Council came to the decision that the allegation is unfounded and that there is no reason to further pursue the case. Unhappy with this acquittal, the matter was finally transferred to the newly established Austrian Agency for Research Integrity that after a further scrutiny followed the Council’s decision [3].

      Criticism of scientific data is absolutely necessary, but to claim fraud in order to get rid of them is unacceptable, whatever the reasons behind.

      1. Diem E, Schwarz C, Adlkofer F, Jahn O, Rüdiger HW (2005) Non-thermal DNA breakage by mobile phone radiation (1800 MHz) in human fibroblasts and transformed GFSH-R17 rat granulosa cells in vitro. Mutat Res 583:178-83.
      2. See „REFLEX Final Report“ in http://www.itis.ethz.ch/assets/Downloads/Papers-Reports/Reports/REFLEXFinal-Report171104.pdf
      3. See “Part I. A campaign to destroy scientific findings” in http://www.kompetenzinitiative.net/assets/broschuerenreihe_heft-5_eng_screen.pdf


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    4. On 2014 Aug 10, Alexander Lerchl commented:

      None


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 26, Harri Hemila commented:

      A comment on this study was published in DOI.

      A response to that comment was published in DOI.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Jul 31, valentina di pietro commented:

      Notes: The substitution I226T has never been found in patients affected by Canavan disease. Bioinformatics tools recently deployed to predict mutations did not confirm substitution of I226T as a mutation. According to PredictSNP (https://loschmidt.chemi.muni.cz/predictsnp1/) which combines six different methods (MAPP, Phd-SNP, Poly-phen1, Poly-phen2, SIFT, SNAP) into a consensus classifier, this is a neutral substitution with an 83% level of confidence. Meta-SNP (http://snps.biofold.org/meta-snp/), a meta-predictor which combines the outputs of PANTHER, PhD-SNP, SIFT and SNAP gives a final score of 0.242 . Only a score >0.5 indicates prediction of disease mutations.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Mar 26, Tom Kindlon commented:

      "Actometer readings would have provided more objective data"

      My e-letter replying to this paper, "Actometer readings would have provided more objective data", can be read here: http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/121/3/e619/reply


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:

      Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Mar 25, Sandra Rieger commented:

      I would like to know which cells were investigated?


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Mar 09, Mark Milton commented:

      This a good review article. However, the values cited for the vitreal volume of the cynomolgus monkey are incorrect. The source articles were misread. The quoted vitreal volumes (1.5 and 3.2 mL) shouldn't be used when calculating a safety margin based upon the differences in vitreal volume between cynomolgus monkey and man.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Aug 08, Yuwei Fan commented:

      The equation 2 in Biaxial Flexural Strength Test section seems not correct. Not sure if the data are valid.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Jun 02, thomas samaras commented:

      Additional information is available on height, body size and longevity from the following publications.

      Samaras TT. Evidence from eight different types of studies showing that smaller body size is related to greater longevity. Journal of Scientific Research & Reports. 2014: 3 (16): 2150-2160, 2014; article no. JSRR.2014.16.003.

      Samaras TT. Human Scaling and Body Mass Index. In: Samaras TT (ed): Human Body Size and the Laws of Scaling: Physiological Performance, Growth, Longevity and Ecological Ramifications. New York: Nova Science Pub; 2007: pp 17-32.

      He Q, Morris BJ, Grove JS, Petrovitch H, Ross W, Masaki KH, et al. Shorter men live longer: Association of height with longevity and FOXO3 genotype in American men of Japanese ancestry. Plos ONE 9(5): e94385. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0094385.

      Salaris L, Poulain M, Samaras TT. Height and survival at older ages among men born in an inland village in Sardinia (Italy), 1866-2006. Biodemography and Social Biology, 58:1, 1-13.

      Bartke A. Healthy Aging: Is Smaller better? A mini-review. Gerontology 2012; 58:337-43.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:

      Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Nov 04, Laura Williams commented:

      Walsh F, 2013 recently investigated the central claim of this article, which is that many soil bacteria are able to grow using different classes of antibiotics as a single carbon source. Contrary to the findings of Dantas et al., Walsh et al. were only able to confirm catabolism of beta-lactam antibiotics, rather than catabolism of multiple classes of antibiotics. Their paper points to the presence of EDTA in the minimal medium used in the original study as a possible source of carbon sufficient for bacterial growth, which would suggest that growth in the media+antibiotic condition is not a indication of catabolism of the antibiotic, but simply antibiotic resistance.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2013 Nov 05, Fiona Walsh commented:

      The beta-lactams in our study were not 'catabolised' but were digested by the antibiotic resistance enzymes, the beta-lactamases. We used the same strains as the Dantas study i.e. two beta-lactam catabolising bacteria and also identified that the degradation of the beta-lactams was due to the beta-lactamases. These bacteria were either Pseudomonas or Burkholderia species, both of which contain chromosomal beta-lactamases. We identified bacteria which presented with a streptomycin or trimethoprim 'catabolising' phenotype, which we isolated from soil (the original strains from Dantas et al., study with such phenotypes were lost). However, the HPLC experiments clearly showed that these antibiotics were not degraded over 28 days, we concluded based on these and other results that the bacteria did not catabolise the antibiotics. The Dantas study described the HPLC/ chemical degradation studies only for the beta-lactams (carbenicillin and penicillin) and extrapolated these results to all other classes of antibiotics described in the study.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jan 08, Tom Kindlon commented:

      More information on what exactly was measured in Reynolds (2004) would have been useful

      An important part of this paper is the indirect cost estimates taken from Reynolds et al [1]. It would have been useful to have been given clear information on what was measured in that study.

      The current paper says at one point: "Indirect costs include transportation, work productivity losses, disability reimbursements, loss of leisure or duties at home, or services provided by family members, friends, or other informal care providers". However what Reynolds measured was the loss of productivity. It didn't not measure the cost of disability re-imbursements, for example.

      This would partly explain differences with some other figures. For example, a report published by Sheffield Halham University in the UK in 2003 estimated the cost to the UK at 3.467 billion pounds Sterling. It calculated the "cost to the nation" by adding together the figure for the lost taxes from people not working plus the cost of paying them disability payments. There could be said to be pluses and minuses with either method of course.

      Also, one other minor point: it might have been useful to point out that part of the reason there would be discrepancies between quoted studies is the effect of inflation. For example, we are told "Lloyd and Pender estimated an average cost of $9,436 per patient with ME/CFS, including about AU $2,000 per patient in direct medical costs". But we are not told in the text that the Lloyd and Pender study was published in 1992 (perhaps the figure was increased due to inflation but this has not been made clear).

      References:

      [1] Reynolds KJ, Vernon SD, Bouchery E, Reeves WC. The economic impact of chronic fatigue syndrome. Cost Eff Resour Alloc. 2004 Jun 21;2(1):4.

      [2] Lloyd AR, Pender H. The economic impact of chronic fatigue syndrome. Med J Aust. 1992 Nov 2;157(9):599-601.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Jul 11, Daniel Haft commented:

      Readers may wish to note that the passenger domain of this autotransporter has multiple copies of repeat that averages about 88 residues in length, with consensus sequence GDNTYAGGTEVEAGTLRVSRDANLGAAAGAVTLDGGALAATASFASARALTLKAAGALDVAAGTTLDWRGAVSGAGKLVKEGAGTLVL. The BatB orthologs discussed in this paper have 17 repeats in Bordetella pertussis and B. bronchiseptica, and 11 repeats in B. parapertussis. The deletion of 531 amino acids in B. parapertussis maps to this repeat region. Additional batB gene translations from Bordetella isolates obtained since this paper show additional examples of changes in the numbers of repeats. Since individual repeats are quite different from each other, these changing repeat copy numbers very likely represent natural variation, not sequencing and assembly errors. Therefore, the shorter form seen in B. parapertussis should not be viewed as non-functional simply because it shows a large deletion relative to B. pertussis.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jan 07, Brett Snodgrass commented:

      Dear Reader,

      Thank you for providing the excellent report and images. Please provide your kind attention to the distinction between the Thebesian veins and the vessels of Wearn.

      http://bit.ly/JTWearn

      https://twitter.com/BrettSnodgrass1/status/415908673412530177

      A possible name for the title may be: Persistent vessels of Wearn Presenting as Ischemic Heart Disease.

      Comments and suggestions are welcome.

      Thank you kindly.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Mar 10, Kristina Hanspers commented:

      The pathways in figures 6-9 are available in the "Open Access Publication" collection at WikiPathways: http://wikipathways.org/index.php/Pathway:WP152, http://wikipathways.org/index.php/Pathway:WP566, http://wikipathways.org/index.php/Pathway:WP341 and http://wikipathways.org/index.php/Pathway:WP211. These pathways can be downloaded for use in network analysis tools such as Cytoscape and PathVisio.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:

      Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Oct 24, Tom Kindlon commented:

      Extremes In Activity Levels Of Those With Persistent Fatigue Were Not Investigated

      I question the claim in Viner et al<sup>1</sup> that "being highly sedentary or highly active independently increased the risk of persistent fatigue, suggesting that divergence in either direction from healthy levels of activity increases the risk for persistent fatigue."

      The authors themselves point out that their "definition of being physically active (1 hour of exercise on >=2 days per week) is roughly similar to the current recommendations for adolescents of the President’s Council on Physical Fitness and Sports and the National Association for Sport and Physical Education: “teens should do at least 20 minutes of vigorous activity 3 days a week and 30 minutes of moderate activity 5 days a week”.<sup>2</sup> So why is this level of activity, which was reported by 48.7% of the young people, being presented as being excessive? If they wanted to investigate being "highly active" (as opposed to simply being “active”), activity levels should not have been dichotomized at level they were in this study.

