10,000 Matching Annotations
  1. Last 7 days
    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Multiple waves of follicles have been proven to exist in multiple species, and different waves of follicles contribute differently to oogenesis and fertility. This work characterizes the wave 1 follicles in mouse comprehensively and compares different waves of follicles regarding their cellular and molecular features. Elegant mouse genetics methods are applied to provide lineage tracing of the wave 1 folliculogenesis, together with sophisticated microscopic imaging and analyses. Single-cell RNA-seq is further applied to profile the molecular features of cells in mouse ovaries from week 2 until week 6. While extensive details about the wave 1 follicles, especially the atresia process, are provided, the authors also identified another group of follicles located in the medullary-cortical boundary, which could also be labeled by the FoxL2-mediated lineage tracing method. The "boundary" or "wave 1.5" follicles are proposed by the authors to be the earliest wave 2 follicles, which contribute to the early fertility of puberty mice, instead of the wave 1 follicles, which undergo atresia with very few oocytes generated. The wave 1 follicle atresia, which degrades most oocytes, on the other hand, expands the number of theca cells and generates the interstitial gland cells in the medulla, where the wave 1 follicles are located. These gland cells likely contribute to the generation of androgen and estrogen, which shape oogenesis and animal development. By comparing scRNA-seq data from cells collected from week 2 until week 6 ovaries, the author profiled the changes in numbers of different cells and identified key genes that differ between wave 1 and wave 2 follicles, which could potentially be another driver of different waves of folliculogenesis. In summary, the authors provide a high amount of new results with good quality that illustrate the molecular and cellular features of different waves of mouse follicles, which could be further reused by other researchers in related fields. The findings related to the boundary follicles could potentially bring many new findings related to oogenesis.

      This paper is overall well-written with solid and intriguing conclusions that are well supported. The reviewer only has some minor comments for the authors' consideration that could potentially help with the readability of the paper.

      (1) The authors identify the wave 1.5 follicles at the medullary-cortex boundary, which begin to develop shortly after 2 weeks. Since the authors already collected scRNA-seq data from week 2 until week 6, could any special gene expression patterns be identified that make wave 1.5 follicle cells different from wave 1 and wave 2?

      (2) Are Figures 1C and 1E Z projections from multiple IF slices? If so, please provide representative IF slice(s) from Figures 1C and 1E and clearly label wave 1 and wave 2 follicles to better illustrate how the wave 1 follicles are clarified and quantified.

      (3) For Figure 3D, please also provide an image showing the whole ovary section, like in Figures 3A and 3C, to better illustrate the localization and abundance of different cells.

      (4) In Figure 4H, expressions of HSD3B1 and PLIN1 seem to be detected in almost all medulla cells. Does this mean all medulla cells gain gland cell features? Or there is only a subset of the medulla cells that are actively expressing these 2 proteins. Please provide image(s) with higher magnification to show more clearly how the expression of these 2 proteins differs among different cells.

      (5) Figure 5: The authors discussed cell number changes for different types of cells from week 2 to week 6. A table, or some plots, visualizing numbers of different cell types, instead of just providing original clusters in Dataset S6, at different time points, would make the changes easier to observe.

      (6) Figure S7: It would be more helpful to directly show the number of wave 1 follicles.

      (7) Did the fluorescence cryosection staining (Line 587 - 595) use the same buffers as in the whole-mount staining (Line 575 - 586)? Please clarify.

      (8) In Line 618, what tissue samples were collected? Please point out clearly.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study explores an important question concerning the developmental trajectory of wave 1 ovarian follicles, leveraging valuable tools such as lineage tracing and single-cell RNA sequencing. These approaches position the authors well to dissect early follicle dynamics. The study would benefit from more in-depth analysis, including quantification using the lineage-traced ovaries, and comparison of wave 1 and 2 follicular cells per stage within the single cell dataset.

      Strengths:

      This study aims to address an important question regarding the developmental trajectories of wave 1 ovarian follicles and how they differ from wave 2 follicles that contribute to long-term fertility. This is an important topic, as many studies on ovarian follicle development rely on samples collected at perinatal timepoints in the mouse, which primarily represent wave 1 follicles, to infer later fertility. The research group has the tools and expertise necessary to tackle these questions.

      Weaknesses:

      Wave 1 follicles are quantified based on the criteria of oocytes larger than 20 µm located within the medullary region, using whole-mount staining. However, the boundary between the medulla and cortex appears somewhat arbitrary. Quantification using FOXL2-lineage-traced ovaries provides a more reliable method for identifying wave 1 follicles. As the developmental trajectory of wave 1 follicles has been well described in Zhang et al. 2013, it would be valuable to provide a more detailed quantification of both labeled and unlabeled follicles by specific follicle stages. In fact, in Zhang et al. 2013, the authors demonstrated that lineage-labeled primordial follicles can be found at the cortex-medulla boundary, suggesting that the observation of labeled "border follicles" is not unexpected. Quantification by follicle stage would provide greater insight into the timing and development of these follicles.

      Similarly, the analysis of wave 1 follicle loss should be performed on lineage-traced ovaries using cell death markers to demonstrate the loss of oocytes and granulosa cells, while confirming the preservation of theca and interstitial cells. In particular, granulosa cell loss should be assessed directly with cell death markers in lineage-traced ovaries, rather than from the loss of tamoxifen-labeled cells, as labeling efficiency varies between follicles (Figure 2G).

      Single-cell RNA sequencing presents a valuable dataset capturing the development of first-wave follicles. The use of a 40µm cell strainer during cell collection for the 10x platform may explain the exclusion of larger oocytes. However, it is still surprising that no oocytes were captured at all. The central question, how wave 1 follicular cells differ from wave 2 cells, should be investigated in more depth, with results validated on FOXL2-lineage-traced ovaries (i.e., Wnt4 staining in wave 1 antral follicles versus wave 2 using lineage-traced ovaries). This analysis should span all stages of follicle development. It also appears to be a missed opportunity that the single-cell sequencing analysis was not performed on lineage-traced ovaries, which would have enabled more definitive identification of wave 1-derived cells.

      Finally, this study does not directly assess fertility outcomes and should therefore refrain from drawing conclusions about the fertility potential of wave 1 follicles.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors have conducted the largest to date Mendelian Randomization (MR) analysis of the association between genetically predicted measures of adiposity and risk of head and neck cancer (HNC) overall and by subsites within HNC. MR uses genetic predictors of an exposure, such as gene variants associated with high BMI or tobacco use, rather than data from individual physical exams or questionnaires and if it can be done in its idealized state, there should be no problems with confounding. Traditional epidemiologic studies have reported a variety of associations between BMI (and a few other measures of adiposity) and risk of HNC that typically differs by the smoking status of the subjects. Those findings are controversial given the complex relationship between tobacco and both BMI and HNC risk. Tobacco smokers are often thinner than no-smokers so this could create an artificial ('confounded') association that may not be fully adjusted away in risk models. The findings of a BMI-HNC association are often attributed to residual confounding and this seems ripe for an MR approach if suitable genetic instrumental variables can be created. Here the authors built a variety of genetic instrumental variables for BMI and other measures of adiposity as well as two instrumental variables for smoking habits and then tested their hypotheses in a large case-controls set of HNC and controls with genetic data.

      The authors found that the genetic model for BMI was associated with HNC risk in simple models, but this association disappeared when using models that better accounted for pleiotropy, the condition when genetic variants are associated with more than one trait such as both BMI and tobacco use. When they used both adiposity and tobacco use genetic instruments in a single model, there was a strong association with genetically predicted tobacco use (as is expected) but there was no remaining association with genetic predictors of adiposity. They conclude that high BMI/adiposity is not a risk factor for HNC.

      Strengths:

      The primary strength was the expansive use of a variety of different genetic instruments for BMI/adiposity/body size along with employing a variety of MR model types, several of which are known to be less sensitive to pleiotropy. They also used the largest case-control sample size to date.

      Weaknesses:

      The lack of pleiotropy is an unconfirmable assumption of MR and the addition of those models is therefore quite important as this is a primary weakness of the MR approach. Given that concern, I read the sensitivity analyses using pleiotropy-robust models as the main result and in that case, they are more limited in their ability to test their hypothesis as these models do not show a robust BMI instrumental variable association.

      Comments on the revised manuscript:

      After the first round of review, the authors have improved the manuscript by (1) adding the requested power calculations and adding text to help the reader integrate that additional information; (2) adding the main effects for the tobacco instruments; (3) updating the comparison of their results to the prior literature; (4) and some other edits to the text. They have declined to include the smoking stratified estimates and provide a rationale for this decision that references the potential for collider bias. While true that yet another bias might be introduced, that gets added to the list and the careful reader would know that. Many important questions in cancer etiology can only be addressed via observational approaches and each observational approach has the potential for a long list of biases. The best inference comes from integrating the totality of the data and realizing that most conclusions are subject to updating as we conduct more work and learn more.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors use high-throughput gene editing technology in larval zebrafish to address whether microexons play important roles in the development and functional output of larval circuits. They find that individual microexon deletions rarely impact behavior, brain morphology, or activity, and raise the possibility that behavioral dysregulation occurs only with more global loss of microexon splicing regulation. Other possibilities exist: perhaps microexon splicing is more critical for later stages of brain development, perhaps microexon splicing is more critical in mammals, or perhaps the behavioral phenotypes observed when microexon splicing is lost are associated with loss of splicing in only a few genes.

      Strengths:

      - The authors provide a qualitative analysis of microexon inclusion during early zebrafish development

      - The authors provide comprehensive phenotyping of microexon mutants, addressing the role of individual microexons in the regulation of brain morphology, activity, and behavior.

    2. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This paper sought to understand how microexons influence early brain function. By selectively deleting a large number of conserved microexons and then phenotyping the mutants with a behavior and brain activity assays, the authors find that most microexons have minimal effects on the global brain activity and broad behaviors of the larval fish-- although a few do have phenotypes.

      Strengths:

      The work takes full advantage of the scale that is afforded in zebrafish, generating a large mutant collection that is missing microexons and systematically phenotyping them with high throughput behaviour and brain activity assays. The work lays an important foundation for future studies that seek to uncover the likely subtle roles that single microexons will play in shaping development and behavior.

      Weaknesses:

      Although the manuscript includes evidence for many mutants that microexon deletion has minimal effect on full length transcript levels, some of the microexon loss does alter transcript levels. Since the mutations usually yielded no phenotype, these effects on full-length transcripts are unlikely to be a major confound. For mircoexon mutants displaying phenotypes, future work will have to tease apart whether secondary effects on the transcripts are contributing to the phenotype.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      This study aims to elucidate the mechanisms by which stress-induced α2A-adrenergic receptor (α2A-AR) internalization leads to cytosolic noradrenaline (NA) accumulation and subsequent neuronal dysfunction in the locus coeruleus (LC). While the manuscript presents an interesting but ambitious model involving calcium dynamics, GIRK channel rundown, and autocrine NA signaling, several key limitations undermine the strength of the conclusions.

      First, the revision does not include new experiments requested by reviewers to validate core aspects of the mechanism. Specifically, there is no direct measurement of cytosolic NA levels or MAO-A enzymatic activity to support the link between receptor internalization and neurochemical changes. The authors argue that such measurements are either not feasible or beyond the scope of the study, leaving a significant gap in the mechanistic chain of evidence.

      Second, the behavioral analysis remains insufficient to support claims of cognitive impairment. The use of a single working memory test following an anxiety test is inadequate to verify memory dysfunction behaviors. Additional cognitive assays, such as the Morris Water Maze or Novel Object Recognition, are recommended but not performed.

      Third, concerns regarding the lack of rigor in differential MAO-A expression in fluorescence imaging were not addressed experimentally. Instead of clarifying the issue, the authors moved the figure to supplementary data without providing further evidence (e.g., an enzymatic assay or quantitative reanalysis of Western blot, or re-staining of IF for MAO-A) to support their interpretation.

      Fourth, concerns regarding TH staining remain unresolved. In Figure S7, the α2A-AR signal appears to resemble TH staining, and vice versa, raising the possibility of labeling errors. It is recommended that the authors re-examine this issue by either double-checking the raw data or repeating the immunostaining to validate the staining.

      Overall, the manuscript offers a potentially interesting framework but falls short in providing the experimental rigor necessary to establish causality. The reliance on indirect reasoning and reorganizing existing data, rather than generating new evidence, limits the overall impact and interpretability of the study.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript investigates the mechanism by which chronic stress induces degeneration of locus coeruleus (LC) neurons. The authors demonstrate that chronic stress leads to the internalization of α2A-adrenergic receptors (α2A-ARs) on LC neurons, causing increased cytosolic noradrenaline (NA) accumulation and subsequent production of the neurotoxic metabolite DOPEGAL via monoamine oxidase A (MAO-A). The study suggests a mechanistic link between stress-induced α2A-AR internalization, disrupted autoinhibition, elevated NA metabolism, activation of asparagine endopeptidase (AEP), and Tau pathology relevant to Alzheimer's disease (AD). The conclusions of this paper are well-supported mainly by the data, but some aspects of image acquisition require further examination.

      Strengths:

      This study clearly demonstrates the effects of chronic stimulation on the excitability of LC neurons using electrophysiological techniques. It also elucidates the role of α2-adrenergic receptor (α2-AR) internalization and the associated upstream and downstream signaling pathways of GIRK-1, using a range of pharmacological agents, highlighting the innovative nature of the work. Additionally, the study identifies the involvement of the MAO-A-DOPEGAL-AEP pathway in this process. The topic is timely, the proposed mechanistic pathway is compelling, and the findings have translational relevance, particularly in relation to therapeutic strategies targeting α2A-AR internalization in neurodegenerative diseases.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The manuscript reports that chronic stress for 5 days increases MAO-A levels in LC neurons, leading to the production of DOPEGAL, activation of AEP, and subsequent tau cleavage into the tau N368 fragment, ultimately contributing to neuronal damage. However, the authors used wild-type C57BL/6 mice, and previous literature has indicated that AEP-mediated tau cleavage in wild-type mice is minimal and generally insufficient to cause significant behavioral alterations. Please clarify and discuss this apparent discrepancy.

      (2) It is recommended that the authors include additional experiments to examine the effects of different durations and intensities of stress on MAO-A expression and AEP activity. This would strengthen the understanding of stress-induced biochemical changes and their thresholds.

