69 Matching Annotations
  1. Jun 2018
    1. sense

      Some of this comes from lack of experience with word problems, but some will be the natural product of teaching strategies. I have seen teachers focus heavily on building charts of words and which mathematical operation they relate to in an attempt to prepare for standardized tests. While a temporary fix, it doesn't do much for understanding the intent of the problem.

    2. The language that children bring with them to school involves a broad set of skills rooted in the early context of adult-child interactions.

      Building vocabulary is one of the topics that keeps coming up in these chapters. A precise vocabulary is what enables children to categorize concepts more specifically and make connections. If you don't have a word for an item, you can't connect to it.

      For example, there are words I know in Chinese that I do not have language for in English. I can connect those to concepts in Chinese, but my vocabulary is not precise enough to expand it across languages.

    3. Would the mother’s learning of formal mathematics be enhanced if it were connected to this knowledge? The literature on learning and transfer suggests that this is an important question to pursue.

      At this point, would there be a need for formal knowledge of mathematics? Isn't the goal of education to prepare a person to be a successful, contributing member of society? She seems to have accomplished that independently through experience.

    4. They found that students learned subsequent text editors more rapidly and that the number of procedural elements shared by two text editors predicted the amount of this transfer. In fact, there was large transfer across editors that were very different in

      This is what we hope to achieve in our dual-immersion program with children making quick connection in their non-native language.

    5. Research has indicated that transfer across contexts is especially difficult when a subject is taught only in a single context rather than in multiple contexts

      I've seen this in our dual-immersion program as children begin to understand the nuances of each language and make connections to concepts they learned in the other language.

    6. Motivation affects the amount of time that people are willing to devote to learning.

      Don't even dare try to get a PreK student to do something that doesn't interest them!

      As teachers, we can chose how to present various objectives. Teaching material within themes that interest students keeps them engaged, which leads to better retention of content.

    7. students need feedback about the degree to which they know when, where, and how to use the knowledge they are learning.

      What does feedback look like in the early years? Perhaps the ongoing dialogue that happens in pretend play or naturally occurs in open-ended questions?

    8. Attempts to cover too many topics too quickly may hinder learning and subsequent transfer

      Often, we are pacing our lessons too quickly to give children meaningful opportunities to engage with concepts. Working with PreK and K students, we often have to stop and remember that this is their first school experience and that time spent exploring new situations and materials provides the context to apply future knowledge to. Rushing is a disservice to their future education.

    9. Transfer is affected by the degree to which people learn with understanding rather than merely memorize sets of facts or follow a fixed set of procedures; see Boxes 3.3 and 3.4.

      What we learn with pleasure, we never forget!

    10. For example, young children often erroneously believe that they can remember information and hence fail to use effective strategies, such as rehearsal.

      Where would their understanding of what it means to be prepared come from? If teachers don't teach readiness, we can't expect students to be truly ready themselves.

      Checklists help with this: I am ready when I.... have completed a poster that includes words and photos. … have presented to a friend 3 times... etc.

      Teachers can also model what preparation means by: verbally articulating that they are ready because their materials are gathered, sufficient, functional, etc. Likewise, they can identify the reasons why they are unprepared (didn't think carefully, the copier broke, etc.)

    11. In fact, expertise can sometimes hurt teaching because many experts forget what is easy and what is difficult for students.

      I spend a lot of time emphasizing this with our Nursery-Kindergarten teachers. When they are frustrated that children aren't sitting still on the carpet for their 20 min group time, we have to stop and remember that these children have never been expected to do this before. We can stop what isn't working and try something different (limit to 10 min, work in small groups, teach in another way, etc.). It also helps to reflect on what other skills are necessary before the current skill can be learned- before a child can sit in a group, he has to have core strength to hold his body upright.

    12. One way to help students learn about conditions of applicability is to assign word problems that require students to use appropriate concepts and formulas (Lesgold, 1984, 1988; Simon, 1980). If well designed, these problems can help students learn when, where, and why to use the knowledge they are learning. Sometimes, however, students can solve sets of practice problems but fail to conditionalize their knowledge because they know which chapter the problems came from and so automatically use this information to decide which concepts and formulas are relevant. Practice problems that are organized into very structured worksheets can also cause this problem. Sometimes students who have done well on such assignments—and believe that they are learning—are unpleasantly surprised when they take tests in which problems from the entire course are randomly presented so there are no clues about where they appeared in a text (Bransford, 1979).