      The question about sedentary activities was: “Outside school hours, on average, how many hours a day do you usually watch TV or videos, play video games, or play on the computer?” If a young person was sedentary for >4 hours a day, it does not mean they was necessarily inactive; indeed in phase 1, 23% of active young people were sedentary for more than 4 hours per day (compared with 30% of inactive young people). Such a lifestyle could be associated with fatigue for other reasons; for example, it could result in a shortage of sleep, rather than it necessarily causing unhealthily low levels of activity.

      1 Viner RM, Clark C, Taylor SJ, Bhui K, Klineberg E, Head J, Booy R, Stansfeld SA. Longitudinal risk factors for persistent fatigue in adolescents. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2008 May;162(5):469-75.

      2 Corbin CB, Pangrazi RP, Le Masurier GC. Physical activity for children: current patterns and guidelines. Res Dig. 2004;5(2):1-8.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 07, Muhammad Aslam commented:

      This is an interesting study but there is a technical mistake. Hopefully the authors can respond to that. On page 1351 (page 2 of article) the authors state ''Here, we have used 2 such Ras effector mutants to identify selective contributions of Ras effectors of the extracellular signal-regulated kinase (ERK)/mitogen-activated protein kinase (MAPK) or PI3K pathway to vascular phenotypes in vivo. RasV12C40 (G12→V12, T40→C40) binds to and selectively activates PI3K, whereas RasV12S35 (G12→V12, Y35→S35) binds to Raf1 and selectively activates the ERK/MAPK pathway.''

      In original H-Ras sequence (NP_001123914.1) there exist no T40 or Y35 rather it is the other way round. In sequence it is T35 and Y40 which is changed to S35 and C40, respectively, as stated by article from Joneson T et al (Science 1996; 271: 810–812).

      Regards,


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Sep 23, Tero Kivelä commented:

      Figure 1 in this paper is problematic. It states to show survival curves for uveal melanoma patients. However, while there were 13 patients with uveal melanoma of whom 12 were treated, the figure shows 18 deaths.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Oct 28, DAVID SANDERS commented:

      See the earlier articles "Sulfhydryl involvement in fusion mechanisms" [2000]Sanders DA, 2000 and "Ancient viruses in the fight against HIV" [2003] Sanders DA, 2003 for the prediction of the results published here.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jan 08, Tom Kindlon commented:

      Various comments

      This paper refers to the use of a re-attribution programme. I thought I would highlight the results of a trial[1] published last year on the topic. It involved testing practice-based training of GPs in reattribution. The method to test the hypothesis was a "cluster randomised controlled trial in 16 practices, 74 GPs and 141 patients with medically unexplained symptoms of 6 hours of reattribution training v. treatment as usual." It found that "Practice-based training in reattribution changed doctor-patient communication without improving outcome of patients with medically unexplained symptoms". Hardly a ringing endorsement of the method.

      There has been a lot of hype about the effectiveness of Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) for Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS). However, a meta-analysis[2] of its efficacy of CBT for CFS published in 2008 might temper some of the enthusiasm. The studies involved a total of 1371 patients. This involved calculating the size of an effect measure, the Cohen's d value. They calculated d using the following method: "Separate mean effect sizes were calculated for each category of outcome variable (e.g., fatigue self-rating) and for each type of outcome variable (mental, physical, and mixed mental and physical). Studies generally included multiple outcome measures. For all analyses except those that compared different categories or types of outcome variables, we used the mean effect size of all the relevant outcome variables of the study."d was calculated to be 0.48. For anyone unfamiliar with Cohen's d values, they are not bounded by 1; also, the higher the score, the bigger the "effect size" i.e. the more "effective" a treatment was found to be. Cohen's d values are considered to be a small effect size at 0.2, a moderate effect size at 0.5, and a large effect size at 0.8[2].

      There are now hundreds of studies that have found "physical" abnormalities of one sort or another in Chronic Fatigue Syndrome. Thus I question the placement of "Chronic Fatigue" (which many/most people would read as referring to Chronic Fatigue Syndrome as it is listed beside Fibromyalgia and Irritable Bowel Syndrome) in figure 1, "Hypothetical scatter plot of dysfunction versus pathology in primary care consultations" where "evidence of pathological change" is said to be "absent". The numerous abnormalities found raise questions about the placement on the scatter plot or else the limitations of the concept. Also how "reversible" the "abnormal functioning, either physiological or psychological" is, remains far from clear given the low recovery rates.

      References:

      [1] Morriss R, Dowrick C, Salmon P, Peters S, Dunn G, Rogers A, Lewis B, Charles-Jones H, Hogg J, Clifford R, Rigby C, Gask L. Cluster randomised controlled trial of training practices in reattribution for medically unexplained symptoms. Br J Psychiatry. 2007 Dec;191:536-42

      [2] Malouff, J. M., et al., Efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy for chronic fatigue syndrome: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review (2007), doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.004

      [3] Cohen J: Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. Edited by: 2. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1988.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 13, Robert Eibl commented:

      After contacting the editor in 2008 about some issues with this paper, she suggested considering a “Corrigendum”, but did not publish it. Please find below my updated and slightly modified letter to the editor:

      Robert Eibl: Single-receptor adhesion measurements on living cells

      Helenius et al. (1) reviewed the use of atomic force microscopy (AFM) for single-cell force spectroscopy (SCFS). They listed in table 1 the references for "receptor-ligand interactions by SCFS using living cells as probes" and pointed to two reports on cell adhesion bonds between integrin alpha4beta1 (very late antigen 4, VLA-4) and its ligand vascular cell adhesion molecule 1 (VCAM-1). Surprisingly, however, Helenius and co-workers have not included the work of Eibl and Benoit (2) in their list, although this original finding on the same integrin to ligand interaction was published well before the two cited references appeared, i.e. 5 and 18 months, respectively, earlier.

      In addition to this major exclusion to the very first AFM report on VLA-4/VCAM-1 measurements at the single-molecule level on a living cell, table 1 contains two more mistakes. First, the information stated to be found in a referenced paper is actually not there: Thie et al. (3) serves as reference for the specific measurement of integrin alpha(L)beta2 (leukocyte function antigen 1, LFA-1) on its ligand interstitial cell adhesion molecule 1 (ICAM-1); these authors -- although including one of the co-authors of this review -- never claimed being able to specifically measure any cell adhesion receptor; on the contrary, they state that they could only speculate regarding the cell adhesion receptors involved in their generally unspecific measurements, which might include several integrins and other cell adhesion receptors. This error may also mislead readers with regard to several other aspects of the AFM technique for measuring leukocyte homing receptors with AFM at the single-molecule or single-receptor level, including the original developers of the approach and the time-frame in which it was developed. Second, table 1 also includes a minor, but repeated typing error: “concavalin A” instead of “concanavalin A”.

      In my view, a detailed step-by-step protocol in this area could have been included at that time in the review, too (4). For readers interested in an extensive overview of this topic, a book chapter reviews this subject and includes a similar table as well as further protocols for experiments (5). Despite the discussed errors, the review includes a very useful overview on many aspects between physics and biology, and may bring the AFM technology into the scope of cell biologists, i.e. the readers of that journal. The authors are free to use their own nomenclature, like SCFS, very consistently through the review, which may appear to be useful for the beginner, but may not always be specific enough: “single-cell” measurements appear to contradict measurements between two cells, and often SCFS is also used for so-called single-molecule measurements on a cell, but a more precise nomenclature was not in the scope of the review.

      REFERENCES

      (1) Helenius, J., Heisenberg, C.P., Gaub, H.E., Muller, D.J. (2008). Single-cell force spectroscopy. J. Cell. Sci. 121, 1785-91

      (2) Eibl, R.H. and Benoit, M. (2004). Molecular resolution of cell adhesion forces. IEE - Nanobiotechnology 151, 128-132

      (3) Thie M, Röspel R, Dettmann W, Benoit M, Ludwig M, Gaub HE, Denker HW (1998). Interactions between trophoblast and uterine epithelium: monitoring of adhesive forces. Hum Reprod. (11):3211-9

      (4) Eibl, R.H. and Moy V.T. (2005). Atomic force microscopy measurements of protein-ligand interactions on living cells. In: Protein-Ligand Interactions. (Editor: G.Ulrich Nienhaus), Humana Press, Totowa, NJ, U.S.A., pp. 437-448 ISBN 1588293726

      (5) Eibl, R.H. (2013). Single-Molecule Studies of Integrins by AFM-Based Force Spectroscopy on Living Cells. Scanning Probe Microscopy in Nanoscience and Nanotechnology 3: 137-169, ISBN 978-3-642-25414-7_6


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 17, Raphael Levy commented:

      The article has been corrected after re-use of a figure from a previous article had been raised.

      There is barely any evidence for the existence of this particular type of stripy nanoparticles blog post, and, more broadly, the evidence behind the structure and special properties of “striped” nanoparticles has been challenged by Cesbron Y, 2012. The publication in 2012 of Cesbron Y, 2012 took three years and has been followed by post-publication peer review of the various existing and new stripy articles on my blog, PubPeer, etc.

      A detailed analysis of this body of work is published today in PloS One by Stirling et al; from the abstract: “through a combination of an exhaustive re-analysis of the original data with new experimental measurements of a simple control sample comprising entirely unfunctionalised particles, we conclusively show that all of the STM evidence for striped nanoparticles published to date can instead be explained by a combination of well-known instrumental artefacts, strong observer bias, and/or improper data acquisition/analysis protocols.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Mar 02, Ali Dadban commented:

      Couldn't find it in the Archives of the journal's website.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Aug 23, Ben Goldacre commented:

      This trial has the wrong trial registry ID associated with it on PubMed: both in the XML on PubMed, and in the originating journal article. The ID given is NCT00364963. We believe the correct ID, which we have found by hand searching, is NCT00364936.