      (3) Please clarify the rationale for the inconsistent stress durations used across Figures 3, 4, and 5. In some cases, a 3-day stress protocol is used, while in others, a 5-day protocol is applied. This discrepancy should be addressed to ensure clarity and experimental consistency.

      (4) The abbreviation "vMAT2" is incorrectly formatted. It should be "VMAT2," and the full name (vesicular monoamine transporter 2) should be provided at first mention.

      Comments on revisions:

      The authors have addressed all of the reviewers' comments.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors present a technically impressive dataset showing that repeated excitation or restraint stress internalises somatodendritic α2A adrenergic autoreceptors (α2A ARs) in locus coeruleus (LC) neurons. Loss of these receptors weakens GIRK-dependent autoinhibition, raises neuronal excitability, and is accompanied by higher MAO A, DOPEGAL, AEP, and tau N368 levels. The work combines rigorous whole-cell electrophysiology with barbadin-based trafficking assays, qPCR, Western blotting, and immunohistochemistry. The final schematic is appealing and, in principle, could explain early LC hyperactivity followed by degeneration in ageing and Alzheimer's disease.

      Strengths:

      - Multi-level approach - The study integrates electrophysiology, pharmacology, mRNA quantification, and protein-level analysis.

      -Use of barbadin to block β-arrestin/AP-2-dependent internalisation is both technically precise and mechanistically informative

      -Well-executed electrophysiology

      -translation relevance

      -converges to a model that peers discussed (scientists can only discuss models - not data!)

      Weaknesses:

      Nevertheless, the manuscript currently reads as a sequence of discrete experiments rather than a single causal chain.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors developed a sequence-based method to predict drug-interacting residues in IDP, based on their recent work, to predict the transverse relaxation rates (R2) of IDP trained on 45 IDP sequences and their corresponding R2 values. The discovery is that the IDPs interact with drugs mostly using aromatic residues that are easy to understand, as most drugs contain aromatic rings. They validated the method using several case studies, and the predictions are in accordance with chemical shift perturbations and MD simulations. The location of the predicted residues serves as a starting point for ligand optimization.

      Strengths:

      This work provides the first sequence-based prediction method to identify potential drug-interacting residues in IDP. The validity of the method is supported by case studies. It is easy to use, and no time-consuming MD simulations and NMR studies are needed.

      Weaknesses:

      The method does not depend on the information of binding compounds, which may give general features of IDP-drug binding. However, due to the size and chemical structures of the compounds (for example, how many aromatic rings), the number of interacting residues varies, which is not considered in this work. Lacking specific information may restrict its application in compound optimization, aiming to derive specific and potent binding compounds.

      Comments on revised version:

      I'm satisfied with the authors' response and the public review does not need further changes.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this work, the authors introduce DIRseq, a fast, sequence-based method that predicts drug-interacting residues (DIRs) in IDPs without requiring structural or drug information. DIRseq builds on the authors' prior work looking at NMR relaxation rates, and presumes that those residues that show enhanced R2 values are the residues that will interact with drugs, allowing these residues to be nominated from the sequence directly. By making small modifications to their prior tool, DIRseq enables the prediction of residues seen to interact with small molecules in vivo.

      Strengths:

      The preprint is well written and easy to follow.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors aim to explore the effects of the electrogenic sodium-potassium pump (Na+/K+-ATPase) on the computational properties of highly active spiking neurons, using the weakly-electric fish electrocyte as a model system. Their work highlights how the pump's electrogenicity, while essential for maintaining ionic gradients, introduces challenges in neuronal firing stability and signal processing, especially in cells that fire at high rates. The study identifies compensatory mechanisms that cells might use to counteract these effects, and speculates on the role of voltage dependence in the pump's behavior, suggesting that Na+/K+-ATPase could be a factor in neuronal dysfunctions and diseases

      Strengths:

      (1) The study explores a less-examined aspect of neural dynamics-the effects of Na+/K+-ATPase electrogenicity. It offers a new perspective by highlighting the pump's role not only in ion homeostasis but also in its potential influence on neural computation.

      (2) The mathematical modeling used is a significant strength, providing a clear and controlled framework to explore the effects of the Na+/K+-ATPase on spiking cells. This approach allows for the systematic testing of different conditions and behaviors that might be difficult to observe directly in biological experiments.

      (3) The study several interesting compensatory mechanisms, such as sodium leak channels and extracellular potassium buffering, which provide useful theoretical frameworks for understanding how neurons maintain firing rate control despite the pump's effects.

      Comments on revisions:proposes

      The revised manuscript is notably improved.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The paper by Weerdmeester, Schleimer, and Schreiber uses computational models to present the biological constraints under which electrocytes - specialized, highly active cells that facilitate electro-sensing in weakly electric fish-may operate. The authors suggest potential solutions that these cells could employ to circumvent these constraints.

      Electrocytes are highly active or spiking (greater than 300Hz) for sustained periods (for minutes to hours), and such activity is possible due to an influx of sodium and efflux of potassium ions into these cells after each spike. The resulting ion imbalance must be restored, which in electrocytes, as with many other biological cells, is facilitated by the Na-K pumps at the expense of biological energy, i.e., ATP molecules. For each ATP molecule the pump uses, three positively charged sodium ions from the intracellular space are exchanged for two positively charged potassium ions from the extracellular space. This creates a net efflux of positive ions into the extracellular space, resulting in hyperpolarized potentials for the cell over time. For most cells, this does not pose an issue, as their firing rate is much slower, and other compensatory mechanisms and pumps can effectively restore the ion imbalances. However, in the electrocytes of weakly electric fish, which spike at exceptionally high rates, the net efflux of positive ions presents a challenge. Additionally, these cells are involved in critical communication and survival behaviors, underscoring their essential role in reliable functioning.

      In a computational model, the authors test four increasingly complex solutions to the problem of counteracting the hyperpolarized states that occur due to continuous NaK pump action to sustain baseline activity. First, they propose a solution for a well-matched Na leak channel that operates in conjunction with the NaK pump, counteracting the hyperpolarizing states naturally. Their model shows that when such an orchestrated Na leak current is not included, quick changes in the firing rates could have unexpected side effects. Secondly, they study the implications of this cell in the context of chirps-a means of communication between individual fish. Here, an upstream pacemaking neuron entrains the electrocyte to spike, which ceases to produce a so-called chirp - a brief pause in the sustained activity of the electrocytes. In their model, the authors demonstrate that including the extracellular potassium buffer is necessary to obtain a reliable chirp signal. Thirdly, they tested another means of communication in which there was a sudden increase in the firing rate of the electrocyte, followed by a decay to the baseline. For this to occur reliably, the authors emphasize that a strong synaptic connection between the pacemaker neuron and the electrocyte is necessary. Finally, since these cells are energy-intensive, they hypothesize that electrocytes may have energy-efficient action potentials, for which their NaK pumps may be sensitive to the membrane voltages and perform course correction rapidly.

      Strengths:

      The authors extend an existing electrocyte model (Joos et al., 2018) based on the classical Hodgkin and Huxley conductance-based models of sodium and potassium currents to include the dynamics of the sodium-potassium (NaK) pump. The authors estimate the pump's properties based on reasonable assumptions related to the leak potential. Their proposed solutions are valid and may be employed by weakly electric fish. The authors explore theoretical solutions to electrosensing behavior that compound and suggest that all these solutions must be simultaneously active for the survival and behavior of the fish. This work provides a good starting point for conducting in vivo experiments to determine which of these proposed solutions the fish employ and their relative importance. The authors include testable hypotheses for their computational models.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The authors previously reported that Heliconius, one genus of the Heliconiini butterflies, evolved to be efficient foragers to feed pollen of specific plants and have massively expanded mushroom bodies. Using the same image dataset, the authors segmented the central complex and associated brain regions and found that the volume of the central complex relative to the rest of the brain is largely conserved across the Heliconiini butterflies. By performing immunostaining to label a specific subset of neurons, the authors found several potential sites of evolutionary divergence in the central complex neural circuits, including the number of GABAergic ellipsoid body ring neurons and the innervation patterns of Allatostatin A expressing neurons in the noduli. These neuroanatomical data will be helpful to guide future studies to understand the evolution of the neural circuits for vector-based navigation.

      Strengths:

      The authors used a sufficiently large scale of dataset from 307 individuals of 41 species of Heliconiini butterflies to solidify the quantitative conclusions and present new microscopy data for fine neuroanatomical comparison of the central complex.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Although the figures display a concise summary of anatomical findings, it would be difficult for non-experts to learn from this manuscript to identify the same neuronal processes in the raw confocal stacks. It would be helpful to have instructive movies to show a step-by-step guide for identification of neurons of interest, segmentations, and 3D visualizations (rotation) for several examples, including ER neurons (to supplement texts in line 347-353) and Allatostatin A neurons.

      (2) Related to (1), it was difficult for me to assess if the data in Figure 7 support the author's conclusions that ER neuron number increased in Heliconius Melpomene. By my understanding, the resolution of this dataset isn't high enough to trace individual axons and therefore authors do not rule out that the portion of "ER ring neurons" in Heliconius may not innervate the ER, as stated in Line 635 "Importantly, we also found that some ER neurons bypass the ellipsoid body and give rise to dense branches within distinct layers in the fan-shaped body (ER-FB)". If they don't innervate the ellipsoid body, why are they named as "ER neurons"?

      (3) Discussions around the lines 577-584 require the assumption that each ellipsoid body (EB) ring neuron typically arborises in a single microglomerulus to form a largely one-to-one connection with TuBu neurons within the bulb (BU), and therefore, the number of BU microglomeruli should provide an estimation of the number of ER neurons. Explain this key assumption or provide an alternative explanation.

      (4) The details of antibody information are missing in the Key resource table. Instead of citing papers, list the catalogue numbers and identifier for commercially available antibodies, and describe the antigen, and whether they are monoclonal or polyclonal. Are antigens conserved across species?

      (5) I did not understand why authors assume that foraging to feed on pollens is a more difficult cognitive task than foraging to feed on nectar. Would it be possible that they are equally demanding tasks, but pollen feeding allows Heliconius to pass more proteins and nucleic acids to their offspring and therefore they can develop larger mushroom bodies?

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, Farnsworth et al. ask whether the previously established expansion of mushroom bodies in the pollen foraging Heliconius genus of Heliconiini butterflies co-evolved with adaptations in the central complex. Heliconius trap line foraging strategies to acquire pollen as a novel resource require advanced spatial memory mediated by larger mushroom bodies, but the authors show that related navigation circuits in the central complex are highly conserved across the Heliconiini tribe, with a few interesting exceptions. Using general immunohistochemical stains and 3D reconstruction, the authors compared volumes of central complex regions, and unlike the mushroom bodies, there was no evidence of expansion associated with pollen feeding. However, a second dataset of neuromodulator and neuropeptide antibody labeling reveals more subtle differences between pollen and non-pollen foragers and highlights sub-circuits that may mediate species-specific differences in behavior. Specifically, the authors found an expansion of GABAergic ER neurons projecting to the fan-shaped body in Heliconius, which may enhance their ability to path-integrate. They also found differences in Allatostatin A immunoreactivity, particularly increased expression in the noduli associated with pollen feeding. These differences warrant closer examination in future studies to determine their functional implication on navigation and foraging behaviors.

      Strengths:

      The authors leveraged a large morphological data set from the Heliconiini to achieve excellent phylogenetic coverage across the tribe with 41 species represented. Their high-quality histology resolves anatomical details to the level of specific, identifiable tracts and cell body clusters. They revealed differences at a circuit level, which would not be obvious from a volumetric comparison. The discussion of these adaptations in the context of central complex models is useful for generating new hypotheses for future studies on the function of ER-FB neurons and the role of Allatostatin A modulation in navigation.

      The conclusions drawn in this paper are measured and supported by rigorous statistics and evidence from micrographs.

      Weaknesses:

      The majority of results in this study do not reveal adaptations in the central complex associated with pollen foraging. However, reporting conserved traits is useful and illustrates where developmental or functional constraints may be acting. The implied hypothesis in the introduction is that expansion of mushroom bodies in Heliconius co-evolved with central complex adaptations, so it may be helpful to set up the alternate hypotheses in the beginning.

      In the main text, the authors describe differences in GABAergic neurons "across several species" but only one Heliconius and one outgroup species seem to be represented in the figures. ER numbers in Figure 7H are only compared for these two species. If this data is available for other species, it would strengthen the paper to add them to the analysis, since this was one of the most intriguing findings in the study. I would want to know if the increased ER number is a trend in Heliconius or specific to H. melpomene.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, the authors theoretically address the topic of interface resistance between a phase-separated condensate and the surrounding dilute phase. In a nutshell, "interface resistance" occurs if material in the dilute phase can only slowly pass through the interface region to enter the dense phase. There is some evidence from FRAP experiments that such a resistance may exist, and if it does, it could be biologically relevant insofar as the movement of material between dense and dilute phases can be rate-limiting for biological processes, including coarsening. The current study theoretically addresses interface resistance at two levels of description: first, the authors present a simple way of formulating interface resistance for a sharp interface model. Second, they derive a formula for interface resistance for a finite-width interface and present two scenarios where the interface resistance might be substantial.

      Strengths:

      The topic is of broad relevance to the important field of intracellular phase separation, and the work is overall credible.

      Weaknesses:

      There are a few problems with the study as presented - mainly that the key formula for the latter section has already been derived and presented in Reference 6 (notably also in this journal), and that the physical basis for the proposed scenarios leading to a large interface resistance is not clearly supported.

      (1) As noted, Equation 32 of the current study is entirely equivalent to Equation 8 of Reference 6, with a very similar derivation presented in Appendix 1 of that paper. In fact, Equation 8 in Reference 6 takes one more step by combining Equations 32 and 35 to provide a general expression for the interface resistance in an integral form. These prior results should be properly cited in the current work - the existing citations to Reference 6 do not make this overlap apparent.

      (2) The authors of the current study go on to examine cases where this shared equation (here Equation 32) might imply a large interface resistance. The examples are mathematically correct, but physically unsupported. In order to produce a substantial interface resistance, the current authors have to suppose that in the interface region between the dense and dilute phases, either there is a local minimum of the diffusion coefficient or a local minimum of the density. I am not aware of any realistic model that would produce either of these minima. Indeed, the authors do not present sufficient examples or physical arguments that would support the existence of such minima.