      This is one of the reasons we as a school are using a digital assessment tool which allows teachers to video students engaged in a real-world application, reflect on it, discuss it with the student later, or identify gaps in knowledge to readdress later.

      I was that student who could tell you which page of the textbook the problem came from and thus how to solve it!

    13. The historians excelled in the elaborateness of understandings they developed in their ability to pose alternative explanations for events and in their use of corroborating evidence. This depth of understanding was as true for the Asian specialists and the medievalists as it was for the Americanists.

      An interesting follow-up to this would be to know if these historians were able to answer factual questions in their youth and this understanding led to higher order thinking or do they have higher order thinking skills independent to the factual knowledge.

    14. Within this picture of expertise, “knowing more” means having more conceptual chunks in memory, more relations or features defining each chunk, more interrelations among the chunks, and efficient methods for retrieving related chunks and procedures for applying these informational units in problem-solving contexts

      All of this is the product of many interactions with the content, trial and error, reading, experimenting, making mistakes and reflecting; it also comes from building a rich vocabulary in the subject matter.

    15. xperts’ thinking seems to be organized around big ideas in physics, such as Newton’s second law and how it would apply, while novices tend to Page 38 Share Cite Suggested Citation:"2 How Experts Differ from Novices." National Research Council. 2000. How People Learn: Brain, Mind, Experience, and School: Expanded Edition. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/9853. × Save Cancel perceive problem solving in physics as memorizing, recalling, and manipulating equations to get answers.

      Part of this has to be a lack of breadth of vocabulary in the subject matter. Nearest I can tell from this paragraph, both experts and competent beginners were able to reach accurate conclusions, but the articulation varied.

    16. For example, electronics technicians were able to reproduce large portions of complex circuit diagrams after only a few seconds of viewing; novices could not.

      Experts have spent so many hours with content that content is chunked in complex ways which would cross all senses. I imagine an expert technician would have gained knowledge through listening, reading, experimenting, failing, reflecting, repeating... A novice may not have put in the time to apply content in all possible ways.

    17. Chess masters perceive chunks of meaningful information, which affects their memory for what they see. Chess masters are able to chunk together several chess pieces in a configuration that is governed by some strategic component of the game.

      Chunking content into categories is critical for memory, but I also think that it is important to fit content into multiple categories to allow the content to be accessed from may different angles.

    18. Transfer from school to everyday environments is the ultimate purpose of school-based learning.

      Yes!!

      And also to create an environment where they are motivated to continue learning!

    19. Opportunities to engage in problem-based learning during the first year of medical school lead to a greater ability to diagnose and understand medical problems than do opportunities to learn in typical lecture-based medical courses (Hmelo, 1995).

      I think this method of problem solving is also more engaging for students of all achievement levels.

    20. New technologies make it possible for students in schools to use tools very much like those used by professionals in workplaces (see Chapter 8).

      I think initially our society may be afraid to hand over technology to our students, however, I think that when used appropriately, they are capable of so much more than we know.

    21. Sometimes new information will seem incomprehensible to students, but this feeling of confusion can at least let them identify the existence of a problem (see, e.g., Bransford and Johnson, 1972; Dooling and Lachman, 1971).

      One of the most dreaded sayings in my classroom is "I don't get it!/I don't know!" This helps me see this in a different light - that feeling that they're feeling means that there is a problem to solve, on their end and on mine!

    22. One way to deal with lack of flexibility is to ask learners to solve a specific case and then provide them with an additional, similar case; the goal is to help them abstract general principles that lead to more flexible transfer (Gick and Holyoak, 1983); see Box 3.7. A second way to improve flexibility is to let students learn in a specific context and then help them engage in “what-if” problem solving designed to increase the flexibility of their understanding. They might be asked: “What if this part of the problem

      This seems to somewhat correlate with the concept of "experts." By giving students additional opportunities to recognize the pattern in the problem/case, they develop the problem solving mindset in looking for that specific pattern.