      This comment is being posted as part of the OpenTrials.net project<sup>[1]</sup> , an open database threading together all publicly accessible documents and data on each trial, globally. In the course of creating the database, and matching documents and data sources about trials from different locations, we have identified various anomalies in datasets such as PubMed, and in published papers. Alongside documenting the prevalence of problems, we are also attempting to correct these errors and anomalies wherever possible, by feeding back to the originators. We have corrected this data in the OpenTrials.net database; we hope that this trial’s text and metadata can also be corrected at source, in PubMed and in the accompanying paper.

      Many thanks,

      Jessica Fleminger, Ben Goldacre*

      [1] Goldacre, B., Gray, J., 2016. OpenTrials: towards a collaborative open database of all available information on all clinical trials. Trials 17. doi:10.1186/s13063-016-1290-8 PMID: 27056367

      * Dr Ben Goldacre BA MA MSc MBBS MRCPsych<br> Senior Clinical Research Fellow<br> ben.goldacre@phc.ox.ac.uk<br> www.ebmDataLab.net<br> Centre for Evidence Based Medicine<br> Department of Primary Care Health Sciences<br> University of Oxford<br> Radcliffe Observatory Quarter<br> Woodstock Road<br> Oxford OX2 6GG


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Aug 19, Morten Oksvold commented:

      Please note that this article has been retracted and should therefore not be cited in the future.

      http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ijc.30270/full


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Aug 20, Daniel Weiss commented:

      This paper employs flawed circular reasoning.

      The methods section clearly states that “in all patients with neurologic, cardiac or joint involvement, a serologic test positive for B. burgdorferi by ELISA and Western blot was required for case inclusion.”

      Then in the results section, the authors state that “among the 44 patients with neurologic, heart or joint abnormalities, all had positive IgM or IgG responses to B. burgdorferi with 2 tier testing”… 2 tier testing had a sensitivity of 100%” .

      This often cited manuscript claims that two tier testing has a sensitivity of 100% among those who have positive two tier tests. I urge all to read this paper before citing it.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Oct 02, Juliane Bremer commented:

      In this review article there is a transposition error in Figure 2, page 444. The F198S-129V mutation in the prion protein that is a cause of Gerstmann-Straussler-Scheinker syndrome, is incorrectly indicated as F189S-129V.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Oct 16, David Keller commented:

      How did same-sex preference genes persist despite their effects on reproduction?

      Evidence suggests the existence of genes which promote homosexual preference. A gene which tends to decrease its own reproduction in this way must have some countervailing effect in order to persist in the gene pool.

      If the same mutation which promotes homosexuality in men also increases fecundity in women, it could persist in equilibrium with wild-type alleles. The reduced number of offspring of male carriers would have to be offset by an increased number of offspring in female carriers large enough to avoid the disappearance of the gene which would otherwise occur over time.

      Another possibility is that social repression of homosexuality had the paradoxical effect of promoting the survival of same-sex preference genes. Modern society's tolerance, by reducing the social pressure to reproduce, could ironically lead to the gradual disappearance of genes which promote homosexual preference.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Nov 20, Morten Oksvold commented:

      An investigation committee at Wayne State University (WSU) recommends that 42 articles from Fazlul Sarkar to be retracted (report finished August 31, 2015). This article represents one of them.

      This information was published by Retraction Watch (November 17, 2016) and you can find a link to the full report here:

      http://retractionwatch.com/2016/11/17/details-of-investigative-report-into-sarkar-released-by-aclu/

      This article should therefore no longer be cited.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Nov 30, GianCarlo Panzica commented:

      A following study in the same rat model, demonstrated that also the parvocellular arginine-vasopressin system of the BST and amygdal is altered in Tfm males, suggesting that also this system is depending by androgens for its full differentiation Allieri F, 2013


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Oct 26, Michael Eisen commented:

      Would like to point to a followup paper from our lab: Lusk RW, 2010 used simulations of enhancer evolution to examine one of the conclusions of this paper - that the enrichment and conservation of paired binding sites we observe is the result of selection for paired sites - and found that a selection on binding site composition alone in the presence of a bias for deletions over insertions can result in an enrichment of clustered binding sites that appear (but are not) to be under purifying selection.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Dec 30, Tom Kindlon commented:

      My published response: "Change in grey matter volume cannot be assumed to be due to cognitive behavioural therapy"

      I had a letter published in reply to the authors' response to the Inge Bramsen letter. Among other things, I highlighted that there was no CFS control group in this study, so one shouldn't assume any change was due to CBT e.g. it could be due to the passage of time (plausible given people with CFS, once diagnosed, are more likely to improve than deteriorate, at least in the short term). The letter can be read here: http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/content/132/7/e119.long


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Aug 23, Ben Goldacre commented:

      This trial has the wrong trial registry ID associated with it on PubMed: both in the XML on PubMed, and in the originating journal article. The ID given is NCT00605058. We believe the correct ID, which we have found by hand searching, is NCT00605085.

      This comment is being posted as part of the OpenTrials.net project<sup>[1]</sup> , an open database threading together all publicly accessible documents and data on each trial, globally. In the course of creating the database, and matching documents and data sources about trials from different locations, we have identified various anomalies in datasets such as PubMed, and in published papers. Alongside documenting the prevalence of problems, we are also attempting to correct these errors and anomalies wherever possible, by feeding back to the originators. We have corrected this data in the OpenTrials.net database; we hope that this trial’s text and metadata can also be corrected at source, in PubMed and in the accompanying paper.

      Many thanks,

      Jessica Fleminger, Ben Goldacre*

      [1] Goldacre, B., Gray, J., 2016. OpenTrials: towards a collaborative open database of all available information on all clinical trials. Trials 17. doi:10.1186/s13063-016-1290-8 PMID: 27056367

      * Dr Ben Goldacre BA MA MSc MBBS MRCPsych<br> Senior Clinical Research Fellow<br> ben.goldacre@phc.ox.ac.uk<br> www.ebmDataLab.net<br> Centre for Evidence Based Medicine<br> Department of Primary Care Health Sciences<br> University of Oxford<br> Radcliffe Observatory Quarter<br> Woodstock Road<br> Oxford OX2 6GG


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Jul 20, Salzman Lab Journal Club commented:

      This paper provides strong evidence for the dramatic functional change in protein coding enabled by precise RNA editing. An interesting followup would be to see if there are changes in expression levels of the editing guide RNA during the course of trypanosome development. Also, it would be interesting to know the level of expression of the dominant negative AEP1-GFP protein relative to endogenous levels of AEP1.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Dec 06, Mark Bolland commented:

      There are several errors/inconsistencies in the data related to treatment group numbers and numbers of falls and fractures in this paper. These include:

      • 1. The text and Table 1 reports 121 participants in each treatment group, but Table 3 reports 120 for Ca and 122 for CaD.
      • 2. Table 3 reports that 75 participants fell with Ca and 49 with CaD, but the breakdown of fallers in Table 3 sums to 71 for Ca and 53 for CaD.
      • 3. The text reports the total number of falls as 171 with Ca and 76 with CaD, but Table 3 reports 169 with Ca and 106 with CaD.
      • 4. The breakdown of total falls in Table 3 sums to at least 111 with CaD, which is greater than the total falls for CaD reported in the text (76) and Table 3 (106).
      • 5. The text reports the mean number of falls per group as 1.41 with Ca and 0.63 with CaD which are incompatible with the reported total number of falls in the text and Table 3.
      • 6. The text reports 13 participants with fracture with Ca whereas Table 3 reports 12.

      These are discussed in two letters to the editor (Osteoporos Int 2015;26:2713 Bolland MJ, 2015 and Arch Osteoporos. 2015;10:43 Bolland MJ, 2015) with responses by the lead author to each letter (Osteoporos Int. 2015;26:2715-6 Pfeifer M, 2015 and Arch Osteoporos. 2015;10:42 Pfeifer M, 2015).

      Given the unwillingness/inability of the author to provide consistent data from the trial, we think it should be excluded from systematic reviews or meta-analyses until consistent data are provided by the authors, with some explanation as to how the errors/inconsistencies occurred.

      We have described the full sequence of events in a separate comment.

      Mark Bolland, Andrew Grey. University of Auckland


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2015 Dec 06, Mark Bolland commented:

      This comment provides background information to an earlier comment.

      We were interested in why different meta-analyses of vitamin D supplements came to different conclusions, and noticed that different meta-analyses used different data from this trial. We identified several errors/inconsistencies in the text and emailed the lead author requesting clarification about the data in March 2014. He responded that the data in the Tables were correct. We replied that there were inconsistencies within the Tables as well and asked for clarification, but he did not respond.

      Therefore in April 2014, we contacted the editor of Osteoporosis International advising the editor of the inconsistencies and requested that they be corrected. We felt this is particularly important as it is a highly cited, influential trial reporting benefits of vitamin D supplements on falls, and the inconsistencies occur in the treatment group numbers and the primary and secondary outcomes, so have a major bearing of the interpretation of the trial results. Our primary concern was/is to use the correct data for the trial in meta-analyses. In May 2014, the editor passed on an extract of the lead author’s response and indicated that an erratum would be published. However, we felt the lead author’s response was inadequate because it left a number of uncorrected inconsistencies/errors in the text. We pointed this out to the editor. We are not sure what action the editor took, but no erratum was published. We followed up with 2 further emails to the editor over the next year, and in April 2015, the editor advised that it seemed unlikely that an erratum would be forthcoming.