      In my view, these two issues limit the general interest of the latter portion of the current manuscript. While point 1 can be remedied by proper citation, point 2 is not so simple to address. The two ways the authors present to produce a substantial interface resistance seem to me to be mathematical exercises without a physical basis. The manuscript will improve if the authors can provide examples or compelling arguments for a minimum of either diffusion coefficient or density between the dense and dilute phases that would address point 2.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This work provides a general theoretical framework for understanding molecular transport across liquid-liquid phase boundaries, focusing on interfacial resistance arising from deviations from local equilibrium. By bridging sharp and continuous interface descriptions, the authors demonstrate how distinct microscopic mechanisms can yield similar effective kinetics and propose practical experimental validation strategies.

      Strengths:

      (1) Conceptually rich and physically insightful interface resistance formulation in sharp and continuous limits.

      (2) Strong integration of non-equilibrium thermodynamics with biologically motivated transport scenarios.

      (3) Thorough numerical and analytical support, with thoughtful connection to current and emerging experimental techniques.

      (4) Relevance to various systems, including biomolecular condensates and engineered aqueous two-phase systems.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The work remains theoretical, mainly, with limited direct comparison to quantitative experimental data.

      (2) The biological implications are only briefly explored; further discussion of specific systems where interface resistance might play a functional role would enhance the impact.

      (3) Some model assumptions (e.g., symmetric labeling or idealized diffusivity profiles) could be further contextualized regarding biological variability.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      The manuscript investigated the kinetics of molecule transport across interfaces in phase-separated mixtures. Through the development of a theoretical approach for a binary mixture in a sharp interface limit, the authors found that interface resistance leads to a slowdown in interfacial movement. Subsequently, they extended this approach to multiple molecular species (incorporating both labeled and unlabeled molecules) and continuous transport models. Finally, they proposed experimental settings in vitro and commented on the necessary optical resolution to detect signatures of interfacial kinetics associated with resistance.

      The investigation of transport kinetics across biomolecular condensate interfaces holds significant relevance for understanding cellular function and dysfunction mechanisms; thus, the topic is important and timely. However, the current manuscript presentation requires improvement. Firstly, the inclusion of numerous equations in the main text substantially compromises readability, and relocation of a part of the formulae and derivations to the Appendix would be more appropriate. Secondly, the manuscript would benefit from more comprehensive comparisons with existing theoretical studies on molecular transport kinetics. The text should also be written to be more approachable for a general readership. Modifications and sufficient responses to the specific points outlined below are recommended.

      (1) The authors introduced a theoretical framework to study the kinetics of molecules across an interface between two coexisting liquid phases and found that interface resistance leads to a slowdown in interfacial movement in a binary mixture and a decelerated molecule exchange between labeled and unlabeled molecules across the phase boundary. However, these findings appear rather expected. The work would be strengthened by a more thorough discussion of the kinetics of molecule transport across interfaces (such as the physical origin of the interface resistance and its specific impact on transport kinetics).

      (2) The formulae in the manuscript should be checked and corrected. Notably, Equation 10 contains "\phi_2\ln\phi_2" while Eq. 11b shows "n^{-1}\ln\phi_2", suggesting a missing factor of "n^{-1}". Similarly, Equation 18 obtained from Equation 11: the logarithmic term in Eq.11a is "n^{-1}\ln phi_1-\ln(1-\phi)" but the pre-exponential factor in Equation 18a is just "\phi_1/(1-\phi*)", where is "n^{-1}"? Additionally, there is a unit inconsistency in Equation 36, where the unit of \rho (s/m) does not match that of the right-hand side expression (s/m^2).

      (3) The authors stated that the numerical solutions are obtained using a custom finite difference scheme implemented in MATLAB in the Appendix. The description of numerical methods is insufficiently detailed and needs to be expanded, including specific equations or models used to obtain specific figures, the introduction of initial and boundary conditions, the choices of parameters and their reasons in terms of the biology.

      (4) The authors claimed that their framework naturally extends to multiple molecular species, but only showed the situation of labeled and unlabeled molecules across a phase boundary. How about three or more molecular species? Does this framework still work? This should be added to strengthen the manuscript and confirm the framework's general applicability.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This work aims to elucidate the molecular mechanisms affected in hypoxic conditions, causing reduced cortical interneuron migration. They use human assembloids as a migratory assay of subpallial interneurons into cortical organoids and show substantially reduced migration upon 24 hours of hypoxia. Bulk and scRNA-seq show adrenomedullin (ADM) up-regulation, as well as its receptor RAMP2, confirmed atthe protein level. Adding ADM to the culture medium after hypoxic conditions rescues the migration deficits, even though the subtype of interneurons affected is not examined. However, the authors demonstrate very clearly that ineffective ADM does not rescue the phenotype, and blocking RAMP2 also interferes with the rescue. The authors are also applauded for using 4 different cell lines and using human fetal cortex slices as an independent method to explore the DLXi1/2GFP-labelled iPSC-derived interneuron migration in this substrate with and without ADM addition (after confirming that also in this system ADM is up-regulated). Finally, the authors demonstrate PKA-CREB signalling mediating the effect of ADM addition, which also leads to up-regulation of GABAreceptors. Taken together, this is a very carefully done study on an important subject - how hypoxia affects cortical interneuron migration. In my view, the study is of great interest.

      Strengths:

      The strengths of the study are the novelty and the thorough work using several culture methods and 4 independent lines.

      Weaknesses:

      The main weakness is that other genes regulated upon hypoxia are not confirmed, such that readers will not know until which fold change/stats cut-off data are reliable.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary

      The manuscript by Puno and colleagues investigates the impact of hypoxia on cortical interneuron migration and downstream signaling pathways. They establish two models to test hypoxia, cortical forebrain assembloids, and primary human fetal brain tissue. Both of these models provide a robust assay for interneuron migration. In addition, they find that ADM signaling mediates the migration deficits and rescue using exogenous ADM. The findings are novel and very interesting to the neurodevelopmental field, revealing new insights into how cortical interneurons migrate and as well, establishing exciting models for future studies. The authors use sufficient iPSC line,s including both XX and XY, so the analysis is robust. In addition, the RNAseq data with re-oxygenation is a nice control to see what genes are changed specifically due to hypoxia. Further, the overall level of validation of the sequencing data and involvement of ADM signaling is convincing, including the validation of ADM at the protein level. Overall, this is a very nice manuscript. I have a few comments and suggestions for the authors.

      Strengths and Weaknesses:

      (1) Can the authors comment on the possibility of inflammatory response pathways being activated by hypoxia? Has this been shown before? While not the focus of the manuscript, it could be discussed in the Discussion as an interesting finding and potential involvement of other cells in the Hypoxic response.

      (2) Could the authors comment on the mechanism at play here with respect to ADM and binding to RAMP2 receptors - is this a potential autocrine loop, or is the source of ADM from other cell types besides inhibitory neurons? Given the scRNA-seq data, what cell-to-cell mechanisms can be at play? Since different cells express ADM, there could be different mechanisms in place in ventral vs dorsal areas.

      (3) For data from Figure 6 - while the ELISA assays are informative to determine which pathways (PKA, AKT, ERK) are active, there is no positive control to indicate these assays are "working" - therefore, if possible, western blot analysis from assembloid tissue could be used (perhaps using the same lysates from Figure 3) as an alternative to validate changes at the protein level (however, this might prove difficult); further to this, is P-CREB activated at the protein level using WB?

      (4) Could the authors comment further on the mechanism and what biological pathways and potential events are downstream of ADM binding to RAMP2 in inhibitory neurons? What functional impact would this have linked to the CREB pathway proposed? While the link to GABA receptors is proposed, CREB has many targets beyond this.

      (5) Does hypoxia cause any changes to inhibitory neurogenesis (earlier stages than migration?) - this might always be known, but was not discussed.

      (6) In the Discussion section, it might be worth detailing to the readers what the functional impact of delayed/reduced migration of inhibitory neurons into the cortex might result in, in terms of functional consequences for neural circuit development.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors aimed to test whether hypoxia disrupts the migration of human cortical interneurons, a process long suspected to underlie brain injury in preterm infants but previously inaccessible for direct study. Using human forebrain assembloids and ex vivo developing brain tissue, they visualized and quantified interneuron migration under hypoxic conditions, identified molecular components of the response, and explored the effect of pharmacological intervention (specifically ADM) on restoring the migration deficits.

      Strengths:

      The major strength of this study lies in its use of human forebrain assembloids and ex vivo prenatal brain tissue, which provide a direct system to study interneuron migration under hypoxic conditions. The authors combine multiple approaches: long-term live imaging to directly visualize interneuron migration, bulk and single-cell transcriptomics to identify hypoxia-induced molecular responses, pharmacological rescue experiments with ADM to establish therapeutic potential, and mechanistic assays implicating the cAMP/PKA/pCREB pathway and GABA receptor expression in mediating the effect. Together, this rigorous and multifaceted strategy convincingly demonstrates that hypoxia disrupts interneuron migration and that ADM can restore this defect through defined molecular mechanisms.

      Overall, the authors achieve their stated aims, and the results strongly support their conclusions. The work has a significant impact by providing the first direct evidence of hypoxia-induced interneuron migration deficits in the human context, while also nominating a candidate therapeutic avenue. Beyond the specific findings, the methodological platform - particularly the combination of assembloids and live imaging - will be broadly useful to the community for probing neurodevelopmental processes in health and disease.

      Weaknesses:

      The main weakness of the study lies in the extent to which forebrain assembloids recapitulate in vivo conditions, as the migration of interneurons from hSO to hCO does not fully reflect the native environment or migratory context of these cells. Nevertheless, this limitation is tempered by the fact that the work provides the first direct observation of human interneuron migration under hypoxia, representing a major advance for the field. In addition, while the transcriptomic analyses are valuable and highlight promising candidates, more in-depth exploration will be needed to fully elucidate the molecular mechanisms governing neuronal migration and maturation under hypoxic conditions.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors used weighted ensemble enhanced sampling molecular dynamics (MD) to test the hypothesis that a double mutant of Abl favors the DFG-in state relative to the WT and therefore causes the drug resistance to imatinib.

      Strengths:

      The authors employed the state-of-the-art weighted ensemble MD simulations with three novel progress coordinates to explore the conformational changes the DFG motif of Abl kinase. The hypothesis regarding the double mutant's drug resistance is novel.

      Weaknesses:

      The study contains many uncertain aspects. A major revision is needed to strengthen the support for the conclusions.

      (1) Specifically, the authors need to define the DFG conformation using criteria accepted in the field, for example, see https://klifs.net/index.php.

      (2) Convergence needs to be demonstrated for estimating the population difference between different conformational states.

      (3) The DFG flip needs to be sampled several times to establish free energy difference.

      (4) The free energy plots do not appear to show an intermediate state as claimed.

      (5) The trajectory length of 7 ns in both Figure 2 and Figure 4 needs to be verified, as it is extremely short for a DFG flip that has a high free energy barrier.

      (6) The free energy scale (100 kT) appears to be one order of magnitude too large.

      (7) Setting the DFG-Asp to the protonated state is not justified, because in the DFG-in state, the DFG-Asp is clearly deprotonated.

      (8) Finally, the authors should discuss their work in the context of the enormous progress made in theoretical studies and mechanistic understanding of the conformational landscape of protein kinases in the last two decades, particularly with regard to the DFG flip.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This is a well-written manuscript on the mechanism of the DFG flip in kinases. This conformational change is important for the toggling of kinases between active (DFG-in) and inactive (DFG-out) states. The relative probabilities of these two states are also an important determinant of the affinity of inhibitors for a kinase. However, it is an extremely slow/rare conformational change, making it difficult to capture in simulations. The authors show that weighted ensemble simulations can capture the DFG flip and then delve into the mechanism of this conformational change and the effects of mutations.

      Strengths:

      The DFG flip is very hard to capture in simulations. Showing that this can be done with relatively little simulation by using enhanced sampling is a valuable contribution. The manuscript gives a nice description of the background for non-experts.

      Weaknesses:

      I was disappointed by the anecdotal approach to presenting the results. Molecular processes are stochastic and the authors have expertise in describing such processes. However, they chose to put most statistical analysis in the SI. The main text instead describes the order of events in single "representative" trajectories. The main text makes it sound like these were most selected as they were continuous trajectories from the weighted ensemble simulations. I would much rather hear a description of the highest probability pathway(s) with some quantification of how probable they are. That would give the reader a clear sense of how representative the events described are.

      I appreciated the discussion of the strengths/weaknesses of weighted ensemble simulations. Am I correct that this method doesn't do anything to explicitly enhance sampling along orthogonal degrees of freedom? Maybe a point worth mentioning if so.

      I don't understand Figure 3C. Could the authors instead show structures corresponding to each of the states in 3B, and maybe also a representative structure for pathways 1 and 2?

      Why introduce S1 and DFG-inter? And why suppose that DFG-inter is what corresponds to the excited state seen by NMR?

      It would be nice to have error bars on the populations reported in Figure 3.

      I'm confused by the attempt to relate the relative probabilities of states to the 32 kca/mol barrier previously reported between the states. The barrier height should be related to the probability of a transition. The DFG-out state could be equiprobable with the DFG-in state and still have a 32 kcal/mol barrier separating them.

      How do the relative probabilities of the DFG-in/out states compare to experiments, like NMR?

      Do the staggered and concerted DFG flip pathways mentioned correspond to pathways 1 and 2 in Figure 3B, or is that a concept from previous literature?

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors aimed to examine how the covariation between cognition (represented by a g-factor based on 12 features of 11 cognitive tasks) and mental health (represented by 133 diverse features) is reflected in MR-based neural markers of cognition, as measured through multimodal neuroimaging (structural, rsfMRI, and diffusion MR). To integrate multiple neuroimaging phenotypes across MRI modalities, they used a so-called stacking approach, which employs two levels of machine learning. First, they built a predictive model from each neuroimaging phenotype to predict a target variable. Next, in the stacking level, they used predicted values (i.e., cognition predicted from each neuroimaging phenotype) from the first level as features to predict the target variable. To quantify the contribution of the neural indicators of cognition explaining the relationship between cognition and mental health, they conducted commonality analyses. Results showed that when they stacked neuroimaging phenotypes within dwMRI, rsMRI, and sMRI, they captured 25.5%, 29.8%, and 31.6% of the predictive relationship between cognition and mental health, respectively. By stacking all 72 neuroimaging phenotypes across three MRI modalities, they enhanced the explanation to 48%. Age and sex shared substantial overlapping variance with both mental health and neuroimaging in explaining cognition, accounting for 43% of the variance in the cognition-mental health relationship.