    23. Transfer is also affected by the context of original learning; people can learn in one context, yet fail to transfer to other contexts. For example, a group of Orange County homemakers did very well at making supermarket best-buy calculations despite doing poorly on equivalent school-like paper-and-pencil mathematics problems (Lave, 1988). Similarly, some Brazilian street children could perform mathematics when making sales in the street but were unable to answer similar problems presented in a school context (Carraher, 1986; Carraher et al, 1985).

      I feel like this really goes back to making their learning applicable...if it's something they are interested in, familiar with, or something they know how to do/have to do, they have a higher rate of succeeding.

    24. Learners of all ages are more motivated when they can see the usefulness of what they are learning and when they can use that information to do something that has an impact on others—especially their local community (McCombs, 1996; Pintrich and Schunk, 1996).

      I remember thinking this in high school, and even college. How is what I'm learning applicable in the real world? There was a community style learnings science project that we completed in one of our courses in college, in which we had to identify a problem in the community, design a curriculum around this problem, and try to solve this problem with our knowledge and findings.

    25. Social opportunities also affect motivation. Feeling that one is contributing something to others appears to be especially motivating (Schwartz et al., 1999).

      I will say that Kagan training certainly opened my eyes to this concept. Students that are afraid of failing, or don't feel the need to participate, not only won't participate, but will often also act out in other ways. Creating a classroom environment that encourages them to try, even if that means failing, is crucial at the beginning of the year.

    26. Motivation affects the amount of time that people are willing to devote to learning. Humans are motivated to develop competence and to solve problems; they have, as White (1959) put it, “competence motivation.” Although extrinsic rewards and punishments clearly affect behavior (see Chapter 1), people work hard for intrinsic reasons, as well.

      How do we improve the level of motivation? Is it something we can improve? Or does it come simply from within the student, and it requires something that grabs their attention or applies to their everyday life.

    27. For example, feedback that signals progress in memorizing facts and formulas is different from feedback that signals the state of the students’ understanding (Chi et al., 1989, 1994).

      Formative assessment is such a key tool in determining this with our students.

    28. Providing students with time to learn also includes providing enough time for them to process information. Pezdek and Miceli (1982) found that on one particular task, it took 3rd graders 15 seconds to integrate pictorial and verbal information; when given only 8 seconds, they couldn’t mentally integrate the information, probably due to short-term memory limitations.

      I wish that we had more time to give them. Often times there is a strict schedule for the curriculum throughout the course of the school year. We are able to decide what needs reteaching/review, however, I certainly wish we had more time to give them.

    29. ters require from 50,000 to 100,000 hours of practice to reach that level of expertise; they rely on a knowledge base containing some 50,000 familiar chess patterns to guide their selection of moves (Chase and Simon, 1973; Simon and Chase, 1973)

      Does this mean that expertise can be achieved with practice?

    30. e noted that the ability to remember properties of veins and arteries (e.g., that arteries are thicker than veins, more elastic, and carry blood from the heart) is not the same as understanding why they have particular properties. The ability to understand becomes important for transfer problems, such as: “Imagine trying to design an artificial artery. Would it have to be elastic? Why or why not?” Students who only memorize facts have little basis for approaching this kind of problem-solving task (Bransford and Stein, 1993; Bransford et al., 1983). The act of organizing facts about veins and arteries around more general principles such as “how structure is related to function” is consistent with the knowledge organization of experts discussed in Chapter 2.

      I love this example. For a student who had simply memorized the facts, they probably would have a difficult time applying the information in a situation where they might be asked to create veins/arteries. I know that with our phonics learning, I hope that they memorize it, but then expect them to transfer the knowledge in their decoding skills, and with other similar words - something I hope to work on in the coming year.

    31. Knowledge that is overly contextualized can reduce transfer; abstract representations of knowledge can help promote transfer.

      Interesting... I think this speaks to the fact that the learning environment needs to be authentic and applicable to what our students are learning.

    32. Educators hope that students will transfer learning from one problem to another within a course, from one year in school to another, between school and home, and from school to workplace. Assumptions about transfer accompany the belief that it is better to broadly “educate” people than simply “train” them to perform particular tasks (e.g., Broudy, 1977).

      I do often hope this, but that's absolutely right, we assume that our students will be able to transfer the information, and many times they are not. Teaching them a skill more broadly may be the solution.