      Therefore, we requested the permission to summarize the issues in a very brief letter. The editor agreed and a short letter summarizing the six errors/inconsistencies in the article was published (Osteoporos Int 2015;26:2713 Bolland MJ, 2015) with a response from the author (Osteoporos Int. 2015;26:2715-6 Pfeifer M, 2015). Unfortunately, in his response the author chose to correct only 2 of the errors identified and introduced a further inconsistency.

      In a further letter to the editor, we therefore highlighted the remaining errors/inconsistencies and the consequence of using different possible data combinations from this trial for meta-analyses. The editor indicated that the issues we raised were important but probably irresolvable and offered to publish the letter in Archives of Osteoporosis. We indicated that our preference was that the data were corrected in the original publication rather than our letter being published. We think it quite straightforward to make the simple necessary corrections to two tables and a couple of sentences of text. We feel it is essential that the corrections occur as the errors are in the most important data from the trial: the treatment group numbers and the primary outcome data for falls. The editor considered the issue further and then published our letter (Arch Osteoporos. 2015;10:43 Bolland MJ, 2015) along with a response from the author (Arch Osteoporos. 2015;10:42 Pfeifer M, 2015).

      In this second response, the author has still not corrected the identified errors, so, because of the inconsistencies/errors in the data, it is not possible to be sure how many women were in each randomized treatment group, how many women had a fall during the trial, how many total falls occurred in each treatment group, or what is the breakdown of participants by numbers of falls (no falls, 1 fall, 2 falls, etc).

      Therefore, we think that the study should be excluded from meta-analyses and systematic reviews and its results viewed with caution until consistent data are provided by the authors with some explanation as to how the errors/inconsistencies occurred.

      Mark Bolland, Andrew Grey. University of Auckland


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Dec 18, Claudia Paiva commented:

      Is it possible that the recurrency is overestimated because patients without recurrent events leave the hospital and are excluded from the study? 1334 cases were found and 127 chosen because they could be followed for long term... The rate of recurrency among high-tone controls is 11% (without vertigo) and 17% (without SN), while in the literature the total recurrency among SSNHL sufferers is approximately 2%.

      Alternatively, it could be due to a higher rate of recurrency in Japan, since no western studies were made concerning the recurrency rates among low tone hearing loss in western countries. Do you know any broad study in Japan that show the total recurrency rate of SSNHL in Japan and percentages of high x low tone hearing loss among total SSNHL?

      It is very important to define the rate of recurrency, so that the patient is aware of the possibility and plot a plan to face it with drs (in case it is high) or can live without the constant burden of "is it going to happen again?" in their heads (in case it is low).

      Anyway, these are just considerations made by a worried low tone SSNHL patient and a scientist from another field.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Apr 25, Sergio Uribe commented:

      Can caries in primary teeth predict caries in permanent teeth in Sweden? Yes considering this paper; No according to https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23235131


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Aug 23, Ben Goldacre commented:

      This trial has the wrong trial registry ID associated with it on PubMed: both in the XML on PubMed, and in the originating journal article. The ID given is NCT005725079. We believe the correct ID, which we have found by hand searching, is NCT00572507.

      This comment is being posted as part of the OpenTrials.net project<sup>[1]</sup> , an open database threading together all publicly accessible documents and data on each trial, globally. In the course of creating the database, and matching documents and data sources about trials from different locations, we have identified various anomalies in datasets such as PubMed, and in published papers. Alongside documenting the prevalence of problems, we are also attempting to correct these errors and anomalies wherever possible, by feeding back to the originators. We have corrected this data in the OpenTrials.net database; we hope that this trial’s text and metadata can also be corrected at source, in PubMed and in the accompanying paper.

      Many thanks,

      Jessica Fleminger, Ben Goldacre*

      [1] Goldacre, B., Gray, J., 2016. OpenTrials: towards a collaborative open database of all available information on all clinical trials. Trials 17. doi:10.1186/s13063-016-1290-8 PMID: 27056367

      * Dr Ben Goldacre BA MA MSc MBBS MRCPsych<br> Senior Clinical Research Fellow<br> ben.goldacre@phc.ox.ac.uk<br> www.ebmDataLab.net<br> Centre for Evidence Based Medicine<br> Department of Primary Care Health Sciences<br> University of Oxford<br> Radcliffe Observatory Quarter<br> Woodstock Road<br> Oxford OX2 6GG


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Jun 22, Rick Gerkin commented:

      This is the classic that brought the application of generalized linear models to the understanding of sensory systems into the mainstream.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 May 15, Bill Sardi commented:

      This abstract does not do justice to this landmark study. 12 weeks on an CR diet significantly differentiated 198 genes, resveratrol 225 genes,resveratrol+polyphenol (Longevinex) diet 1711 genes. Life-long CR diet in mice differentiates 831 genes. This suggests what takes a lifetime to accomplish epigenetically can be accomplished over a shorter time! Furthermore, the nutraceutical switched 677 of 831 (82%) longevity genes in the same direction (expressed or silenced) as CR, which makes this nutraceutical the closest molecular mimic of CR to date. (Note: I have a commercial interest in this nutraceutical)


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Dec 29, Keizo Takao commented:

      The raw data of behavioral tests, which are not described in this paper, are shown in the Mouse Phenotype Database (http://www.mouse-phenotype.org/). "ImageLD", "ImageEP", "ImageFZ", and "ImageTM", image analysis application softwares used in this article, are now freely available from http://www.mouse-phenotype.org/software.html.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 30, Harri Hemila commented:

      Brundage JF, 2008 suggest that the high mortality associated with 1918-19 flu pandemic was caused by secondary bacterial pneumonia. They propose that antibiotics and bacterial vaccines should be stockpiled for the next flu pandemic.

      I would like to suggest that as part of pandemic-related research activities, the effect of vitamin C on bacterial pneumonia should be investigated. In dozens of animal studies, vitamin C protected against infections by various viruses and bacteria, see Hemilä 2006, pp. 5-9, 105-21.

      In the early 20th century, Alfred Hess carried out extensive studies of scurvy and summarized a large series of autopsy findings as follows: “pneumonia, lobular or lobar, is one of the most frequent complications [of scurvy] and causes of death” and “secondary pneumonias, usually broncho-pneumonic in type, are of common occurrence, and in many [scurvy] epidemics constitute the prevailing cause of death”, see Hemilä H, 2007. Thus, there seemed to be a close association between vitamin C and pneumonia.

      Hemilä H, 2013 carried out a Cochrane review, and found 3 controlled trials that looked at whether vitamin C prevents pneumonia and 2 that looked at whether it might help in curing pneumonia; 2 of them were RCTs. Each of the 5 trials found that vitamin C supplementation was beneficial.

      As to bacterial pneumonia caused by influenza A infection, Kimbarowski JA, 1967 is particularly relevant as they administered vitamin C to soldiers of the former USSR who were hospitalized because of influenza A. Their main purpose was to examine an investigational laboratory test; but, as a secondary issue, they reported the number of bronchopneumonia cases in the vitamin C and control groups after hospitalization. There were 10 cases of bronchopneumonia in the control group compared with 2 cases in the vitamin C group (P = 0.02, Fisher’s exact test). Although the trial is methodologically unsatisfactory in comparison with current standards, the difference in the occurrence of pneumonia in the study groups cannot be dismissed because of obvious biases Hemilä H, 2013. Methodologically satisfactory trials are needed to corroborate or refute the possibility that vitamin C has an effect on bacterial pneumonia caused by influenza.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Aug 24, Ben Goldacre commented:

      This trial has the wrong trial registry ID associated with it on PubMed: both in the XML on PubMed, and in the originating journal article. The ID given is NCT03318019. We believe the correct ID, which we have found by hand searching, is NCT00318019.

      This comment is being posted as part of the OpenTrials.net project<sup>[1]</sup> , an open database threading together all publicly accessible documents and data on each trial, globally. In the course of creating the database, and matching documents and data sources about trials from different locations, we have identified various anomalies in datasets such as PubMed, and in published papers. Alongside documenting the prevalence of problems, we are also attempting to correct these errors and anomalies wherever possible, by feeding back to the originators. We have corrected this data in the OpenTrials.net database; we hope that this trial’s text and metadata can also be corrected at source, in PubMed and in the accompanying paper.

      Many thanks,

      Jessica Fleminger, Ben Goldacre*

      [1] Goldacre, B., Gray, J., 2016. OpenTrials: towards a collaborative open database of all available information on all clinical trials. Trials 17. doi:10.1186/s13063-016-1290-8 PMID: 27056367

      * Dr Ben Goldacre BA MA MSc MBBS MRCPsych<br> Senior Clinical Research Fellow<br> ben.goldacre@phc.ox.ac.uk<br> www.ebmDataLab.net<br> Centre for Evidence Based Medicine<br> Department of Primary Care Health Sciences<br> University of Oxford<br> Radcliffe Observatory Quarter<br> Woodstock Road<br> Oxford OX2 6GG


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Dec 14, Rafael Najmanovich commented:

      An online web-tool to detect binding site similarities against a database of non-redundant ligand-protein combinations using IsoCleft has been published here: Kurbatova N, 2013


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Mar 01, Jörg Hakenberg commented:

      GNAT results for Medline citations are now available from our Sourceforge project for download. Please see https://sourceforge.net/projects/gnat/files/results/medline/ .


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Aug 23, Ben Goldacre commented:

      This trial has the wrong trial registry ID associated with it on PubMed: both in the XML on PubMed, and in the originating journal article. The ID given is NCT003229758. We believe the correct ID, which we have found by hand searching, is NCT00329758.