      Strengths:

      (1) A big study population (UK Biobank with 14000 subjects).

      (2) The description of the methods (including Figure 1) is helpful in understanding the approach.

      (3) This revised manuscript is much improved compared to the previous version.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Although the background and reason for the study are better described in this version of the manuscript, the relevance of the question is, in my opinion, still questionable. The authors aimed to determine whether neural markers of cognition explain the covariance between cognition and mental health and which of the 72 MRI-based features contribute to explaining most of the covariance. I would like to invite the authors to make a stronger case for the relevance, keeping the clinical and scientific relevance in mind (what would you explain to the clinician, what would you explain to the people with lived experience, and how can this knowledge contribute to innovation in mental health care?).

      (2) The discussion on the interpretation of the positive and negative PLRS loadings is not very convincing, and the findings are partly counterintuitive. For example (1) how to explain that distress has a positive loading and anxiety/trauma has a negative loading?; (2) how to explain that mental health features like wellbeing and happiness load in the same direction as psychosis and anxiety/trauma? From both a clinical and a neuroscientific perspective, this is hard to interpret.

      (3) The analysis plan has not been preregistered (e.g. at OSF).

      Note: the computational aspects of the methods fall beyond my expertise.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The goal of this manuscript was to examine whether neural indicators explain the relationship between cognition and mental health. The authors achieved this aim by showing that the combination of MRI markers better predicted the cognition-mental health covariation.

      Strengths:

      The evidence supporting the conclusions is compelling. There is a large sample (UK biobank data) and a clear description of advanced analyses.

      Weaknesses:

      In the previous version of the paper, it was not completely clear what it means to look at the overlap between cognition and mental health. The authors have addressed this in the current version.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The experiment is interesting and well executed and describes in high detail fish behaviour in thermally stratified waters. The evidence is strong but the experimental design cannot distinguish between temperature and vertical position of the treatments.

      Strengths:

      High statistical power, solid quantification of behaviour.

      Weaknesses:

      A major issue with the experimental design is the vertical component of the experiment. Many thermal preference and avoidance experiments are run using horizontal division in shuttlebox systems or in annular choice flumes. These remove the vertical stratification component so that hot and cold can be compared equally, without the vertical layering as a confounding factor. The method chosen, with its vertical stratification, is inherently unable to control for this effect because warm water is always above, and cold water is always below. This complicates the interpretations.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      The paper by Naudascher et al., investigates an interesting question: How do fish react to and avoid thermal disturbances from the optimum that occur on fast timescales. Previous work has identified potential strategies of warm avoidance in fish on short timescales while strategies for cold avoidance are far more elusive. The work combines a clever experimental paradigm with careful analysis to show that trout parr avoid cold water by limiting excursions across a warm-cold thermal interface. While direct measurements of the interface are lacking, thermal dynamics simulations suggest that trout parr avoid the warm-cold interface in the absence of gradient information.

      The authors assume that the thermal interface triggers the upward turning behavior, possibly leading to the formation of an associative memory. However, an alternative explanation is that exposure to cold water during initial excursions increases the tendency for upward turns. In other words, exposure to a cold interface changes the behavioral state leading to increases in gravity controlled upward turning. This could be an adaptive strategy since for temperatures > 4C swimming upwards is a good strategy to reach warmer water. That being said, the vertical design offers new insight and is ecologically relevant.

    1. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Egawa et al describe the developmental timeline of the assembly of nodes of Ranvier in the chick brainstem auditory circuit. In this unique system, the spacing between nodes varies significantly in different regions of the same axon from early stages, which the authors suggest is critical for accurate sound localization. Egawa et al set out to determine which factors regulate this differential node spacing. They do this by using immunohistological analyses to test the correlation of node spacing with morphological properties of the axons, and properties of oligodendrocytes, glial cells that wrap axons with the myelin sheaths that flank the nodes of Ranvier. They find that axonal structure does not vary significantly, but that oligodendrocyte density and morphology varies in the different regions traversed by these axons, which suggests this is a key determinant of the region-specific differences in node density and myelin sheath length. They also find that differential oligodendrocyte density is partly determined by secreted neuronal signals, as (presumed) blockage of vesicle fusion with tetanus toxin reduced oligodendrocyte density in the region where it is normally higher. Based on these findings, the authors propose that oligodendrocyte morphology, myelin sheath length, and consequently nodal distribution are primarily determined by intrinsic oligodendrocyte properties rather than neuronal factors such as activity.

      Major comments:

      (1) The authors should test the efficiency of TeNT to validate that vesicular release is indeed inhibited from expressing neurons. Additionally, the authors should clarify if their TeNT expression system results in the whole tract being silenced, or results in sparse vesicular release inhibition in only a few neurons.

      (2) The authors should revise their statistical analyses throughout, and supply additional information to explain the rationale for the statistical tests used, including e.g. data normality, paired sampling, number of samples/independent biological replicates.

      (3) The main finding of the study is that the density of nodes differs between two regions of the chicken auditory circuit, probably due to morphological differences in the respective oligodendrocytes. Can the authors discuss if this finding is likely to be specific to the avian auditory circuit?

      (4) The study shows a correlation between node spacing and oligodendrocyte density, but the authors did not manipulate oligodendrocyte density per se (i.e. cell-autonomously). The authors should either include such experiments, or discuss their value in supporting the interpretation of their results.

      (5) The authors should discuss very pertinent prior studies, in particular to contextualize their findings with (a) known neuron-autonomous modes of node formation prior to myelination, (b) known effects of vesicular fusion directly on myelinating capacity and oligodendrogenesis, (c) known correlation of myelin length and thickness with axonal diameter, (d) regional heterogeneity in the oligodendrocyte transcriptome.

      Significance:

      In our view the study tackles a fundamental question likely to be of interest to a specialized audience of cellular neuroscientists. This descriptive study is suggestive that in the studied system, oligodendrocyte density determines the spacing between nodes of Ranvier, but further manipulations of oligodendrocyte density per se are needed to test this convincingly.

    2. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors have investigated the myelination pattern along the axons of chick avian cochlear nucleus. It has already been shown that there are regional differences in the internodal length of axons in the nucleus magnocellularis. In the tract region across the midline, internodes are longer than in the nucleus laminaris region. Here the authors suggest that the difference in internodal length is attributed to heterogeneity of oligodendrocytes. In the tract region oligodendrocytes would contribute longer myelin internodes, while oligodendrocytes in the nucleus laminaris region would synthesize shorter myelin internodes. Not only length of myelin internodes differs, but also along the same axon unmyelinated areas between two internodes may vary. This is an interesting contribution since all these differences contribute to differential conduction velocity regulating ipsilateral and contralateral innervation of coincidence detector neurons. However, the demonstration falls rather short of being convincing.

      Major comments:

      (1) The authors neglect the possibility that nodal cluster may be formed prior to myelin deposition. They have investigated stages E12 (no nodal clusters) and E15 (nodal cluster plus MAG+ myelin). Fig. 1D is of dubious quality. It would be important to investigate stages between E12 and E15 to observe the formation of pre-nodes, i.e., clustering of nodal components prior to myelin deposition.

      (2) The claim that axonal diameter is constant along the axonal length need to be demonstrated at the EM level. This would also allow to measure possible regional differences in the thickness of the myelin sheath and number of myelin wraps.

      (3) The observation that internodal length differs is explain by heterogeneity of sources of oligodendrocyte is not convincing. Oligodendrocytes a priori from the same origin remyelinate shorter internode after a demyelination event.

      Significance:

      The authors suggest that the difference in internodal length is attributed to heterogeneity of oligodendrocytes. In the tract region oligodendrocytes would contribute longer myelin internodes, while oligodendrocytes in the nucleus laminaris region would synthesize shorter myelin internodes. Not only length of myelin internodes differs, but also along the same axon unmyelinated areas between two internodes may vary. This is an interesting contribution since all these differences contribute to differential conduction velocity regulating ipsilateral and contralateral innervation of coincidence detector neurons.

      Comments on revised version:

      This revised version is in large improved and the responses to reviewers' comments are generally relevant. However, the response regarding pre-nodes is not satisfactory. I understand that the authors prefer to avoid further experimentations, but I think this is an important point that needs to be clarified. Exploring stages between E12 and E15 are therefore of importance. When carefully examining some of the figures (Fig. 1E or 2D) I think that at E15 they may well be pre-nodes formation prior to myelin deposition, on structure the authors considered to be heminodes. To be convincing they should use double or triple labeling with, in addition to the nodal proteins (ankG and/or Nav pan), a good myelin marker such as antiPLP. The rat monoclonal developed by late Pr Ikenaka would give a sharper staining than the anti MAG they used. (I assume the clone must still be available in Okazaki ).

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The electrocardiogram (ECG) is routinely used to diagnose and assess cardiovascular risk. However, its interpretation can be complicated by sex-based and anatomical variations in heart and torso structure. To quantify these relationships, Dr. Smith and colleagues developed computational tools to automatically reconstruct 3D heart and torso anatomies from UK Biobank data. Their regression analysis identified key sex differences in anatomical parameters and their associations with ECG features, particularly post-myocardial infarction (MI). This work provides valuable quantitative insights into how sex and anatomy influence ECG metrics, potentially improving future ECG interpretation protocols by accounting for these factors.

      Strengths:

      (1) The study introduces an automated pipeline to reconstruct heart and torso anatomies from a large cohort (1,476 subjects, including healthy and post-MI individuals).

      (2) The 3-stage reconstruction achieved high accuracy (validated via Dice coefficient and error distances).

      (3) Extracted anatomical features enabled novel analyses of disease-dependent relationships between sex, anatomy, and ECG metrics.

      (4) Open-source code for the pipeline and analyses enhances reproducibility.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The linear regression approach, while useful, may not fully address collinearity among parameters (e.g., cardiac size, torso volume, heart position). Although left ventricular mass or cavity volume was selected to mitigate collinearity, other parameters (e.g., heart center coordinates) could still introduce bias.

      (2) The study attributes residual ECG differences to sex/MI status after controlling for anatomical variables. However, regression model errors could distort these estimates. A rigorous evaluation of potential deviations (e.g., variance inflation factors or alternative methods like ridge regression) would strengthen the conclusions.

      (3) The manuscript's highly quantitative presentation may hinder readability. Simplifying technical descriptions and improving figure clarity (e.g., separating superimposed bar plots in Figures 2-4) would aid comprehension.

      (4) Given established sex differences in QTc intervals, applying the same analytical framework to explore QTc's dependence on sex and anatomy could have provided additional clinically relevant insights.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Missed diagnosis of myocardial ischemia (MI) is more common in women, and treatment is typically less aggressive. This diagnosis stems from the fact that women's ECGs commonly exhibit 12 lead ECG biomarkers that are less likely to fall within the traditional diagnostic criteria. Namely, women have shorter QRS durations and lower ST junction and T wave amplitudes, but longer QT intervals, than men. To study the impact, this study aims to quantify sex differences in heart-torso anatomy and ECG biomarkers, as well as their relative associations, in both pre- and post-MI populations. A novel computational pipeline was constructed to generate torso-ventricular geometries from cardiac magnetic resonance imaging. The pipeline was used to build models for 425 post-myocardial infarction subjects and 1051 healthy controls from UK Biobank clinical images to generate the population.

      Strengths:

      This study has a strength in that it utilizes a large patient population from the UK Biobank (425 post-MI and 1051 healthy controls) to analyze sex-based differences. The computational pipeline is state-of-the-art for constructing torso-ventricular geometries from cardiac MR and is clinically viable. It draws on novel machine learning techniques for segmentation, contour extraction, and shape modeling. This pipeline is publicly available and can help in the large-scale generation of anatomies for other studies. This allows computation of various anatomical factors (torso volume, cavity volume, etc), and subsequent regression analysis on how these factors are altered before and after MI from the 12-lead ECG.

      Weaknesses:

      Major weaknesses stem from the fact that, while electrophysiological factors appear to play a role across many leads, both post-MI and healthy, the electrophysiological factors are not stated or discussed. The computational modeling pipeline is validated for reconstructing torso contours; however, potential registration errors stemming from ventricular-torso construction are not addressed within the context of anatomical factors, such as the tilt and rotation of the heart. This should be discussed as the paper's claims are based on these results. Further analysis and explanation are needed to understand how these sex-specific results impact the ECG-based diagnosis of MI in men and women, as stated as the primary reason for the study at the beginning of the paper. This would provide a broader impact within the clinical community. Claims about demographics do not appear to be supported within the main manuscript but are provided in the supplements. Reformatting the paper's structure is required to efficiently and effectively present and support the findings and outcomes of this work.

  2. resu-bot-bucket.s3.ca-central-1.amazonaws.com resu-bot-bucket.s3.ca-central-1.amazonaws.com
    1. Instructed 1,000+ students on manufacturing best practices, emphasizing safety and build quality.

      Quantify the impact of your instruction. Did it lead to fewer errors or higher quality projects? Provide metrics.

    2. Trained over 100 students every semester on the safety protocols and applicable use cases for all MakerSpace equipment including 3D printers(FDM/SLA), laser cutters, CNC Machines, thermal formers, hand/power tools.

      Include the impact of your training. Did it lead to improved safety records or student confidence?

    3. Developed python-based computer vision dice recognition application capable of detecting and logging results for multiple dice types (D4–D20).

      Mention the user base or potential applications of this project. Who would benefit from it?

    4. Created standards for employee software interaction, improved efficiency, reducing operation costs by 40%.

      Detail what specific standards were created. How did they lead to the 40% cost reduction? Be more specific.

    5. Revised, modularized, and updated old assembly program to a modern code base removing 22 detected bugs enabling future feature implementation.

      Explain how bug removal improved functionality or user experience. Provide examples of features enabled.

    6. Unified three isolated programs into one software solution utilizing Java, PHP, SQL(MySQL), and RESTful API, removing the need for paper communication digitizing employee work.

      Quantify the impact of digitizing work. How much time or cost was saved? Include specific metrics.

    7. Supported 45 project groups with project management including Project Charter, Scope, DOD, Stakeholder management, WBS/WBS dictionary, scrum ceremonies, risk assessment, Agile, lifecycle, and product handover.