    33. Learning is important because no one is born with the ability to function competently as an adult in society. It is especially important to understand the kinds of learning experiences that lead to transfer, defined as the ability to extend what has been learned in one context to new contexts (e.g., Byrnes, 1996:74).

      I think this really speaks to the nature/nurture theory... or rather it makes me wonder how much of the intelligence is inherited, how much is learned, and how the two work together to create information and knowledge.

    34. People’s mental models of what it means to be an expert can affect the degree to which they learn throughout their lifetimes. A model that assumes that experts know all the answers is very different from a model of the accomplished novice, who is proud of his or her achievements and yet also realizes that there is much more to learn.

      Love growth mindset. We did a big lesson on it at the beginning of the year, and utilized class dojo to help teach it - but it's so important to revisit it throughout the year... I think they forget, and so do we, how important mindset can be.

    35. The second cautionary note is that although the study of experts provides important information about learning and instruction, it can be misleading if applied inappropriately. For example, it would be a mistake simply to expose novices to expert models and assume that the novices will learn effectively; what they will learn depends on how much they know already. Discussions in the next chapters (3 and 4) show that effective instruction begins with the knowledge and skills that learners bring to the learning task.

      I think this is extremely important to note. We want our students to succeed, and perhaps even earn the title of experts at some point in their educational journey. However... we have to be mindful in the way we go about it.

    36. Steven began his unit on Hamlet without ever mentioning the name of the play. To help his students grasp the initial outline of the themes and issues of the play, he asked them to imagine that their parents had recently divorced and that their mothers had taken up with a new man. This new man had replaced their father at work, and “there’s some talk that he had something to do with the ousting of your dad” (Grossman, 1990:24). Steven then asked students to think about the circumstances that might drive them so mad that they would contemplate murdering another human being. Only then, after students had contemplated these issues and done some writing on them, did Steven introduce the play they would be reading.

      I can certainly appreciate taking something unfamiliar to them, and making it so that it's applicable.

    37. For example, contrast two types of Japanese sushi experts (Hatano and Inagaki, 1986): one excels at following a fixed recipe; the other has “adaptive expertise” and is able to prepare sushi quite creatively. These appear to be examples of two very different types of expertise, one that is relatively routinized and one that is flexible and more adaptable to external demands: experts have been characterized as being “merely skilled” versus “highly competent” or more colorfully as “artisans” versus “virtuosos” (Miller, 1978). These differences apparently exist across a wide range of jobs.

      I don't think there's necessarily anything wrong with either approach, however, I love the creativity and the confidence that come with adaptive expertise, whether it's in regards to sushi, math, science, or reading.

    38. Expert teachers know the kinds of difficulties that students are likely to face; they know how to tap into students’ existing knowledge in order to make new information meaningful; and they know how to assess their students’ progress. Expert teachers have acquired pedagogical content knowledge as well as content knowledge; see Box 2.4.

      I think this takes a good portion of the school year to diagnose (at least for me). For some of my students, I knew that math concepts would be difficult for them unless they had a visual representation or a manipulative to work with. I knew for other students it would simply take a reminder for them to slow down, reread, and double check their work. But it took at least a month or so if not more to determine which students were going to struggle where, and how.

    39. Expertise in a particular domain does not guarantee that one is good at helping others learn it.

      Could not agree with this more.

    40. People’s abilities to retrieve relevant knowledge can vary from being “effortful” to “relatively effortless” (fluent) to “automatic” (Schneider and Shiffrin, 1977). Automatic and fluent retrieval are important characteristics of expertise.

      I saw this in my students all year long, and WOW, how fascinating it is to see the difference in the ease of retrieving that information from student to student. For some of my kids I could ask any type, any level of question and the retrieval of information was effortless. For others, for example while measuring reading fluency, it took great effort in retrieving what they knew about phonics rules, to apply that to what they were seeing on the page, therefor decreasing their fluency.

    41. One way to help students learn about conditions of applicability is to assign word problems that require students to use appropriate concepts and formulas (Lesgold, 1984, 1988; Simon, 1980)

      I love the complexity that word problems offer, but I think that we need to do just as much thinking to compose these word problems as our students do to solve them, in that we need to be sure that we are asking questions that will first and foremost engage them in the strategy that we want them to utilize, or perhaps a different similar strategy that works best for them, we also need to make the word problem applicable to something that they either have some prior knowledge about, or have some interest in. For example, I noticed that a problem from our curriculum used an example that talked in detail about cassette tapes. To me, it didn't seem like a big distraction (because it's something I already knew about), but to them it was a huge distraction, because they knew nothing about it.