      This comment is being posted as part of the OpenTrials.net project<sup>[1]</sup> , an open database threading together all publicly accessible documents and data on each trial, globally. In the course of creating the database, and matching documents and data sources about trials from different locations, we have identified various anomalies in datasets such as PubMed, and in published papers. Alongside documenting the prevalence of problems, we are also attempting to correct these errors and anomalies wherever possible, by feeding back to the originators. We have corrected this data in the OpenTrials.net database; we hope that this trial’s text and metadata can also be corrected at source, in PubMed and in the accompanying paper.

      Many thanks,

      Jessica Fleminger, Ben Goldacre*

      [1] Goldacre, B., Gray, J., 2016. OpenTrials: towards a collaborative open database of all available information on all clinical trials. Trials 17. doi:10.1186/s13063-016-1290-8 PMID: 27056367

      * Dr Ben Goldacre BA MA MSc MBBS MRCPsych<br> Senior Clinical Research Fellow<br> ben.goldacre@phc.ox.ac.uk<br> www.ebmDataLab.net<br> Centre for Evidence Based Medicine<br> Department of Primary Care Health Sciences<br> University of Oxford<br> Radcliffe Observatory Quarter<br> Woodstock Road<br> Oxford OX2 6GG


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Feb 10, Arnaud Chiolero MD PhD commented:

      This fantastic paper gives a clear explanation of why it is essential to think about specific interventions to assess the causal effect of overweight on health outcomes.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Oct 24, Tom Kindlon commented:

      Information on occupational performance does not suggest 37% of the patients were recovered

      In a comment,<sup>1</sup> I pointed out that the thresholds for recovery using were set very low. However it is useful to investigate the other data to see what it might say about recovery rates. Also other data from other studies.

      This paper claims that CBT led to a recovery rate of 37% in CFS patients. That is one of the highest if not the highest recovery rate I can recall from a CBT study.

      It is my impression that, outside the Netherlands, claims of CBT leading to recovery in CFS are not that common. For example, Prof. Simon Wessely, who could be said to be one of the chief proponents of CBT for CFS worldwide over the last couple of decades has said that CBT is not "remotely curative".<sup>2</sup>

      Recently, a meta-analysis of the efficacy of CBT for CFS was published.<sup>3</sup> The studies involved a total of 1371 patients. This involved calculating the size of an effect measure, the Cohen's d value.

      They calculated d using the following method: "Separate mean effect sizes were calculated for each category of outcome variable (e.g., fatigue self- rating) and for each type of outcome variable (mental, physical, and mixed mental and physical). Studies generally included multiple outcome measures. For all analyses except those that compared different categories or types of outcome variables, we used the mean effect size of all the relevant outcome variables of the study."

      d was calculated to be 0.48.

      For anyone unfamiliar with Cohen's d values, they are not bounded by 1; also, the higher the score, the bigger the "effect size" i.e. the more "effective" a treatment was found to be. Cohen's d values are considered to be a small effect size at 0.2, a moderate effect size at 0.5, and a large effect size at 0.8.<sup>4</sup>

      So this suggests CBT leading to a 37% recovering would be unusual.

      I noticed in the discussion section, we are told: "Concerning work productivity, fewer patients had a paid job after treatment than before, but the mean hours of paid work per week had increased after treatment." We are not given the percentage of patients who work.

      What we are told is that the median number of hours actually worked before the treatment was 0 hours. That means that at least 50% of the patients were working 0 hours per week. For the whole group, they worked a mean number of 9.4 hours. Some of these people who were not actually working were actually in paid employment at the start as the median number of hours they were contracted to work was 7 hours. Like most studies of adults with CFS, these patients were of working age. The mean age was 38.1 with a standard deviation of 10.2. 34% were male (larger than most studies) and 66% were female.

      Given the sample group, one would think that an "effective" treatment for CFS which is supposed to improve functioning and which the authors claim led to a recovery rate of 37% would dramatically effect the professional functioning of the patients.

      Indeed at least one study of CBT has used hours worked in its definition of recovery: "Predetermined criteria for "complete recovery" required that patients no longer met chronic fatigue syndrome criteria, were employed full-time, and scored less than 4 on the Fatigue Questionnaire and more than 83 on the Medical Outcomes Study Short-Form General Health Survey physical functioning scale."

      However in the current study we find that the mean number of hours work they are contracted to work actually decreased from a mean (SD) of 16.2 (16.3) to 14.9 (16.2) and given that median number of hours actually worked after the intervention is 0 hours, more than 50% of the patients are still not working at the end.

      [The number of hours actually worked did increase from a mean (SD) of 9.4 (13.5) to 11.4 (14.7) but a quick t-test suggests this isn't statistically significant].

      (contd.)


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2013 Oct 24, Tom Kindlon commented:

      (This follows on from an earlier comment that I thought was already long enough)

      Some of the calculations in this study use in the study use the recovery rate of 37%.

      For example: "Given the recovery rate of 37% the COR of implementing CBT for CFS was 5.320 per recovered CFS patient. The COR acceptability curve (figure 3) shows that the probability that implementing CBT for CFS has a favorable COR is 100% when the decision maker values a recovered CFS patient at least 6.500."

      They become very different if the threshold for recovery was much different.

      Alternatively, with the lax definition for recovery they used, the amount of people who would have "recovered" without treatment may be a lot higher than the 5% assumed in this study.

      "Finally, to get an impression of this study's results when compensating for spontaneous recovery, an additional analysis was performed. This was done from the health care perspective, presuming a spontaneous recovering rate of 5% [2], implying a recovery rate due to treatment of 32%. It revealed that the COR would rise from 5.320 to about 5.969 per recovered patient."

      I also don't understand why a "recovered group" should have a much worse average score in a domain e.g. around the lower end of any scale. Surely the null hypothesis would be that there is no difference between the means and if this is not satisfied, it's not a "recovered group". What could be done would be that some people would have to be taken out of the "recovered group" until one got to a situation where the mean of the recovered group was within a confidence interval for the mean of the normal population (the variance of a mean is of course much smaller than the variance of an individual entry i.e. the average value would have to be "pretty close" to the mean of the healthy population). Of course, it could be the case that there would only be a recovered group of 1 (say) as none of the values were above the mean.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2013 Oct 24, Tom Kindlon commented:

      Another paper by the authors provides information on the effects of self-efficacy and fatigue severity on health use

      I find it strange that in this paper, there is no mention of: "Determinants of health care use in chronic fatigue syndrome patients: a cross-sectional study." That study was written the four authors of the current study plus one other person.<sup>1</sup>

      I'm far from an expert on health economics but it seems to have information and data relevant to the current study or at least worthy of mention in the discussion section.

      They found for example, that: "self-efficacy showed a positive instead of a negative relation with health care use. We checked the direct correlation between self-efficacy and health care use, which also appeared to be positive (Pearson's R=0.12, p=.04). It thus seems that using more health care services might form an aspect of, or is stimulated by, a high self-efficacy instead of being the result of a low self-efficacy."

      One of the aims of CBT for CFS is to increase self-efficacy. As they say in the paper: "Subsequently, dysfunctional fatigue related cognitions are being challenged to diminish somatic attributions of fatigue, to improve a sense of control over symptoms and to facilitate behavior change. Finally a plan for work rehabilitation is outlined and worked out. Patients without a paid job focus on rehabilitation in other personal activities. The last session deals with relapse prevention and further improvement of self-control."

      Self-efficacy is a commonly used term in the literature on CBT for CFS.

      That study<sup>1</sup> also found that: "Fatigue severity itself showed no relation with health care use." So improving fatigue scores will not necessarily change health care use.

      References:

      [1] Scheeres K, Wensing M, Severens H, Adang E, Bleijenberg G. Determinants of health care use in chronic fatigue syndrome patients: a cross-sectional study. J Psychosom Res. 2008 Jul;65(1):39-46.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    4. On 2014 Jan 08, Tom Kindlon commented:

      Thresholds for recovery were set very very "low" (perhaps the bottom percentile and 5th lowest percentile of the healthy adult population on the two scales used)

      (I originally posted this as a comment here: http://www.biomedcentral.com/1472-6963/8/175/comments. However all the paragraph breaks have been deleted so I doubt many would read it there)

      The thresholds for recovery seem very very low: "Patients were defined as being CSI at post treatment if they had a reliable change index > 1.96 on the CIS fatigue severity subscale [22], a fatigue severity score <= 35 and a Rand-36 physical functioning score > = 65".

      Many of the patients already likely had a "physical functioning score >=65" given the mean (SD) values before treatment were: "Physical impairment (Rand 36) 54.0(23.4)" And the threshold for recovery was only 0.47 SDs above the initial mean score. I am aware of the questions on the SF-36 PF subscale (scores can range from 0 to 100 with the higher the score, the better their "physical functionaling") and I don't believe most healthy adults would believe scoring 65 on the SF-36 PF scale would mean they were recovered. As a study[1], that was co-written by one of the authors of this study (Gijs Bleijenberg), pointed out, a community study found that "healthy adults without a chronic condition" had "a mean score of 93.1 (SD 11.7)." The authors of that study[1] pointed out they did not know the exact distribution of the SF-36 subscales - they just made the assumption that the mean - 1SD would represent a threshold for the 85th percentile and rounded this figure to 80.The threshold in the current study is 65. That is 2.4 SDs below the healthy population's mean score. If the same assumptions were made (i.e. that the curve was normally distributed), this would represent the bottom percentile!