      Clarify your role in project management. Did you lead or facilitate? Highlight your direct contributions.

    8. Planned and implemented creative projects following the school’s curriculum and objectives, improving students’ understanding of course material, resulting in an average of a letter grade improvement.

      Specify how you measured the improvement in understanding. Include metrics or feedback to enhance impact.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This is a high-quality and extensive study that reveals differences in the self-assembly properties of the full set of 109 human death fold domains (DFDs). Distributed amphifluoric FRET (DAmFRET) is a powerful tool that reveals the self-assembly behaviour of the DFDs, in non-seeded and seeded contexts, and allows comparison of the nature and extent of self-assembly. The nature of the barriers to nucleation is revealed in the transition from low to high AmFRET. Alongside analysis of the saturation concentration and protein concentration in the absence of seed, the subset of proteins that exhibited discontinuous transitions to higher-order assemblies was observed to have higher concentrations than DFDs that exhibited continuous transitions. The experiments probing the ~20% of DFDs that exhibit discontinuous transition to polymeric form suggest that they populate a metastable, supersaturated form in the absence of cognate signal. This is suggestive of a high intrinsic barrier to nucleation.

      Strengths:

      The differences in self-assembly behaviour are significant and likely identify mechanistic differences across this large family of signalling adapter domains. The work is of high quality, and the evidence for a range of behaviours is strong. This is an important and useful starting point since the different assembly mechanisms point towards specific cellular roles. However, understanding the molecular basis for these differences will require further analysis.

      An impressive optogenetic approach was engineered and applied to initiate self-assembly of CASP1 and CASP9 DFDs, as a model for apoptosome initiation in these two DFDs with differing continuous or discontinuous assembly properties. This comparison revealed clear differences in the stability and reversibility of the assemblies, supporting the hypothesis that supersaturation-mediated DFD assembly underlies signal amplification in at least some of the DFDs.

      The study reveals interesting correlations between supersaturation of DFD adapters in short- and long-lived cells, suggestive of a relationship between the mechanism of assembly and cellular context. Additionally, the comprehensive nature of the study provides strong evidence that the interactions are almost all homomeric or limited to members of the same DFD subfamily or interaction network. Similar approaches with bacterial proteins from innate immunity operons suggest that their polymerisation may be driven by similar mechanisms.

      Weaknesses:

      Only a limited investigation of assembly morphology was conducted by microscopy. There was a tendency for discontinuous structures to form fibrillar structures and continuous to populate diffuse or punctate structures, but there was overlap across all categories, which is not fully explored. The methodology used to probe oligomeric assembly and stability (SDD-AGE) does not justify the conclusions drawn regarding stability and native structure within the assemblies.

      The work identifies important differences between DFDs and clearly different patterns of association. However, most of the detailed analysis is of the DFDs that exhibit a discontinuous transition, and important questions remain about the majority of other DFDs and why some assemblies should be reversible and others not, and about the nature of signalling arising from a continuous transition to polymeric form.

      Some key examples of well-studied DFDs, such as MyD88 and RIPK,1 deserve more discussion, since they display somewhat surprising results. More detailed exploration of these candidates, where much is known about their structures and the nature of the assemblies from other work, could substantiate the conclusions here and transform some of the conclusions from speculative to convincing.

      The study concludes with general statements about the relationship between stochastic nucleation and mortality, which provide food for thought and discussion but which, as they concede, are highly speculative. The analogies that are drawn with batteries and privatisation will likely not be clearly understood by all readers. The authors do not discuss limitations of the study or elaborate on further experiments that could interrogate the model.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript from Rodriguez Gama et al. proposes several interesting conclusions based on different oligomerization properties of Death-Fold Domains (DFDs) in cells, their natural abundance, and supersaturation properties. These ideas are:<br /> (1) DFDs broadly store the cell's energy by remaining in a supersaturated state;<br /> (2) Cells are constantly in a vulnerable state that could lead to cell death;<br /> (3) The cell's lifespan depends on the supersaturation levels of certain DFDs.

      Overall, the evidence supporting these claims is not completely solid. Some concerns were noted.

      Strengths:

      Systematic analysis of DFD self-assembly and its relationship with protein abundance, supersaturation, cell longevity, and evolution.

      Weaknesses

      (1) On page 2, it is stated, "Nucleation barriers increase with the entropic cost of assembly. Assemblies with large barriers, therefore, tend to be more ordered than those without. Ordered assembly often manifests as long filaments in cells," as a way to explain the observed results that DFDs assemblies that transitioned discontinuously form fibrils, whereas those that transitioned continuously (low-to-high) formed spherical or amorphous puncta. It is unlikely to be able to differentiate between amorphous and structured puncta by conventional confocal microscopy. Some DFDs self-assemble into structured puncta formed by intertwined fibrils. Such fibril nets are more structured and thus should be associated with a higher entropic cost. Therefore, the results in Figure 1B do not seem to agree with the reasoning described.

      (2) Errors for the data shown in Figure 1B would have been very useful to determine whether the population differences between diffuse, punctate, and fibrillar for the continuous (low-to-high) transition are meaningful.

      (3) A main concern in the data shown in Figure 1B and F is that the number of counts for discontinuous compared to continuous is small. Thus, the significance of the results is difficult to evaluate in the context of the broad function of DFDs as batteries, as stated at the beginning of the manuscript.

      (4) The proteins or domains that are self-seeded (Figure 1F) should be listed such that the reader has a better understanding of whether domains or full-length proteins are considered, whether other domains have an effect on self-seeding (which is not discussed), and whether there is repetition.

      (5) The authors indicate an anticorrelation between transcript abundance and Csat based on the data shown in Figure 2B; however, the data are scattered. It is not clear why an anticorrelation is inferred.

      (6) It would be useful to indicate the expected range of degree centrality. The differences observed are very small. This is specifically the case for the BC values. The lack of context and the small differences cast doubts on their significance. It would be beneficial to describe these data in the context of the centrality values of other proteins.

      (7) Page 3 section title: "Nucleation barriers are a characteristic feature of inflammatory signalosome adaptors." This title seems to contradict the results shown in Figure 2D, where full-length CARD9 and CARD11 are classified as sensors, but it has been reported that they are adaptor proteins with key roles in the inflammatory response. Please see the following references as examples: The adaptor protein CARD9 is essential for the activation of myeloid cells through ITAM-associated and Toll-like receptors. Nat Immunol 8, 619-629 (2007), and Mechanisms of Regulated and Dysregulated CARD11 Signaling in Adaptive Immunity and Disease. Front Immunol. 2018 Sep 19;9:2105.

      However, both CARD9 and CARD11 show discontinuous to continuous behavior for the individual DFDs versus full-length proteins, respectively, in contrast to the results obtained for ASC, FADD, etc. FADD plays a key role in apoptosis but shows the same behavior as BCL10 and ASC. However, the manuscript indicates that this behavior is characteristic of inflammatory signalosomes. What is the explanation for adaptor proteins behaving in different ways? This casts doubts about the possibility of deriving general conclusions on the significance of these observations, or the subtitles in the results section seem to be oversimplifications.

      (8) IFI16-PYD displays discontinuous behavior according to Figure S1H; however, it is not included in Figure 2D, but AIM 2 is.

      (9) To demonstrate that "Nucleation barriers facilitate signal amplification in human cells," constructs using APAF1 CARD, NLRC4 CARD, caspase-9 CARD, and a chimera of the latter are used to create what the authors refer to as apoptsomes. Even though puncta are observed, referring to these assemblies as apoptosomes seems somewhat misleading. In addition, it is not clear why the activity of caspase-9 was not measured directly, instead of that of capsae-3 and 7, which could be activated by other means. The polymerization of caspase-1 CARD with NLRC4 CARD, leading to irreversible puncta, could just mean that the polymers are more stable. In fact, not all DFDs form equally stable or identical complexes, which does not necessarily imply that a nucleation barrier facilitates signal amplification. Could this conclusion be an overstatement?

      (10) To demonstrate that "Innate immune adaptors are endogenously supersaturated," it is stated on page 5 that ASC clusters continue to grow for the full duration of the time course and that AIM2-PYD stops growing after 5 min. The data shown in Figure 4F indicate that AIM2-PYD grows after 5 mins, although slowly, and ASC starts to slow down at ~ 13 min. Because ASC has two DFDs, assemblies can grow faster and become bigger. How is this related to supersaturation?

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This work shows that a specific adenosine deaminase protein in Dictyostelium generates the ammonia that is required for tip formation during Dictyostelium development. Cells with an insertion in the adgf gene aggregate but do not form tips. A remarkable result, shown by several different ways, is that the adgf mutant can be rescued by exposing the mutant to ammonia gas. The authors also describe other phenotypes of the adgf mutant such as increased mound size, altered cAMP signaling, and abnormal cell type differentiation. It appears that the adgf mutant has defects the expression of a large number of genes, resulting in not only the tip defect but also the mound size, cAMP signaling, and differentiation phenotypes.

      Strengths:

      The data and statistics are excellent.

      Weaknesses:

      The key weakness is understanding why the cells bother to use a diffusible gas like ammonia as a signal to form a tip and continue development. The rescue of the mutant by adding ammonia gas to the entire culture indicates that ammonia conveys no positional information within the mound. By the time the cells have formed a mound, the cells have been starving for several hours, and desperately need to form a fruiting body to disperse some of themselves as spores, and thus need to form a tip no matter what. One can envision that the local ammonia concentration is possibly informing the mound that some minimal number of cells are present (assuming that the ammonia concentration is proportional to the number of cells), but probably even a miniscule fruiting body would be preferable to the cells compared to a mound. This latter idea could be easily explored by examining the fate of the adgf cells in the mound - do they all form spores? Do some form spores? Or perhaps the ADGF is secreted by only one cell type, and the resulting ammonia tells the mound that for some reason that cell type is not present in the mound, allowing some of the cells to transdifferentiate into the needed cell type. Thus elucidating if all or some cells produce ADGF would greatly strengthen this puzzling story.

      Comments on revisions:

      Looks better, but I think you answered my questions (listed as weaknesses in the public review) in the reply to the reviewer but not in the paper. I'd suggest carefully thinking about my questions and addressing them in the Discussion. You did however do all of the things in the paper that were listed as "Recommendations for the authors"

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The paper describes new insights into the role of adenosine deaminase-related growth factor (adgf), an enzyme that catalyses the breakdown of adenosine into ammonia and inosine, in tip formation during Dictyostelium development. The adgf null mutant has a pre-tip mound arrest phenotype, which can be rescued by external addition of ammonia. Analysis suggests that the phenotype involves changes in cAMP signaling possibly involving a histidine kinase dhkD, but details remain to be resolved.

      Strengths:

      The generation of an adgf mutant showed a strong mound arrest phenotype and successful rescue by external ammonia. Characterisation of significant changes in cAMP signaling components, suggesting low cAMP signaling in the mutant and identification of the histidine kinase dhkD as a possible component of the transduction pathway. Identification of a change in cell-type differentiation towards prestalk fate

      Weaknesses:

      Lack of details on developmental time course of adgf activity and cell-type-specific differences in adgf expression. Absence of measurements to show that ammonia addition to the null mutant can rescue the proposed defects in cAMP signaling. No direct measurements in the dhkD mutant to show that it acts upstream of sdgf in the control of changes in cAMP signaling and tip formation.

      Comments on revisions:

      The revised version of the paper has improved significantly in terms of structure and clarity. The additional data on rescue of total cAMP production by ammonia (Fig. 7C) in the adgf- mutant and the 5-fold increased prespore expression of adgf RNA compared to prestalk cells (Fig 9) are useful data additions.

      The link between changes in cAMP signaling (lower aca expression) and wave geometry ( concentric waves rather than spiral waves) remains speculative.

      I noted that Fig 6 contains different images than the previous version (Fig 7).

      The statement "Interestingly, Klebsiella pneumoniae physically separated from the Dictyostelium adgf mutants in a partitioned dish, also rescues the mound arrest phenotype suggesting a cross-kingdom interaction that drives development" in the summary is rather overdone. All experiments were performed with axenic strains (no bacteria).

      as is the sentence "Remarkably, in higher vertebrates, adgf expression is elevated during gastrulation and thus adenosine deamination may be a conserved process driving organizer development in different organisms"<br /> The data supporting this in the supplementary information is hardly legible and poorly presented. What is shown is ADA expression in different tissues, not at different stages. I would suggest taking these figures out and concentrating the summary on the key mechanistic findings of the paper.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The paper sets out to examine the social recognition abilities of a 'solitary' jumping spider species. It demonstrates that based on vision alone spiders can habituate and dishabituate to the presence of conspecifics. The data support the interpretation that these spiders can distinguish between conspecifics on the basis of their appearance.

      Strengths:

      The study presents two experiments. The second set of data recapitulates the findings of the first experiment with a independent set of spiders, highlighting the strength of the results. The study also uses a highly quantitative approach to measuring relative interest between pairs of spiders based on their distance.

      Weaknesses:

      The study design is overly complicated, while missing key controls, and the data presented in the figures are not clearly connected to study. The discussion is challenging to understand and appears to make unsupported conclusions.

      (1) Study design: The study design is rather complicated and as a result it is difficult to interpret the results. The spiders are presented with the same individual twice in a row, called a habituation trial. Then a new individual is presented twice in a row. The first of these is a dishabituation trial and the second another habituation trial (but now habituating to a second individual). This done with three pairings and then this entire structure is repeated over three sessions. The data appear to show the strong effects of differences between habituation and dishabituation trials in the first session. The decrease in differential behavior between the so-called habituation and dishabituation trials in sessions 2 and 3 are explained as a consequence of the spiders beginning to habituate in general to all of the individuals. The claim that the spiders remember specific individuals is somewhat undercut because all of the 'dishabituation' trials in session 2 are toward spiders they already met for 14 minute previously but seemingly do not remember in session 2. In session 3 it is ambiguous what is happening because the spiders no longer differentiate between the trial types. This could be due to fatigue or familiarity. A second experiment is done to show that introducing a totally novel individual, recovers a large dishabituation response, suggesting that the lack of differences between 'habituation' and 'dishabituation' trials in session 3 is the result of general habituation to all of the spiders in the session rather than fatigue. As mentioned before, these data do support the claim that the spiders differentiate among individuals.