    42. They knew that no single document or picture could tell the story of history; hence, they thought very hard about their choices. In contrast, the students generally just looked at the pictures and made a selection without regard or qualification. For students, the process was similar to finding the correct answer on a multiple choice test.

      I feel like most students go into assessments with the mindset that it will be a multiple choice answer. How can we teach them to think beyond that, and have the mindset that the historians did in this case scenario?

    43. The historians excelled in the elaborateness of understandings they developed in their ability to pose alternative explanations for events and in their use of corroborating evidence. This depth of understanding was as true for the Asian specialists and the medievalists as it was for the Americanists.

      I think this really comes down to the type of assessment. It sounds like the type of assessment here assessed surface level facts, definitions, etc., which was perhaps easier for the students to retain, or maybe it's what they were taught. However, when the assessment style changed, in asking the how, why, and more in depth questions, the historians excelled.

    44. There are 26 sheep and 10 goats on a ship. How old is the captain? Most adults have enough expertise to realize that this problem is unsolvable, but many school children didn’t realize this at all. More than three-quarters of the children in one study attempted to provide a numerical answer to the problems. They asked themselves whether to add, subtract, multiply, or divide, rather than whether the problem made sense. As one fifth-grade child explained, after giving the answer of 36: “Well, you need to add or subtract or multiply in problems like this, and this one seemed to work best if I add” (Bransford and Stein, 1993:196).

      Prior to teaching in first grade, I was a math interventionist. This was one of my biggest ongoing struggles with the students that I worked with. They know how to add, subtract, multiply and divide. The difficult part was getting them to understand why we use those functions in certain situations, and how they can help us find our answer. What I've seen a lot in our math curriculum in first grade (Engage NY) is that they emphasize speed and quickness, not in the sense of rushing through their work, but forcing them to ask themselves what strategy is going to get me there the quickest? For example, instead of adding 2 + 2 + 2 + 2, we could multiply 2 x 4 to find our answer more quickly. I think part of that mindset is necessary when looking at word problems and determining what function/strategy to use in order to solve it.

    45. Pause times

      This reminds me of "think time," a crucial tool in the classroom.

    46. In representing a schema for an incline plane, the novice’s schema contains primarily surface features of the incline plane. In contrast, the expert’s schema immediately connects the notion of an incline plane with the laws of physics and the conditions under which laws are applicable.

      My understanding of the difference between a novice and an expert seems to be in this situation the depth of knowledge that they are able to apply.

    47. Differences in how physics experts and novices approach problems can also be seen when they are asked to sort problems, written on index cards, according to the approach that could be used to solve them (Chi et al., 1981).

      We were asked to do something like this in a professional development math lab. During this time we observe a teacher in the classroom, and for the remainder of the day we discuss concepts, understandings, etc. In one particular exercise we were asked to organize a set of problems that were solved by first grade students, and we had to organize them by the way in which they were solve (each had a title).

    48. Experts usually mentioned the major principle(s) or law(s) that were applicable to the problem, together with a rationale for why those laws applied to the problem and how one could apply them (Chi et al., 1981). In contrast, competent beginners rarely referred to major principles and laws in physics; instead, they typically described which equations they would use and how those equations would be manipulated (Larkin, 1981, 1983).

      I find this very interesting, and I also agree with it. In my first grade classroom we teach in math about the commutative property and the associative property. Very rarely are the kids able to verbalize which property justifies a solution or solves a problem, but they know exactly how it works.

    49. Expert 6: On the left monitor, the students’ note taking indicates that they have seen sheets like this and have had presentations like this before; it’s fairly efficient at this point because they’re used to the format they are using. Expert 7: I don’t understand why the students can’t be finding out this information on their own rather than listening to someone tell them because if you watch the faces of most of them, they start out for about the first 2 or 3 minutes sort of paying attention to what’s going on and then just drift off. Expert 2: …I haven’t heard a bell, but the students are already at their desks and seem to be doing purposeful activity, and this is about the time that I decide they must be an accelerated group because they came into the room and started something rather than just sitting down and socializing.