      For the CIS fatigue severity subscale (where the possible scores are 8-56 with the higher the score, the greater the fatigue), that same study that Gijs Bleijenberg co-wrote[2] used (to calculate thresholds i.e. from another study) a "normal group of 53 healthy adults with a mean age of 37.1 (SD 11.5)" who had "a mean score on the CIS-fatigue of 17.3 (SD 10.1)."[3] The ages of those healthy adults are similar to the ages of the CFS patients in this study: Mean (SD) 38.1 (10.2). In that study[1], they estimated that the 85th percentile (mean+1SD) would be 27 (due to rounding). This study uses 35 or the mean + 1.7525SD or the 95th percentile. Put another way, patients in this study could be considered recovered if they scored in the bottom percentile on the physical functioning subscale (of the SF-36) and in the 5th lowest percentile on the CIS-fatigue scale!

      References:

      [1] Knoop H, Bleijenberg G, Gielissen MF, van der Meer JW, White PD. Is a full recovery possible after cognitive behavioural therapy for chronic fatigue syndrome? Psychother Psychosom. 2007;76(3):171-6.

      [2] Aaronson NK, Muller M, Cohen PD, Essink-Bot ML, Fekkes M, Sanderman R, Sprangers MA, te Velde A, Verrips E: Translation, validation, and norming of the Dutch language version of the SF-36 Health Survey in community and chronic disease population. J Clin Epidemiol 1998; 51: 1055-1068.

      [3] Vercoulen JHMM, Alberts M, Bleijenberg G: De Checklist Individual Strength (CIS) (The Checklist Individual Strength). Gedragstherapie (Behavioural Therapy) 1999; 32: 642-649.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Dec 28, Tsuyoshi Miyakawa commented:

      "ImageLD", an image analysis appilcation software shown in this video article, is now freely availble from http://www.mouse-phenotype.org/software.html.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 30, Harri Hemila commented:

      Morens DM, 2008 examined pieces of lung tissues from 1918-19 flu pandemic victims and perused contemporary autopsy reports, concluding that the high mortality associated with influenza was caused by secondary bacterial pneumonia. They suggested that antibiotics and bacterial vaccines should be stockpiled for the next flu pandemic.

      I would like to propose that as part of pandemic-related research activities, the effect of vitamin C on bacterial pneumonia should be investigated.

      In mice, influenza A infection decreases the level of vitamin C in the lungs Hennet T, 1992, and vitamin C deficiency leads to more severe pathological changes in these organs Li W, 2006. In dozens of animal studies, vitamin C protected against infections by various viruses and bacteria Hemilä 2006, pp. 5-9,105-21. In the early 20th century, Alfred Hess carried out extensive studies of scurvy and summarized a large series of autopsy findings as follows: “pneumonia, lobular or lobar, is one of the most frequent complications [of scurvy] and causes of death” and “secondary pneumonias, usually broncho-pneumonic in type, are of common occurrence, and in many [scurvy] epidemics constitute the prevailing cause of death”, see Hemilä H, 2007. Furthermore, in about two dozen placebo-controlled trials, vitamin C reduced the duration and severity of the common cold suggesting that the vitamin may have effects on the respiratory system of humans even in the absence of frank deficiency Hemilä H, 2013.

      Because of the evidence suggesting that vitamin C may have an effect on pneumonia, Hemilä H, 2013 carried out a Cochrane review and found three controlled trials that looked at whether vitamin C prevents pneumonia and two that looked at whether it might help in curing pneumonia. Each of the five trials found that vitamin C supplementation was beneficial. Two of the studies were double-blind placebo-controlled RCTs, one was prophylactic and the other therapeutic.

      Pitt HA, 1979 administered vitamin C to US marine recruits and reported 7 cases of pneumonia in the placebo-group compared with 1 case in the vitamin C group. Hunt C, 1994 administered vitamin C to elderly people who were admitted to hospital in the UK because of bronchopneumonia or acute exacerbation of chronic bronchitis. They reported a significant decrease in the “total respiratory score” by vitamin C administration, and 5 deaths in the placebo group compared with 1 death in the vitamin C group. Hunt et al. tested the effect of vitamin C “over and above those of normal medication (mainly antibiotics and cough medicines) to which all participants were exposed” so that all their patients received antibiotics and vitamin C was not an alternative to them.

      As to bacterial pneumonia caused by influenza A infection, Kimbarowski JA, 1967 is particularly interesting as they administered vitamin C to soldiers of the former USSR who were hospitalized because of influenza A. Their main purpose was to examine an investigational laboratory test; however, as a secondary issue, they reported the number of bronchopneumonia cases in the study groups after hospitalization. The reason for diagnosing pneumonia was that the authors excluded those cases from their further study of the laboratory test. Thus, the pneumonia cases occurred after vitamin C supplementation was initiated for the influenza A patients. The two arms were balanced for the severity of influenza. The allocation method was not described but the study arms were of very similar size (112 versus 114 in the control and vitamin C arms) so it is possible that allocation occurred sequentially in the two trial arms. A placebo was not mentioned in the paper and apparently not used. Blinding of outcome assessment was not described; however, since pneumonia was a nuisance issue in their study, it seems improbable that the trial authors had substantial bias in their diagnosis of pneumonia. There were 10 cases of bronchopneumonia in the control group compared with 2 cases in the vitamin C group (P = .02, Fisher’s exact test).

      Even though Kimbarowski and Mokrow’s trial is methodologically unsatisfactory in comparison with current standards, the difference in the occurrence of pneumonia in the study groups cannot be dismissed because of obvious biases Hemilä H, 2013. Furthermore, the finding is consistent with other evidence suggesting that under some conditions vitamin C may affect respiratory infections. Methodologically satisfactory trials are needed to corroborate or refute the possibility that vitamin C has an effect on bacterial pneumonia caused by influenza.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Aug 05, Tracy Shields commented:

      According to a retraction statement published online 2 July 2014, the article was retracted "due to errors in the data analysis which affect the article's findings" by agreement between the journal Editor-in-Chief, the authors, and publisher. See: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/jocn.12652/abstract


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Dec 29, Keizo Takao commented:

      The raw data of behavioral tests, which are not described in this paper, are shown in the Mouse Phenotype Database (http://www.mouse-phenotype.org/). "ImageFZ", image analysis application software used in this article, is now freely available from http://www.mouse-phenotype.org/software.html.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Oct 03, Morten Oksvold commented:

      Please note that after an investigation at the University of Cologne, six articles where T. Wenz figures as first or senior author were found to contain questionable data due to scientific misconduct. This article is one of these six articles.

      The conclusion from the report was ready June 28, 2016, please see the link (in German):

      http://www.portal.uni-koeln.de/9015.html?&tx_news_pi1[news]=4335&tx_news_pi1[controller]=News&tx_news_pi1[action]=detail&cHash=1deb8399d7f796d65ca9f6ae4764a1ce


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jan 08, Brett Snodgrass commented:

      Dear Authors,

      Thank you for the excellent article. Please provide your kind consideration to the vascular nomenclature and the distinction between the Thebesian veins and the vessels of Wearn.

      Might the article be titled as the "Wearn Coronary System?" The fine (minute) nature of the vessels is consistent with the arteriosinusoidal type of vessels of Wearn. However, arterio-capillary-cameral & arterio-capillary-venular-cameral connections might be considered.

      The text highlights found at the following website may illustrate the difference.

      https://twitter.com/BrettSnodgrass1/status/417972133642240000/

      Comments and suggestions are welcome.

      Thank you kindly.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 May 18, Misha Koksharov commented:

      FYI:

      In contrast to Luciola mingrelica luciferase, in Photinus pyralis luc the homologous mutation Y33H doesn't affect pH-sensitivity of its color (at least there are no any differences in E. coli colonies compared with WT Ppy). Apparently, something is different in the surrounding interactions (it's not particularly surprising given the 67% sequence identity between the P. pyralis and L. mingrelica luciferases).

      Regarding this region of the 3D structure, the mutation D234G in Ppy luc comes to mind: it makes Ppy luc quite pH-resistant (hence, greenish colony color with low pH-dependent shift); however, in the fully pH-sensitive L. mingrelica luc the corresponding residue is already G236.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jan 27, Anders von Heijne commented:

      Development of visual diagnostic skills in pathology, as in radiology, is a sine qua non to become proficient in ones field. It requires training, feedback and guidance by more experienced collegues. It would be interesting if the author would like to update readers how the changes in the educational system actually impacted training and professional development, now several years after the original paper.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jan 22, Ernesto Guiraldes commented:

      This is an interesting report. I would suggest that the fact that the patient was eventually diagnosed as having Cystic fibrosis be made clear in the abstract.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Mar 18, Jacob Blumenthal commented:

      Hi, as the corresponding author of this paper, I will be happy to receive any of your comments. Please contact me via my current e-mail: jacob.blu@gmail.com or visit my site Stem Cells and Tissue Engineering.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Dec 29, Keizo Takao commented:

      The raw data of behavioral tests, which are not described in this paper, are shown in the Mouse Phenotype Database (http://www.mouse-phenotype.org/). "ImageLD", "ImageEP", and "ImageFZ", image analysis application softwares used in this article, are now freely available from http://www.mouse-phenotype.org/software.html.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Dec 29, Keizo Takao commented:

      The raw data of behavioral tests, which are not described in this paper, are shown in the Mouse Phenotype Database (http://www.mouse-phenotype.org/). "ImageEP", "ImageLD" and "ImageFZ", image analysis application softwares used in this article, are now freely available from http://www.mouse-phenotype.org/software.html.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Oct 20, Ivan Oransky commented:

      This study has been retracted. Here's background from Retraction Watch (which I co-founded): http://retractionwatch.wordpress.com/2013/10/09/retraction-appears-for-stem-cell-researcher-found-to-have-used-funds-for-his-companys-gain/