      The data from session 1 are easy to interpret. The data from sessions 2 and 3 are harder to understand, but these are the trials in which they meet an individual again after a substantial period of separation. Other studies looking at recognition in ants and wasps (cited by the authors) have done a 4 trial design in which focal animal A meets B in the first trial, then meet C in the second trial, meets B again in the third trial, and then meets D in the last trial. In that scenario trials 1, 2 and 4 are between unfamiliar individuals and trial 3 is between potentially familiar individuals. In both the ants and wasps, high aggression is seen in species with and without recognition on trial 1, with low aggression specifically for trials with familiar individuals in species with recognition. Across different tests, species or populations that lack recognition have shown a general reduction in aggression towards all individuals that becomes progressively less aggressive over time (reminiscent of the session 2 and 3 data) while others have maintained modest levels of aggression across all individuals. The 4 session design used in those other studies provides an unambiguous interpretation of the data, while controlling for 'fatigue'. That all trials in sessions 2 and 3 are always with familiar individuals make it challenging to understand how much the spiders are habituating to each other versus having some kind of associative learning of individual identity and behavior.

      The data presentation is also very complicated. How is it the case that a negative proportion of time is spent? The methods reveal that this metric is derived by comparing the time individuals spent in each region relative to the previous time they saw that individual. At the very least, data showing the distribution of distances from the wall would be much easier to interpret for the reader.

      (2) "Long-term social memory": It is not entirely clear what is meant by the authors when they say 'long-term social memory', though typically long-term memory refers to a form of a memory that require protein synthesis. While the precise timing of memory formation varies across species and contexts, a general rule is that long term memory should last for > 24 hours (e.g., Dreier et al 2007 Biol Letters). The longest time that spider are apart in this trial set up is something like an hour. There is no basis to claim that spiders have long term social memory as they are never asked to remember anyone after a long time apart. The odd phrasing of the 'long-term dishabutation' trial makes it seem that it is testing a long-term memory, but it is not. The spiders have never met. The fact that they are very habituated to one set of stimuli and then respond to a new stimulus is not evidence of long-term memory. To clearly test memory (which is the part really lacking from the design), the authors would need to show that spiders - upon the first instance of re-encountering a previously encountered individual are already 'habituated' to them but not to some other individuals. The current data suggest this may be the case, but it is just very hard to interpret given the design does not directly test memory of individuals in a clear and unambiguous manner.

      (3) Lack of a functional explanation and the emphasis on 'asociality': It is entirely plausible that recognition is pleitropic byproduct of the overall visual cognition abilities in the spiders. However, the discussion that discounts territoriality as a potential explanation is not well laid out. First, many species that are 'asocial' nevertheless defend territories. It is perhaps best to say such species are not group living, but they have social lives because they encounter conspecifics and need to interact with them. Indeed, there are many examples of solitary living species that show the dear enemy effect, a form of individual recognition, towards familiar territorial neighbors. The authors in this case note that territorial competition is mediated by the size of color of the chelicerae (seemingly a trait that could be used to distinguish among individuals). Apparently because previous work has suggested that territorial disputes can be mediated by a trait in the absence of familiarity has led them to discount the possibility that keeping track of the local neighbors in a potentially cannibalistic species could be a sufficient functional reason. In any event, the current evidence presented certainly does not warrant discounting that hypothesis.

      Comments on Revision:

      The authors have not actually addressed my points and their comments conflate discrimination with recognition. The extensive discussion about how babies are tested for discrimination tasks in their rebuttal misses the point. I believe that the data do show that the spiders discriminate between individuals but whether individuals are recognized (i.e., remembered) is less clear. The authors defend their convoluted study design, but it is overly complex and challenging to interpret the data as a result.

      The main issue with the design is that they do not actually test for any kind of memory of specific individuals after a substantial time of separation. Instead they show that a new individuals is still surprising/dishabituating. That is nice evidence for discrimination but does not show memory in a clear and unambiguous way.

      My comments and critique are unchanged since they didn't really change the paper. New experiments were needed and they didn't do any. Perhaps it is hard to get the spiders where they are? I don't really understand why they didn't do additional experiments as part of this revision.

    2. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Jumping spiders (family Salticidae) have extraordinarily good eyesight, but little is known about how sensitive these small animals might be to the identity of other individuals that they see. Here, experiments were carried out using Phidippus regius, a salticid spider from North America. There were three steps in the experiments; first, a spider could see another spider; then its view of the other spider was blocked; and then either the same or a different individual spider came into view. Whether it was the same or a different individual that came into view in the third step had a significant effect on how close together or far apart the spiders positioned themselves. It has been demonstrated before that salticids can discriminate between familiar and unfamiliar individuals while relying on chemical cues, but this new research on P. regius provides the first experimental evidence that a spider can discriminate by sight between familiar and unfamiliar individuals.

      Clark RJ, Jackson RR (1995) Araneophagic jumping spiders discriminate between the draglines of familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics. Ethology, Ecology and Evolution 7:185-190

      Strengths:

      This work is a useful step toward a fuller understanding of the perceptual and cognitive capacities of spiders and other animals with small nervous systems. By providing experimental evidence for a conclusion that a spider can, by sight, discriminate between familiar and unfamiliar individuals, this research will be an important milestone. We can anticipate a substantial influence on future research.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The conclusions should be stated more carefully.

      (2) It is not clearly the case that the experimental methods are based on 'habituation (learning to ignore; learning not to respond). Saying 'habituation' seems to imply that certain distances are instances of responding and other distances are instances of not responding but, as a reasonable alternative, we might call distance in all instances a response. However, whether all distances are responses or not is a distracting issue because being based on habituation is not a necessity.

      (3) Besides data related to distances, other data might have been useful. For example, salticids are especially well known for the way they communicate using distinctive visual displays and, unlike distance, displaying is a discrete, unambiguous response.

      (4) Methods more aligned with salticids having extraordinarily good eyesight would have useful. For example, with salticids, standardising and manipulating stimuli in experiments can be achieved by using mounts, video playback and computer-generated animation.

      (5) An asocial-versus-social distinction is too imprecise, and it may have been emphasised too much. With P. regius, irrespective of whether we use the label asocial or social, the important question pertains to the frequency of encounters between the same individuals and the consequences of these encounters.

      (6) Hypotheses related to not-so-strictly adaptive factors are discussed and these hypotheses are interesting, but these considerations are not necessarily incompatible with more strictly adaptive influences being relevant as well.

      Comments on Revision:

      The authors have responded reasonably to the comments I made. There is nothing else that I wish to add.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors aim to explore how interdisciplinarity and internationalization-two increasingly prominent characteristics of scientific publishing-have evolved over the past century. By constructing entropy-based indices from a large-scale bibliometric dataset (OpenAlex), they examine both long-term trends and recent dynamics in these two dimensions across a selection of leading disciplinary and multidisciplinary journals. Their goal is to identify field-specific patterns and structural shifts that can inform our understanding of how science has become more globally collaborative and intellectually integrated.

      Strengths and Weaknesses:

      The paper's primary strength lies in its comprehensive temporal scope and use of a rich, openly available dataset covering over 56 million articles. The interdisciplinary and internationalization indices are well-founded and allow meaningful comparisons across fields and time. Moreover, the distinction between disciplinary and multidisciplinary journals adds valuable nuance. However, some methodological choices, such as the use of a 5-year sliding window to compute trend values, are insufficiently justified and under-explained. The paper also does not fully address disparities in data coverage across disciplines and time, which may affect the reliability of historical comparisons. Finally, minor issues in grammar and clarity reduce the overall polish of the manuscript.

      Evaluation of Findings:

      Overall, the authors have largely succeeded in achieving their stated aims. The findings-such as the sharp rise in internationalization in fields like Physics, and the divergence in interdisciplinarity trends across disciplines-are clearly presented and generally well-supported by the data. The authors effectively demonstrate that scientific journals have not followed a uniform trajectory in terms of structural evolution. However, greater clarity in trend estimation methods and better acknowledgment of dataset limitations would help to further substantiate the conclusions and enhance their generalizability.

      Impact and Relevance:

      This study makes a timely and meaningful contribution to the fields of scientometrics, sociology of science, and science policy. Its combination of scale, historical depth, and field-level comparison offers a useful framework for understanding changes in scientific publishing practices. The entropy-based indicators are simple yet flexible, and the use of open bibliometric data enhances reproducibility and accessibility for future research. Policymakers, journal editors, and researchers interested in publication dynamics will likely find this work informative, and its methods could be applied or extended to other structural dimensions of scholarly communication.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This paper uses large-scale publication data to examine the dynamics of interdisciplinarity and international collaborations in research journals. The main finding is that interdisciplinarity and internationalism have been increasing over the past decades, especially in prestigious general science journals.

      Strengths:

      The paper uses a state-of-the-art large-scale publication database to examine the dynamics of interdisciplinarity and internationalism. The analyses span over a century and in major scientific fields in natural sciences, engineering, and social sciences. The study is well designed and has provided a range of robustness tests to enhance the main findings. The writing is clear and well organized.

      Weaknesses:

      While the research provides interesting perspectives for the reader to learn about the trends of journal preferences, I have a few points for the authors to consider that might help strengthen their work.

      The first thing that comes to mind is the epistemic mechanism of the study. Why should there be a joint discussion combining internationalism and interdisciplinarity? While internationalism is the tendency to form multinational research teams to work on research projects, interdisciplinarity refers to the scope and focus of papers that draw inspiration from multiple fields. These concepts may both fall into the realm of diversity, but it remains unclear if there is any conceptual interplay that underlies the dynamics of their increase in research journals.

      It is also unclear why internationalization is increasing. Although the authors have provided a few prominent examples in physics, such as CERN and LIGO, which are complex and expensive experimental facilities that demand collective efforts and investments from the global scientific community, whether some similar concerns or factors drive the growth of internationalism in other fields remains unknown. I can imagine that these concerns do not always apply in many fields, and the authors need to come up with some case studies in diverse fields with some sociological theory to support their empirical findings.

      The authors use Shannon entropy as a measure of diversity for both internationalism and interdisciplinarity. However, entropy may fail to account for the uneven correlations between fields, and the range of value chances when the number of categories changes. The science of science and scientometrics community has proposed a range of diversity indicators, such as the Rao-Stirling index and its derivatives. One obvious advantage of the RS index is that it explicitly accounts for the heterogeneous connections between fields, and the value ranges from 0 to 1. Using more state-of-the-art metrics to quantify interdisciplinarity may help strengthen the data analytics.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This work investigated whether cytoplasmic poroelastic properties play an important role in cellular mechanical response over length scales and time scales relevant to cell physiology. Overall, the manuscript concludes that intracellular cytosolic flows and pressure gradients are important for cell physiology and that they act of time- and length-scales relevant to mechanotransduction and cell migration.

      Strengths:

      Their approach integrates both computational and experimental methods. The AFM deformation experiments combined with measuring z-position of beads is a challenging yet compelling method to determine poroelastic contributions to mechanical realization.

      The work is quite interesting and will be of high value to the field of cell mechanics and mechanotransduction.

      Weaknesses:

      The weaknesses I noted earlier were adequately addressed in the revised version.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Malboubi et al. present an experimental framework to investigate the rheological properties of the cell cytoplasm. Their findings support a model where the cytoplasm behaves as a poroelastic material governed by Darcy's law. They demonstrate that this poroelastic behavior delays the equilibration of hydrostatic pressure gradients within the cytoplasm over timescales of 1 to 10 seconds following a perturbation, likely due to fluid-solid friction within the cytoplasmic matrix. Furthermore, under sustained perturbations such as depressurization, they reveal that pressure gradients can persist for minutes, which they propose might potentially influence physiological processes like mechanotransduction or cell migration typically happening on these timescales.

      Strengths:

      This article holds significant value within the ongoing efforts of the cell biology and biophysics communities to quantitatively characterize the mechanical properties of cells. The experiments are innovative and thoughtfully contextualized with quantitative estimates and a finite element model that supports the authors' hypotheses.

      Comments & Questions:

      The authors have successfully addressed the questions and comments raised in my previous review, significantly improving the manuscript's depth. Regarding my last question on the predicted saturation of the time lag, the authors propose the interesting hypothesis that the cell cortex becomes dominant at distances beyond 30 microns and plan to test this hypothesis at a later stage.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this delightful study, the authors use local indentation of the cell surface combined with out-of-focus microscopy to measure the rates of pressure spread in the cell and to argue that the results can be explained with the poroelastic model. Osmotic shock that decreases cytoskeletal mesh size supports this notion. Experiments with water injection and water suction further support it, and also, together with a mechanical model and elegant measurements of decreasing fluorescence in the cell 'flashed' by external flow, demonstrate that the membrane is permeable, and that steady flow and pressure gradient can exist in a cell with water source/sink in different locations. Use of blebs as indicators of the internal pressure further supports the notion of differential cytoplasmic pressure.

      Strengths:

      The study is very imaginative, interesting, novel and important.

      Weaknesses: I have two broad critical comments:

      (1) I sense that the authors are correct that the best explanation of their results is the passive poroelastic model. Yet, to be thorough, they have to try to explain the experiments with other models and show why their explanation is parsimonious. For example, one potential explanation could be some mechanosensitive mechanism that does not involve cytoplasmic flow; another could be viscoelastic cytoskeletal mesh, again not involving poroelasticity. I can imagine more possibilities. Basically, be more thorough in the critical evaluation of your results. Besides, discuss potential effect of significant heterogeneity of the cell.

      (2) The study is rich in biophysics but a bit light on chemical/genetic perturbations. It could be good to use low levels of chemical inhibitors for, for example, Arp2/3, PI3K, myosin etc, and see the effect and try to interpret it. Another interesting question - how adhesive strength affects the results. A different interesting avenue - one can perturb aquaporins. Etc. At least one perturbation experiment would be good.

      Comments on revisions: I am satisfied with the revisions

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The study presents significant findings on the role of mitochondrial depletion in axons and its impact on neuronal proteostasis. It effectively demonstrates how the loss of axonal mitochondria and elevated levels of eIF2β contribute to autophagy collapse and neuronal dysfunction. The use of Drosophila as a model organism and comprehensive proteome analysis adds robustness to the findings.