      Interesting... it seems that the experts while they do observe a great deal more, also don't understand why these students don't just "get it"

    50. the expert teachers had very different understandings of the events they were watching than did the novice teachers; see examples in Box 2.2.

      I would be interested to know what these events were and how they were interpreted by the experts versus the novices. I also wonder can someone become an expert over time?

    51. The superior recall ability of experts, illustrated in the example in the box, has been explained in terms of how they “chunk” various elements of a configuration that are related by an underlying function or strategy. Since

      Chunking is SUCH a huge learning tool. We use it as a decoding strategy in reading, a tool in comprehension with nonfiction texts, etc. It's not surprising that chunking correlates with expertise.

    52. DeGroot concluded that the knowledge acquired over tens of thousands of hours of chess playing enabled chess masters to out-play their opponents. Specifically, masters were more likely to recognize meaningful chess configurations and realize the strategic implications of these situations; this recognition allowed them to consider sets of possible moves that were superior to others. The meaningful patterns seemed readily apparent to the masters, leading deGroot (1965:33–34) to note:

      This makes me wonder, does expertise come with experience, or does it also require the ability to recognize patters, the ability to apply, etc.

    53. Though experts know their disciplines thoroughly, this does not guarantee that they are able to teach others.

      I've always found this very interesting. What allows an expert to teach others then? Or do the two not coincide?

    54. 3. Formative assessments—ongoing assessments designed to make students’ thinking visible to both teachers and students—are essential. They permit the teacher to grasp the students’ preconcep tions, understand where the students are in the “developmental cor ridor” from informal to formal thinking, and design instruction accordingly. In the assessment-centered classroom environment, for mative assessments help both teachers and students monitor progress.

      I love finding new ways to incorporate technology into formative assessment, a lot of the time it allows us to track their progress and understanding!

    55. FIGURE 1.1 With knowledge of how people learn, teachers can choose more purposefully among techniques to accomplish specific goals

      Love this graphic organizer - easier to see how a lot of these learning styles, tools, etc, relate/

    56. Assessment for purposes of accountability (e.g., statewide assessments) must test deep understanding rather than surface knowledge. Assessment tools are often the standard by which teachers are held accountable. A teacher is put in a bind if she or he is asked to teach for deep conceptual understanding, but in doing so produces students who perform more poorly on standardized tests. Unless new assessment tools are aligned with new approaches to teaching, the latter are unlikely to muster support among the schools and their constituent parents. This goal is as important as it is difficult to achieve. The format of standardized tests can encourage measurement of factual knowledge rather than conceptual understanding, but it also facilitates objective scoring. Measuring depth of understanding can pose challenges for objectivity. Much work needs to be done to minimize the trade-off between assessing depth and assessing objectively.

      ESA (Third Grade Reading Law) has pushed standardized testing in schools across the state. I wonder about the depth of the assessments.

    57. The roles for assessment must be expanded beyond the traditional concept of testing. The use of frequent formative assessment helps make students’ thinking visible to themselves, their peers, and their teacher. This provides feedback that can guide modification and refinement in thinking. Given the goal of learning with understanding, assessments must tap understanding rather than merely the ability to repeat facts or perform isolated skills.

      I love the idea of project based learning, and I think that it could be used as an assessment tool as well. It goes beyond the definitions and forces application of their knowledge, as well as focusing on the how and why of the concept at hand. I would be interested in seeing how the two (assessment/project based learning) intertwine.

    58. If their initial understanding is not engaged, they may fail to grasp the new concepts and information that are Page 15 Share Cite Suggested Citation:"1 Learning: From Speculation to Science." National Research Council. 2000. How People Learn: Brain, Mind, Experience, and School: Expanded Edition. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/9853. × Save Cancel taught, or they may learn them for purposes of a test but revert to their preconceptions outside the classroom.

      How do we go about engaging their initial understanding? How do we determine what that understanding is?

    59. These hypothetical teachers—A, B, and C—are abstract models that of course fit real teachers only partly, and more on some days than others. Nevertheless, they provide important glimpses of connections between goals for learning and teaching practices that can affect students’ abilities to accomplish these goals.