      And here is corresponding author Gerold Feuer's response to the retraction, in which he says he can "unequivocally state that the data in all published manuscripts is valid and sound and is not falsified or fabricated": http://retractionwatch.wordpress.com/2013/10/11/stem-cell-scientist-says-data-in-retracted-paper-is-not-falsified-or-fabricated/


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Aug 29, Massimo Ciccozzi commented:

      I read with interest the paper by Santosh et al. HIV 2 infections were geographically restricted, affecting West African countries1, 2, whereas in Europe the prevalence of HIV-2 is high in those countries that have socioeconomic relationships with this African region 3-5 . However, increased migration from/to other countries and international travels might increase the risk of spreading of this virus worldwide. We recently published a phylogenetic analysis of HIV2 case series in Italy using sequences isolated from Indian patients try to connect this infection with other from different countries 6. This article is very interesting and give important information on HIV-2 using a complete genome analysis. In Indian country few papers have been written in Phylogenetic analysis and Santosh and coauthors have given a sort of thrust in this way. In my opinion I only suggest the use of Bayesian Methods to have more robust information about the origin of Indian epidemics, moreover I encourage all Indian researchers, that are able to make sequences, to do this. It is also important that the scientific community don't lower the watch and monitor this infection also because to study HIV 2 can be an opportunity to better understand the HIV disease pathogenesis, protein immunity and this have to be taken before it disappears.<br> References

      1. Dougan S, Patel B, Tosswill JH, and Sinka K: Diagnoses of HIV-1 and HIV-2 in England, Wales, and Northern Ireland associated with west Africa. Sex Transm Infect 2005;81:338– 341.
      2. Matheron S, Mendoza-Sassi G, Simon F, Olivares R, Coulaud JP, and Brun-Vezinet F: HIV-1 and HIV-2 AIDS in African patients living in Paris. AIDS 1997;11:934–936.3

      3. Valadas E, Franc¸a L, Sousa S, and Antunes F: 20 years of HIV-2 infection in Portugal: Trends and changes in epidemiology. Clin Infect Dis 2009;48:1166–1167.

      4. Barin F, Cazein F, Lot F, et al.: Prevalence of HIV-2 and HIV-1 group O infections among new HIV diagnoses in France: 2003– 2006. AIDS 2007;21:2351–2353.

      5. Soriano V, Gomes P, Heneine W, et al.: Human immunodeficiency virus type 2 (HIV-2) in Portugal: Clinical spectrum, circulating subtypes, virus isolation, and plasma viral load. J Med Virol 2000;61:111–116.

      6. D’Ettorre,G, Lo Presti A,Gori C, Cella E,Bertoli A,Vullo V,Perno CF, Ciotti M,. Foley Brian T, and Massimo Ciccozzi. An HIV Type 2 Case Series in Italy: A Phylogenetic Analysis.(2013) AIDS Res HUM Retroviruses


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Oct 03, Brett D Thombs commented:

      The authors recommend routine depression screening for all heart patients. Even in primary care, where physicians are trained to manage depression, this is not recommended. This recommendation by the AHA was not based on a systematic review of evidence of benefit. See two systematic reviews:

      Thombs BD, de Jonge P, Coyne JC, Whooley MA, Frasure-Smith N, Mitchell AJ, Zuidersma M, Eze-Nliam C, Bezerra B, Smith CG, Soderlund K, Ziegelstein RC. Depression screening and patient outcomes in cardiovascular care: A systematic review. JAMA. 2008;300(18):2161-2171.

      Thombs BD, Roseman M, Coyne JC, de Jonge P, Delisle VC, Arthurs E, Levis B, Ziegelstein R. Does Evidence Support the American Heart Association's Recommendation to Screen Patients for Depression in Cardiovascular Care? An Updated Systematic Review. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(1):e52654.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Oct 24, Tom Kindlon commented:

      Data subsequently releasted from on this study reveals that there was no change in activity levels

      Some people may be interested to know that a review of three Dutch Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS) studies that was subsequently released<sup>1</sup> showed that this intervention did not result in an increase in physical activity levels in this study<sup>2</sup> along with two other Dutch CBT studies <sup>3,4.</sup> The authors say these three studies were based on the same general therapeutic approach to the illness.<sup>5</sup>

      The mean (standard deviations) for the guided self-instructions/CBT and Control groups were respectively 63.1 (23.5) and 63.5 (21.8) before treatment and 67.3 (22.5) and 67.8 (21.4) at the second assessment. In terms of change scores, this equates to: 4.3 (20.4) and 4.3 (21.0). Different devices can be used to measure activity levels; for the actometers used in this study, healthy controls were previously found to have a mean Actometer score of 91 (S.D.=25).<sup>6</sup> That study found that the mean Actometer score of tested CFS patients was 66 (S.D.=22).<sup>6</sup>

      Another research team in the US also found similar results with regard to physical activity.<sup>7</sup> In a study investigating an intervention involving Cognitive Behavior Therapy (CBT) which included encouraging CFS patients for going for longer walks, they found that on the SF-36 Physical Functioning (PF) scale, patients improved from a pre-treatment mean (SD) of 49.44 (25.19) to 58.18 (26.48) post-treatment, equivalent to a Cohen's d value of 0.35. On the Fatigue Severity Scale (FSS), the improvement as measured by the cohen's d value was even great (0.78) from an initial pre-treatment mean (SD) of 5.93 (0.93) to a 5.20 (0.95) post-treatment. However on actigraphy there was actually a numerical decrease from a pre-treatment mean (SD) of 224696.90 (158389.64) to 203916.67 (122585.92) post-treatment (cohen's d: -0.13).

      These studies raise questions about what are the best outcome measures to use in trials of CBT for CFS.

      References:

      [1] Wiborg JF, Knoop H, Stulemeijer M, Prins JB, Bleijenberg G. How does cognitive behaviour therapy reduce fatigue in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome? The role of physical activity. Psychol Med. 2010 Jan 5:1-7. [Epub ahead of print]

      [2] Knoop H, van der Meer JW, Bleijenberg G (2008). Guided self-instructions for people with chronic fatigue syndrome: randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry 193, 340-341.

      [3] Stulemeijer M, de Jong LW, Fiselier TJ, Hoogveld SW, Bleijenberg G (2005). Cognitive behaviour therapy for adolescents with chronic fatigue syndrome: randomised controlled trial. British Medical Journal 330. Published online : 7 December 2004. doi:10.1136/bmj.38301.587106.63.

      [4] Prins JB, Bleijenberg G, Bazelmans E, Elving LD, de Boo TM, Severens JL, van der Wilt GJ, Spinhoven P, van der Meer JW (2001). Cognitive behaviour therapy for chronic fatigue syndrome: a multicentre randomised controlled trial. Lancet 357, 841-847.

      [5] Bleijenberg G, Prins JB, Bazelmans E (2003). Cognitive behavioral therapies. In Handbook of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (ed. L. A. Jason, P. A. Fennell and R. R. Taylor), pp. 493-526. Wiley: New York.

      [6] Van der Werf SP, Prins JB, Vercoulen JH, van der Meer JW, Bleijenberg G (2000). Identifying physical activity patterns in chronic fatigue syndrome using actigraphic assessment. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 49, 373-379.

      [7] Friedberg F, Sohl S. Cognitive-behavior therapy in chronic fatigue syndrome: is improvement related to increased physical activity? J Clin Psychol. 2009 Feb 11.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2013 Oct 24, Tom Kindlon commented:

      Are there CBT studies with CFS controls that found objective (e.g. actometer) increases in activity?

      In an e-letter,<sup>1</sup> I pointed out the problem of the lack of use and/or reporting of objective outcome measures in the area.

      I have read more papers on the topic since then and it is interesting that the Nijmegen group themselves have been aware of this issue for over a decade. For example, two of the three co-authors of this study co-wrote a paper in 1997<sup>2</sup> which said: "It is not clear whether subjective accounts of physical activity level adequately reflect the actual level of physical activity. Therefore the primary aims of the present study were to assess actual activity level in patients with CFS to validate claims of lower levels of physical activity and to validate the reported relationship between fatigue and activity level that was found on self-report questionnaires. In addition, we evaluated whether physical activity level adequately can be assessed by self-report measures. An Accelerometer was used as a reference for actual level of physical activity.". The authors reported on the correlations on 7 outcome measures in relation to the actometer readings: "none of the self-report questionnaires had strong correlations with the Actometer. Thus, self-report questionnaires are no perfect parallel tests for the Actometer." The authors pointed out that "the subjective instruments do not measure actual behaviour. Responses on these instruments appear to be an expression of the patients' views about activity and may be biased by cognitions concerning illness and disability."

      This finding was re-iterated in another paper three years later again involving Bleijenberg and Van der Meer:<sup>3</sup> "In earlier studies of our research group, actual motor activity has been recorded with an ankle-worn motion-sensing device (actometer) in conjunction with self-report measures of physical activity. The data of these studies suggest that self-report measures of activity reflect the patients' view about their physical activity and may have been biased by cognitions concerning illness and disability."

      So could it be the case that Cognitive Behaviour Therapy for CFS is simply changing how patients respond to self-report questionnaires and that no actual changes of activity are occurring? Some research suggests this is possible.<sup>4</sup>

      References

      1 Kindlon T. How effective is the treatment for CFS? http://bjp.rcpsych.org/content/193/4/340.abstract/reply#bjprcpsych_el_22380

      2 Vercoulen JH, Bazelmans E, Swanink CM, Fennis JF, Galama JM, Jongen PJ, Hommes O, Van der Meer JW, Bleijenberg G. Physical activity in chronic fatigue syndrome: assessment and its role in fatigue. J Psychiatr Res. 1997 Nov-Dec;31(6):661-73.