      In this revision, the authors have responded thoughtfully to previous concerns. In particular, they have addressed the need for a quantitative analysis of age-dependent changes in eIF2β and eIF2α. By adding western blot data from multiple time points (7 to 63 days), they show that eIF2β levels gradually increase until middle age, then decline. In milton knockdown flies, this pattern appears shifted, supporting the idea that mitochondrial defects may accelerate aging-related molecular changes. These additions clarify the temporal dynamics of eIF2β and improve the overall interpretation.

      Other updates include appropriate corrections to figures and quantification methods. The authors have also revised some of their earlier mechanistic claims, presenting a more cautious interpretation of their findings.

      Overall, this work provides new insights into how mitochondrial transport defects may influence aging-related proteostasis through eIF2β. The manuscript is now more convincing, and the revisions address the main points raised earlier. I find the updated version much improved.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      In the manuscript, the authors aimed to elucidate the molecular mechanism that explains neurodegeneration caused by the depletion of axonal mitochondria. In Drosophila, starting with siRNA depletion of milton and Miro, the authors attempted to demonstrate that the depletion of axonal mitochondria induces the defect in autophagy. From proteome analyses, the authors hypothesized that autophagy is impacted by the abundance of eIF2β and the phosphorylation of eIF2α. The authors followed up the proteome analyses by testing the effects of eIF2β overexpression and depletion on autophagy. With the results from those experiments, the authors proposed a novel role of eIF2β in proteostasis that underlies neurodegeneration derived from the depletion of axonal mitochondria, which they suggest accelerates age-dependent changes rather than increasing their magnitude.

      Strong caution is necessary regarding the interpretation of translational regulation resulting from the milton KD. The effect of milton KD on translation appears subtle, if present at all, in the puromycin incorporation experiments in both the initial and revised versions. Additionally, the polysome profiling data in the revised manuscript lack the clear resolution for ribosomal subunits, monosomes, and polysomes that is typically expected in publications.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Zhang and colleagues examine neural representations underlying abstract navigation in the entorhinal cortex (EC) and hippocampus (HC) using fMRI. This paper replicates a previously identified hexagonal modulation of abstract navigation vectors in abstract space in EC in a novel task involving navigating in a conceptual Greeble space. In HC, the authors claim to identify a three-fold signal of the navigation angle. They also use a novel analysis technique (spectral analysis) to look at spatial patterns in these two areas and identify phase coupling between HC and EC. Finally, the authors propose an EC-HPC PhaseSync Model to understand how the EC and HC construct cognitive maps. While the wide array of techniques used is impressive and their creativity in analysis is admirable, overall, I found the paper a bit confusing and unconvincing. I recommend a significant rewrite of their paper to motivate their methods and clarify what they actually did and why. The claim of three-fold modulation in HC, while potentially highly interesting to the community, needs more background to motivate why they did the analysis in the first place, more interpretation as to why this would emerge in biology, and more care taken to consider alternative hypotheses seeped in existing models of HC function. I think this paper does have potential to be interesting and impactful, but I would like to see these issues improved first.

      General comments:

      (1) Some of the terminology used does not match the terminology used in previous relevant literature (e.g., sinusoidal analysis, 1D directional domain).

      (2) Throughout the paper, novel methods and ideas are introduced without adequate explanation (e.g., the spectral analysis and three-fold periodicity of HC).

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      The authors report results from behavioral data, fMRI recordings, and computer simulations during a conceptual navigation task. They report 3-fold symmetry in behavioral and simulated model performance, 3-fold symmetry in hippocampal activity, and 6-fold symmetry in entorhinal activity (all as a function of movement directions in conceptual space). The analyses are thoroughly done, and the results and simulations are very interesting.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The aim of the experiment reported in this paper is to examine the nature of the representation of a template of an upcoming target. To this end, participants were presented with compound gratings (consisting of tilted to the right and tilted to the left lines) and were cued to a particular orientation - red left tilt or blue right tilt (counterbalanced across participants). There two directly compared conditions: (i) no ping: where there was a cue, that was followed by a 5.5-7.5s delay, then followed by a target grating in which the cued orientation deviated from the standard 45 degrees; and (ii) ping condition in which all aspects were the same with the only difference that a ping (visual impulse presented for 100ms) was presented after the 2.5 seconds following the cue. There was also a perception task in which only the 45 degrees to the right or to the left lines were presented. It was observed that during the delay, only in the ping condition, were the authors able to decode orientation of the to be reported target using the cross-task generalization. Attention decoding, on the other hand, was decoded in both ping and non-ping conditions. It is concluded that the visual system has two different functional states associated with a template during preparation: a predominantly non-sensory representation for guidance and a latent sensory-like for prospective stimulus processing.

      Strengths:

      There is so much to be impressed with in this report. The writing of the manuscript is incredibly clear. The experimental design is clever and innovative. The analysis is sophisticated and also innovative -the cross-task decoding, the use of Mahalanobis distance as a function of representational similarity, the fact that the question is theoretically interesting, the excellent figures.

      Comments on revisions:

      I have no further comments.

    2. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      This paper discusses how non-sensory and latent, sensory-like attentional templates are represented during attentional preparation. Using multivariate pattern analysis, they found that visual impulses can enhance the decoding generalization from perception to attention tasks in the preparatory stage in the visual cortex. Furthermore, the emergence of the sensory-like template coincided with enhanced information connectivity between V1 and frontoparietal areas and was associated with improved behavioral performance. It is an interesting paper with supporting evidence for the latent, sensory-like attentional template.

      Comments on revisions:

      I appreciate the authors' thoughtful revisions, which have addressed my earlier concerns. I have no further comments.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study evaluates whether species can shift geographically, temporally, or both ways in response to climate change. It also teases out the relative importance of geographic context, temperature variability, and functional traits in predicting the shifts. The study system is large occurrence datasets for dragonflies and damselflies split between two time periods and two continents. Results indicate that more species exhibited both shifts than one or the other (or neither), and that geographic context and temperature variability were more influential than traits. The results have implications for future analyses (e.g. incorporating habitat availability) and for choosing winner and loser species under climate change. The results also seem to support climate vulnerability assessments for species that rely on geographic range size and geospatial climate data layers rather than more detailed information (like demographic rates, abundances, or traits) that may not be so readily available. The methodology would be useful for other taxa and study regions with strong participatory ("citizen") science and extensive occurrence data.

      Strengths:

      This is an organized and well written paper that builds on a popular topic and moves it forward. It has the right idea and approach, and the results are useful answers to the predictions and for conservation planning (i.e. identifying climate winners and losers). There is technical proficiency and analytical rigor driven by an understanding of the data and its limitations.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This paper explores a highly interesting question regarding how species migration success relates to phenology shifts, and it finds a positive relationship. The findings are significant, and the strength of the evidence is solid. However, there are substantial issues with the writing, presentation, and analyses that need to be addressed. First, I disagree with the conclusion that species that don't migrate are "losers" - some species might not migrate simply because they have broad climatic niches and are less sensitive to climate change. Second, the results concerning species' southern range limits could provide valuable insights. These could be used to assess whether sampling bias has influenced the results. If species are truly migrating, we should observe northward shifts in their southern range limits. However, if this is an artifact of increased sampling over time, we would expect broader distributions both north and south. Finally, Figure 1 is missed panel B, which needs to be addressed.

      Comments on revised version:

      The revision has substantially improved the paper.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In their article "Range geography and temperature variability explain cross-continental convergence in range and phenology shifts in a model insect taxon" the authors rigorously investigate the spatial and temporal trends in the occurrence of odonate species and their potential drivers. Specifically, they examine whether species shift their geographic ranges poleward or alter their phenology to cope with changing conditions. Leveraging opportunistic observations of European and North American odonates, they find that species showing significant range shifts also exhibited shifts to earlier emergence. Considering a broad range of potential predictors, their results reveal that geographical factors, but not functional traits, are associated with these shifts.

      Strengths:

      The article addresses an important topic in ecology and conservation that is particularly timely in the face of reports of substantial insects declines in North America and Europe over the past decades. Through data integration the authors leverage the rich natural history record for odonates, broadening the taxonomic scope of analyses of temporal trends in phenology and distribution. The combination of phenological and range shifts in one framework presents an elegant way to reconcile previous findings and informs about the drivers of biodiversity loss.

      Weaknesses:

      To better understand whether species shifting both their ranges and phenology are more successful, or as stated here are 'clear winners', and hence whether those that do neither are more vulnerable would require integrating population trends alongside the discussed response. The ~10% species that have not shifted their distribution or phenology might have not declined in abundance, if they have rapidly adapted to local changes in climatic conditions (i.e. they might show a plastic response). These species might be the real 'winners', while species that have recently shifted their ranges or phenology may eventually reach hard limits. The authors are discussing this limitation but might want to adapt their wording, given the potential for misinterpretation. The finding that species with more northern ranges showed lesser northward shifts would speak to the fact that some species have already reached such a geographical range limit.

      Achievements and impact:

      The results support broad differences in the response of odonate species to climate change, and the prediction that range geography and temperature seasonality are more important predictors of these changes than functional traits. Simultaneously addressing range and phenological shifts highlights that most species exhibit coupled responses but also identifies a significant portion of species that do not respond in these ways that are of critical conservation concern. These results are important for improving forecasts of species' responses to climate change and identifying species of particularly conservation concern. Although not exhaustive regarding abundance trends, the study presents an important step towards a general framework for investigating the drivers of multifaceted species responses.

    1. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript explores behavioral responses of C. elegans to hydrogen sulfide, which is known to exert remarkable effects on animal physiology in a range of contexts. The possibility of genetic and precise neuronal dissection of responses to H2S motivates the study of responses in C. elegans. The revised manuscript does not seem to have significantly addressed what was lacking in the initial version.

      The authors have added further characterization of possible ASJ sensing of H2S by calcium imaging but ASJ does not appear to be directly involved. Genetic and parallel analysis of O2 and CO2 responsive pathways do not reveal further insights regarding potential mechanisms underlying H2S sensing. Gene expression analysis extends prior work. Finally, the authors have examined how H2S-evoked locomotory behavioral responses are affected in mutants with altered stress and detoxification response to H2S, most notably hif-1 and egl-9. These data, while examining locomotion, are more suggestive that observed effects on animal locomotion are secondary to altered organismal toxicity as opposed to specific behavioral responedse

      Overall, the manuscript provides a wide range of intriguing observations, but mechanistic insight or a synthesis of disparate data is lacking.

    2. Reviewer #4 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors establish a behavioral paradigm for avoidance of H2S and conduct a large candidate screen to identify genetic requirements. They follow up by genetically dissecting a large number of implicated pathways - insulin, TGF-beta, oxygen/HIF-1, and mitochondrial ROS, which have varied effects on H2S avoidance. They additionally assay whole-animal gene expression changes induced by varying concentrations and durations of H2S exposure.

      Strengths:

      The implicated pathways are tested extensively through mutants of multiple pathway molecules. The authors address previous reviewer concerns by directly testing the ability of ASJ to respond to H2S via calcium imaging. This allows the authors to revise their previous conclusion and determine that ASJ does not directly respond to H2S and likely does not initiate the behavioral response.

      Weaknesses:

      Despite the authors focus on acute perception of H2S, I don't think the experiments tell us much about perception. I think they indicate pathways that modulate the behavior when disrupted, especially because most manipulations used broadly affect physiology on long timescales. For instance, genetic manipulation of ASJ signaling, oxygen sensing, HIF-1 signaling, mitochondrial function, as well as starvation are all expected to constitutively alter animal physiology, which could indirectly modulate responses to H2S. The authors rule out effects on general locomotion in some cases, but other physiological changes could relatively specifically modulate the H2S response without being involved in its perception.

      I am actually not convinced that H2S is directly perceived by the C. elegans nervous system at all. As far as I can tell, the avoidance behavior could be a response to H2S-induced tissue damage rather than the gas itself.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The revised manuscript addresses several reviewer concerns, and the study continues to provide useful insights into how ZIP10 regulates zinc homeostasis and zinc sparks during fertilization in mice. The authors have improved the clarity of the figures, shifted emphasis in the abstract more clearly to ZIP10, and added brief discussion of ZIP6/ZIP10 interactions and ZIP10's role in zinc spark-calcium oscillation decoupling. However, some critical issues remain only partially addressed.

      (1) Oocyte health confound: The use of Gdf9-Cre deletes ZIP10 during oocyte growth, meaning observed defects could result from earlier disruptions in zinc signaling rather than solely from the absence of zinc sparks at fertilization. The authors acknowledge this and propose transcriptome profiling as a future direction. However, since mRNA levels often do not accurately reflect protein levels and activity in oocytes, transcriptomics may not be particularly informative in this context. Proteomic approaches that directly assess the molecular effects of ZIP10 loss seem more promising. Although current sensitivity limitations make proteomics from small oocyte samples challenging, ongoing improvements in this area may soon allow for more detailed mechanistic insights.

      (2) ZIP6 context and focus: The authors clarified the abstract to emphasize ZIP10, enhancing narrative clarity. This revision is appropriate and appreciated.

      (3) Follicular development effects: The biological consequences of ZIP6 and ZIP10 knockout during folliculogenesis are still unknown. The authors now say these effects will be studied in the future, but this still leaves a major mechanistic gap unaddressed in the current version.

      (4) Zinc spark imaging and probe limitations: The addition of calcium imaging enhances the clarity of Figure 3. However, zinc fluorescence remains inadequate, and the authors depend solely on FluoZin-3AM, a dye known for artifacts and limited ability to detect subcellular labile zinc. The suggestion that C57BL/6J mice may differ from CD1 in vesicle appearance is plausible but does not fully address concerns about probe specificity and resolution. As the authors acknowledge, future studies with more selective probes would increase confidence in both the spatial and quantitative analysis of zinc dynamics.

      (5) Mechanistic insight remains limited: The revised discussion now recognizes the lack of detailed mechanistic understanding but does not significantly expand on potential signaling pathways or downstream targets of ZIP10. The descriptive data are useful, but the inability to pinpoint how ZIP10 mediates zinc spark regulation remains a key limitation. Again, proteomic profiling would probably be more informative than transcriptomic analysis for identifying ZIP10-dependent pathways once technical barriers to low-input proteomics are overcome.