      I think this is extremely accurate. It's impossible to be one kind of teacher all the time. It's ok to be teacher A, or teacher B some of the time, as long as you make an effort to be teacher C most of the time.

    60. Imagine three teachers whose practices affect whether students learn to take control of their own learning (Scardamalia and Bereiter, 1991). Teacher A’s goal is to get the students to produce work; this is accomplished by supervising and overseeing the quantity and quality of the work done by the students. The focus is on activities, which could be anything from old-style workbook activities to the trendiest of space-age projects. Teacher B assumes responsibility for what the students are learning as they carry out their activities. Teacher C does this as well, but with the added objective of continually turning more of the learning process over to the students. Walking into a classroom, you cannot immediately tell these three kinds of teachers apart. One of the things you might see is the students working in groups to produce videos or multimedia presentations. The teacher is likely to be Page 13 Share Cite Suggested Citation:"1 Learning: From Speculation to Science." National Research Council. 2000. How People Learn: Brain, Mind, Experience, and School: Expanded Edition. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi: 10.17226/9853. × Save Cancel found going from group to group, checking how things are going and responding to requests.

      Sounds exactly like Kagan! Teacher A: independent work Teacher B: group work Teacher C: cooperative learning

    61. New developments in the science of learning also emphasize the importance of helping people take control of their own learning

      Teachers as facilitators of learning - students should be doing more of the talking than the teacher.

    62. addressed in order for them to change their beliefs (e.g., Confrey, 1990; Mestre, 1994; Minstrell, 1989; Redish, 1996).

      Ah, there it is, addressing misconceptions. The tricky part I think is figuring out what these are!

    63. Pre-Existing Knowledge

      Reading this heading sends me back to the conversations we have had in our college classes as well as with our administrator, what do they already know, how do we address misconceptions, how can we build on this knowledge, etc.

    64. Learning research suggests that there are new ways to introduce students to traditional subjects, such as mathematics, science, history and literature, and that these new approaches make it possible for the majority of individuals to develop a deep understanding of important subject matter.

      Last year and this year I went through NGSX training as well as Phenomenal Science (Central Michigan University) training, which focuses on the idea of presenting the phenomena first to engage students, and allow them to create a hypothesis as to why something happened.

    65. Developmental researchers have shown that young children understand a great deal about basic principles of biology and physical causality, about number, narrative, and personal intent, and that these capabilities make it possible to create innovative curricula that introduce important concepts for advanced reasoning at early ages.

      Could not agree more with this statement/finding. Often I think we as a society assume a certain naivety about young children, but their critical reasoning skills and explanations that take place during activities such as "number talks" as well as during cooperative learning activities in which they are required to coach others with the "how and why" instead of just giving their team mates the answer.

    66. Thirty years ago, educators paid little attention to the work of cognitive scientists, and researchers in the nascent field of cognitive science worked far removed from classrooms. Today, cognitive researchers are spending more time working with teachers, testing and refining their theories in real classrooms where they can see how different settings and classroom interactions influence applications of their theories.

      Collaboration not only in the school proves to be extremely important and effective, but also across disciplines.

    67. Equally important, the growth of interdisciplinary inquiries and new kinds of scientific collaborations have begun to make the path from basic research to educational practice somewhat more visible, if not yet easy to travel.

      This got me thinking along the lines of NGSX science standards, and the cross cutting concepts that allow us to apply some of these science standards across the curriculum.

    68. As we illustrate, a new theory of learning is coming into focus that leads to very different approaches to the design of curriculum, teaching, and assessment than those often found in schools today.

      I could not agree more. Education continues to change each an every day to adapt to the needs of the students. Recently our school went through Kagan training to explore the concept of cooperative learning. I can say that it is unlike the way we have taught in the past, but it creates results, relationships, and classroom environments unlike those that we've seen before.

    69. Today, the world is in the midst of an extraordinary outpouring of scientific work on the mind and brain, on the processes of thinking and learning, on the neural processes that occur during thought and learning, and on the development of competence.

      I would be curious to see what kind of connections are physically made during this process, or rather in cases of learning disabilities what connections fail to be made. In a general education classroom it's always rewarding and fascinating to see those "light bulb" moments, and I wonder what that physically looks like in their brains when those light bulb moments do occur, and what it looks like when they don't.