      3 van der Werf SP, Prins JB, Vercoulen JH, van der Meer JW, Bleijenberg G. Identifying physical activity patterns in chronic fatigue syndrome using actigraphic assessment. J Psychosom Res. 2000 Nov;49(5):373-9.

      4 Wiborg JF, Knoop H, Stulemeijer M, Prins JB, Bleijenberg G. How does cognitive behaviour therapy reduce fatigue in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome? The role of physical activity. Psychol Med. 2010 Aug;40(8):1281-7.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2013 Oct 24, Tom Kindlon commented:

      Treating severely affected CFS patients with CBT

      This study found that the "more severely disabled patients benefit less from the self-instructions" (to be more specific, "the treatment effect is more than halved for patients with an SIP8 score of 1 standard deviation above the mean").

      At least one other Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) for Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS) study found that the outcome may be related to the level of impairment at baseline[1]. In this study, 10 (37%) reported being "better or much better" following treatment and 17 (63%) were the same or worse (we are not given a breakdown between the two categories).

      Comparing the two groups at baseline, the patients who reported improvement reported significantly less (p<0.05) functional impairment as measured by the SIP-8 [a mean (SD) of 1330 (417) vs 1985 (730)], less daily observed fatigue [7.4 (2.6) vs 9.7 (2.3)], and less daily observed pain [4.5 (2.6) vs 7.8 (3.5)]. For the pre-treatment variable "mean hours working a week" a trend (p=0.062) was found with improved patients working more hours at baseline compared to non-improved patients [10.9 (12.8) vs 2.6 (6.6)]. As with the current study, there was not a statistical difference on initial fatigue (CIS-fatigue). The paper<sup>1</sup> devotes quite a bit of space to this analysis including a table (Table 3).

      Reference:

      1 Bazelmans E, Prins JB, Lulofs R, van der Meer JW, Bleijenberg G. Cognitive behaviour group therapy for chronic fatigue syndrome: a non-randomised waiting list controlled study. Psychother Psychosom. 2005;74(4):218-24


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Jul 07, Morten Oksvold commented:

      This article should have been retracted after an investigation by The University of Maryland found this article to contain "compromised" data (a total of 26 articles in 11 journals were affected). The journal Cancer Research was informed in August 2016, according to Retraction Watch.

      http://retractionwatch.com/2017/04/26/university-asked-numerous-retractions-eight-months-later-three-journals-done-nothing/


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 06, Shervin Assari commented:

      One of the unique contributions by Professor Shemshadi to a field with limited studies.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Mar 07, Devi Prasad Mohapatra commented:

      This is a very interesting and practical article. Highly recommended for all cleft surgeons. What remains to be seen is how important is the experience of the operating surgeon and technique of palatoplasty in contributing to the formation of palatal fistula. Would the authors possibly throw some light on that?


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Nov 05, Pedro Mendes commented:

      This yeast metabolic reconstruction has since been further improved by Dobson PD, 2010, Heavner BD, 2012 and Heavner BD, 2013. The up to date data set is available from http://yeast.sf.net/.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jan 08, Tom Kindlon commented:

      The references don't show that CFS "affects at least 4 million adults in the United States"

      This is not necessarily a major point to do with the methodology. However given the paper involves CFS medical education, and gives few facts about CFS, one would think that the statements that are made are at least reasonably accurate.

      However, this statement clearly isn't: "CFS affects at least 4 million adults in the United States [2-4]." The references given are the first three references below. The second study found a prevalence of 422 per 100,000 adults and the third study found a prevalence of 235 per 100,000 adults. These aren't even close to 4 million adults - the second one suggests a figure of at least 400,000 adults. This study involved one of the authors (William C Reeves). The other study[1], also involved Reeves, did find a prevalence of 2540 per 100,000 adults, which would equate to over 4 million adults. As can be seen, this is much higher than previous estimates of the prevalence of CFS in the US. The reason for the huge discrepancy is largely because it used a different method of defining CFS[4].

      There has been some criticism of this new definition[5]. Unlike previous times when the CDC produced definitions for CFS[6,7], the definition used in this study is generally only being used by the CDC-funded CFS research team [the cohorts from the Wichita 2-day study in 2003 (not to be confused with the Reyes study) and the Georgia prevalence study[3]]. So it's far from clear that most people would accept that CFS "affects at least 4 million adults in the United States". But certainly two of the three studies given to back up this reference did not find anything close to such a prevalence.

      References:

      [1] Reeves WC, et al. Prevalence of chronic fatigue syndrome in metropolitan, urban, and rural Georgia. Population Health Metrics 2007, 5:5.

      [2] Jason LA, Richman JA, Rademaker AW, Jordan KM, Plioplys AV, Taylor RR, McCready W, Huan CF, Plioplys S: A community-based study of chronic fatigue syndrome. Arch Int Med 1999, 159:2129-2137.

      [3] Reyes M, Nisenbaum R, Hoaglin DC, Emmons C, Stewart G, Randall B, Stewart JA, Abbey S, Jones JF, Gantz N, Minden S, Reeves WC: Prevalence and incidence of chronic fatigue syndrome in Wichita, Kansas. Arch Intern Med 2003, 163:1530-1536.

      [4] Reeves WC, Wagner D, Nisenbaum R, Jones JF, Gurbaxani B, Solomon L, Papanicolaou DA, Unger ER, Vernon SD, Heim C: Chronic Fatigue Syndrome – A clinically empirical approach to its definition and study. BMC Medicine 2005, 3:19 (15 December 2005)

      [5] Jason LA, Richman JA: How Science Can Stigmatize: The Case of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome. Journal of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome, Vol. 14(4), 2007

      [6] Fukuda, K., Straus, S.E., Hickie, I., Sharpe, M.C., Dobbins, J.G., & Komaroff, A. (1994). The chronic fatigue syndrome: A comprehensive approach to its definition and study. Annals of Internal Medicine, 121 (12):953-959. http://www.annals.org/cgi/content/full/121/12/953

      [7] Holmes GP, Kaplan JE, Gantz NM, Komaroff AL, Schonberger LB, Straus SE, et al. Chronic fatigue syndrome: a working case definition. Ann Intern Med. 1988; 108:387-9.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jul 02, Balázs Győrffy commented:

      We have recently (2014) summarized an up-to-date version of EOC biomarkers: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18926090. Drop a mail and we will be happy to share our paper: penzvaltozsofi@gmail.com


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Nov 09, Jeffrey Beall commented:

      Much of the text in this article matches word-for-word the text in an earlier-published article. The earlier article is: Tanaka E.. Clinically important pharmacokinetic drug-drug interactions: role of cytochrome P450 enzymes. J Clin Pharm Ther. 1998 Dec;23(6):403-16.

      PMID: 10048501


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Dec 29, Keizo Takao commented:

      The URL of the gene-brain-phenotyping database has now changed to http://www.mouse-phenotype.org/ (the Mouse Phenotype Database). "ImageLD", "ImageEP", and "ImageTM", image analysis application softwares used in this article, are now freely available from http://www.mouse-phenotype.org/software.html.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Jan 23, Christopher Southan commented:

      IUPHAR-DB is now subsumed into a new resource. For the latest description see "The IUPHAR/BPS Guide to PHARMACOLOGY in 2016" https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26464438


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Dec 30, Keizo Takao commented:

      The raw data of behavioral tests, which are not described in this paper, are shown in the Mouse Phenotype Database (http://www.mouse-phenotype.org/). "ImageLD", "ImageEP", "ImageTM", and "ImageFZ", image analysis application softwares used in this article, are now freely available from http://www.mouse-phenotype.org/software.html.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Mar 18, ZHONGMING ZHAO commented:

      My lab recently moved to the University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston. The web site for this database is now available at https://bioinfo.uth.edu/ERGR/.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Aug 23, Ben Goldacre commented:

      This trial has the wrong trial registry ID associated with it on PubMed: both in the XML on PubMed, and in the originating journal article. The ID given is NCT00211459. We believe the correct ID, which we have found by hand searching, is NCT00121459.

      This comment is being posted as part of the OpenTrials.net project<sup>[1]</sup> , an open database threading together all publicly accessible documents and data on each trial, globally. In the course of creating the database, and matching documents and data sources about trials from different locations, we have identified various anomalies in datasets such as PubMed, and in published papers. Alongside documenting the prevalence of problems, we are also attempting to correct these errors and anomalies wherever possible, by feeding back to the originators. We have corrected this data in the OpenTrials.net database; we hope that this trial’s text and metadata can also be corrected at source, in PubMed and in the accompanying paper.

      Many thanks,

      Jessica Fleminger, Ben Goldacre*

      [1] Goldacre, B., Gray, J., 2016. OpenTrials: towards a collaborative open database of all available information on all clinical trials. Trials 17. doi:10.1186/s13063-016-1290-8 PMID: 27056367

      * Dr Ben Goldacre BA MA MSc MBBS MRCPsych<br> Senior Clinical Research Fellow<br> ben.goldacre@phc.ox.ac.uk<br> www.ebmDataLab.net<br> Centre for Evidence Based Medicine<br> Department of Primary Care Health Sciences<br> University of Oxford<br> Radcliffe Observatory Quarter<br> Woodstock Road<br> Oxford OX2 6GG


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 May 23, Swapnil Rane commented:

      I am skeptical about the diagnosis. Based on the histological features and immunohistochemical profile reported in this article, I would categorize the tumor as a malignant mesothelioma instead of adnexal tumor of wolffian origin.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Sep 22, Christopher Sampson commented:

      This study purports to evaluate biannual (twice a year) screening. However - though it is not entirely clear from the manuscript - the results suggest that it actually evaluates biennial (every 2 years) screening.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.