      Overall, the authors have reasonably revised and clarified key points raised by reviewers, and the manuscript now reads more clearly. However, the main limitation, lack of mechanistic insight and the inability to distinguish between developmental and fertilization-stage roles of ZIP10, remains unresolved. These should be explicitly acknowledged when framing the conclusions.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study builds on earlier work showing that early-life odor exposure can trigger glial-mediated pruning of specific olfactory neuron terminals in Drosophila. Moving from indirect to direct functional imaging, the authors show that pruning during a narrow developmental window leads to long-lasting suppression of odor responses in one neuron type (Or42a) but not another (Or43b). The combination of calcium and voltage imaging with connectomic analysis is a strength, though the voltage imaging results are less straightforward to interpret and may not reflect synaptic output changes alone.

      Strengths:

      Biologically, one of the main strengths of this work is the direct comparison between two odor-responsive OSN types that differ in their long-term adaptation to early-life odor exposure. While Or42a OSNs undergo pruning and remain persistently suppressed into late adulthood, Or43b OSNs, which also respond to the same odor, show little lasting change. This contrast not only underscores the cell-type specificity of critical-period plasticity but also points to a potential role of inhibitory network architecture in determining susceptibility. The persistence of the Or42a suppression well beyond the developmental window provides compelling evidence that early glia-mediated pruning can imprint a stable, life-long functional state on selected sensory channels. By situating these functional outcomes within the context of detailed connectomic data, the study offers a framework for linking structural connectivity to long-term sensory coding stability or vulnerability.

      Weaknesses:

      The narrative begins with the absence of changes in PN dendrites and axons. While this establishes specificity, it is a relatively weak starting point compared to the novel OSN functional results. Calcium imaging with GCaMP, though widely used, is an indirect measure of synaptic function, and reduced signals could reflect changes in non-synaptic calcium influx as well as release probability. The interpretation of the voltage imaging results is also unclear: if suppression were solely due to impaired synaptic release, one might expect action potential-evoked voltage signals to remain unchanged. The reported changes raise the possibility of deficits in action potential initiation or propagation, which would shift the mechanistic explanation.

      The difference between Or42a and Or43b OSNs is attributed to varying inhibitory input densities from connectome data, but this remains speculative without functional tests such as manipulating GABA receptor expression in OSNs. In Or43b, there is essentially no strong phenotype, making it premature to ascribe the absence of suppression solely to inhibitory connectivity. Finally, the study does not connect circuit-level changes to behavioral outcomes; assays of odor-guided attraction or discrimination could place the findings in an organismal context. Some introduction material overlaps with the authors' 2024 paper, and the novelty of the present study could be signposted more clearly.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Recent work from the authors identified the synaptic changes and glial reaction that occur during exposure of a Drosophila odorant receptor neuron population to continued exposure of a stimulating odorant. This work markedly advanced our understanding of cellular response to critical periods. This current Advance manuscript carries that work forward and examines the non-autonomous responses to constant odorant exposure. The authors discover that the changes to ORN populations are not accompanied by changes to either PN dendrite or PN axon volume, nor are they concurrent with changes in postsynaptic PN structures. These changes are, however, notable, accompanied by changes in Ca2+ and voltage responses in ORNs. Importantly, this set of responses is specific to the Or42a ORNs (that are highly sensitive to the odorant in question, ethyl butyrate) and not the Or43b ORNs (which respond to ethyl butyrate, but not as drastically). Finally, the authors include connectomics analyses showing that Or43b and Or42a ORNs differ in their synaptic input/output relationships.

      This is an excellent use of the Advance mechanism for the journal, as these are important follow-up findings for the parent story. The non-autonomous effects (or lack thereof) on PNs is an important part of the story, as is the functional response of Or42a ORNs and the differing response of similarly (but not identically) sensitive Or43b ORNs. The experiments are well-conceived, controlled, and conducted. Where the story falters a bit, though, is with the connectomics analysis. The authors show distinct differences between Or43b and Or42b ORN input-output relationships, and suggest that those differences may underlie the differences observed in their response to ethyl butyrate exposure during the critical period. This is certainly a possibility, but as it stands now, it is too disconnected to offer significant proof. There would have to be additional experiments to address this. Right now, the inclusion of the connectomics work feels like a distraction at best, and a complete non sequitur at worst. To be clear, the connectomics work is well done and I have no issues with its validity, but it is not helpful to the central thesis of the work. I would suggest the authors either remove it entirely or strongly rethink how it fits into the paper.

      Major Concerns:

      (1) The examination of PN axon terminals in the MB and LH is interesting, but it is only one possibility. Oftentimes, the volume of neurons remains constant with perturbation, while the synapse number is affected. Figure 1C and E would be greatly helped by examining synapse number (via Brp or Brp-Short) in the PN axons.

      (2) The use of dlg1[4K] is a strong use of a new tool, but the result is surprising. The presynaptic ORN synapse number onto the PNs is notably changed, but that is not reflected in a postsynaptic PSD-95 change. That suggests a compensatory mechanism that the authors might explore. A good proportion of PN puncta should be postsynaptic to those ORNs, so why aren't they adjusted?

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors performed an in-depth analysis of three mouse strains with different levels of susceptibility to metabolic disease. Transcriptomics analyses of relevant deep tissues revealed many strain-specific differences in response to diet. They used gene set enrichment analysis to highlight possible biological pathways that may be involved in obesity and its metabolic consequences. These results were then confirmed using public data in both mice and humans.

      Strengths:

      Overall, this is an interesting study into the biological basis of differing phenotypic outcomes in response to metabolic challenges. The findings uncover several pathways that may shed light on the etiology of obesity and the associated health risks, as well as offer potential therapeutic avenues to prevent them.

      Weaknesses:

      While the experimental design and analysis are mostly good, some aspects of the present paper could be improved.

      (1) Most results are insufficiently described. P-values are almost entirely absent in the main text. Sometimes the significance is indicated in the figures, and other times it is missing. For example, strains are sometimes described as having a higher XYZ, something that is never shown in the plots, and no p-value is ever given.

      (2) While the biological methods are meticulously described, statistical methods are barely mentioned in the methods section. For example, line 578, "multiple comparisons (...) were performed using the glht function of the multcomp package". What is this? What method does it use? And how was mediation analysis done? Line 575 mentions that models were compared, with no description of how this was done. Mentioning the package (or even function) is not sufficient. The package and function are an implementation; they are not the method. The actual method needs to be clearly mentioned and (at least minimally) described, in addition to having the reference for methods that are not ubiquitous (i.e., the Benjamin-Hochberg method is well-enough established to forgo this).

      (3) The methods should also be briefly introduced in the results section before describing the results of those methods.

      (4) The role of immune signaling pathways and associated phenotypes (e.g., monocyte fraction) is over-interpreted. While the differences shown are convincing, they do not convincingly show a role in either obesity or disease. The parsimonious explanation is that such changes happen as a consequence of dyslipidemia rather than a cause. It is possible that these pathways play a more direct role in this, but the authors do not present compelling evidence of this, and, failing this, the language in the text needs to be toned down.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      This study investigated changes in metabolic health across three genetically diverse mouse strains (NZO/HlLtJ, C57BL/6J mice, CAST/EiJ) that were fed either control or high-fat high-sucrose diets. The strength of this study is the depth of metabolic phenotyping, the use of both male and female mice, and the multi-tissue metabolic analysis, including metabolic and gene expression analysis in pancreatic islets, kidney, muscle, heart, liver, and adipose tissue.

      Weaknesses include that only three mouse strains were included in this comparison, particularly given that similar comparisons have been published in the past and that the Jax lab has access to a wide range of mouse strains with diverse genetic backgrounds. Why were CAST mice included over (for example) BALB/c mice that are more commonly used in metabolic studies and are well known to show protection against diet-induced metabolic disease? Furthermore, the feeding regime was limited to 9 weeks, which may not be sufficient to evoke pronounced metabolic remodelling.

      NZO mice are well known to develop obesity. However, only approximately 50% develop type 2 diabetes and beta-cell dysfunction. How were these mice selected in the study? The results state 'Most of the male NZO mice and a few female mice displayed overt diabetes', suggesting that all mice were included irrespective of their diabetic phenotype. More information on the rationale for this is required.

      The transcriptomics data are presented in a convoluted way. As a reader, the main interest would be to determine the differences in diet-induced adaptations within each strain (e.g., why are CAST mice resistant to diet-induced metabolic defects?). However, the way Figure 4 is currently presented does not allow for this. Instead, the data are 'compressed' by looking at general changes in metabolic pathways between tissues in all three mouse strains. In addition, Figure 4E does not show the directionality of the responses within each pathway. For example, are the metabolism and inflammation pathways suppressed or activated? While more data is shown for adipose tissue, this is not sufficient.

      Currently, the metabolic cage data are separated by diet within the main figures. However, given that the diet effect is the major comparison, this needs to be rearranged, and strain differences within each diet could be shown within the supplement.

      The graphs lack labelling throughout to specify which lines/bars represent which strains and diets. This is particularly the case in the metabolic cage analysis.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Using three strains of mice that are founders of the Diversity Outbred Population of mice, this paper attempts to identify key genetic drivers of obesity and metabolic dysfunction. Through a series of in-depth phenotyping experiments, the authors describe substantial differences in the propensity of these strains to develop obesity and complications associated with obesity. The key here was the careful selection of these strains, as they mostly spanned the spectrum of minor susceptibility (C57BL/6J), major susceptibility (NZO/HILtJ), and complete resistance to diet-induced obesity (CAST/EiJ). This was done in the setting of both a normal diet and a high-fat diet. These studies identified that one of the most transcriptionally activated tissues in this setting across the strains was adipose tissue. Furthermore, a critical pathway in adipose tissue that inferred protection against obesity in the CAST strain was related to immune infiltration. Subsequently, the authors extended their studies into this phenotype using their existing access to the vast array of genetic information from the DO datasets. From this analysis, it was identified that a key region on Chr19 had a significant influence on this phenotype, and subsequent work investigated the potentially causal genes. Overall, this study encompasses an impressive amount of in vivo and genetic work and identifies some new gene regulators associated with obesity complications, which warrant further investigation.

      Strengths:

      This study engages multiple mouse lines with diet intervention, as well as powerful genetic mapping tools to isolate genetic drivers of various obesity related phenotypes. The animal studies are thorough and well performed, and they also include detailed omics analysis of several tissues. Subsequent genetic mapping uses some of the world's most powerful preclinical genetic approaches, and findings identify some novel genes associated with obesity.

      Weaknesses:

      These mouse lines and hybrid genetic screens in this paper have been used for some years now to map similar phenotypes, so in that sense, the approach is not overly novel. Moreover, the most compelling and exciting part of the study, in this reviewer's opinion, is the DO mapping of the immune phenotype in adipose tissue. In some ways, the authors could have potentially come to this same conclusion without the need to perform the mouse studies in the three different strains (other than the nice storytelling of finding the phenotype initially in CAST). Likewise, with this being the most novel aspect of the study, it was a shame that the genes identified at Chr19 were not investigated in more detail in the manuscript, other than just some associative outcomes in mice and humans. It would have been pleasing to see some attempt to validate one of these genes in a mouse model, linking it to either obesity or immune phenotypes in WAT.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Yamazaki et al. conducted multiple microscopy-based GFP localization screens, from which they identified proteins that are associated with PM/cell wall damage stress response. Specifically, the authors identified that bud-localized TMD-containing proteins and endocytotic proteins are associated with PM damage stress. The authors further demonstrated that polarized exocytosis and CME are temporally coupled in response to PM damage, and CME is required for polarized exocytosis and the targeting of TMD-containing proteins to the damage site. From these results, the authors proposed a model that CME delivers TMD-containing repair proteins between the bud tip and the damage site.

      Strengths:

      Overall, this is a well-written manuscript, and the experiments are well-conducted. The authors identified many repair proteins and revealed the temporal coordination of different categories of repair proteins. Furthermore, the authors demonstrated that CME is required for targeting of repair proteins to the damage site, as well as cellular survival in response to stress related to PM/cell wall damage. Although the roles of CME and bud-localized proteins in damage repair are not completely new to the field, this work does have conceptual advances by identifying novel repair proteins and proposing the intriguing model that the repairing cargoes are shuttled between the bud tip and the damaged site through coupled exocytosis and endocytosis.

      Weaknesses:

      While the results presented in this manuscript are convincing, they might not be sufficient to support some of the authors' claims. Especially in the last two result sessions, the authors claimed CME delivers TMD-containing repair proteins from the bud tip to the damage site. The model is no doubt highly possible based on the data, but caveats still exist. For example, the repair proteins might not be transported from one localization to another localization, but are degraded and resynthesized. Although the Gal-induced expression system can further support the model to some extent, I think more direct verification (such as FLIP or photo-convertible fluorescence tags to distinguish between pre-existing and newly synthesized proteins) would significantly improve the strength of evidence.

      Major experiment suggestions:

      (1) The authors may want to provide more direct evidence for "protein shuttling" and for excluding the possibility that proteins at the bud are degraded and synthesized de novo near the damage site. For example, if the authors could use FLIP to bleach bud-localized fluorescent proteins, and the damaged site does not show fluorescent proteins upon laser damage, this will strongly support the authors' model. Alternatively, the authors could use photo-convertible tags (e.g., Dendra) to differentiate between pre-existing repair proteins and newly synthesized proteins.

      (2) In line with point 1, the authors used Gal-inducible expression, which supported their model. However, the author may need to show protein abundance in galactose, glucose, and upon PM damage. Western blot would be ideal to show the level of full-length proteins, or whole-cell fluorescence quantification can also roughly indicate the protein abundance. Otherwise, we cannot assume that the tagged proteins are only expressed when they are growing in galactose-containing media.

      (3) Similarly, for Myo2 and Exo70 localization in CME mutants (Figure 4), it might be worth doing a western or whole-cell fluorescence quantification to exclude the caveat that CME deficiency might affect protein abundance or synthesis.

      (4) From the authors' model in Figure 7, it looks like the repair proteins contribute to bud growth. Does laser damage to the mother cell prevent bud growth due to the reduction of TMD-containing repair proteins at the bud? If the authors could provide evidence for that, it would further support the model.

      (5) Is the PM repair cell-cycle-dependent? For example, would the recruitment of repair proteins to the damage site be impaired when the cells are under alpha-factor arrest?

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      This paper remarkably reveals the identification of plasma membrane repair proteins, revealing spatiotemporal cellular responses to plasma membrane damage. The study highlights a combination of sodium dodecyl sulfate (SDS) and lase for identifying and characterizing proteins involved in plasma membrane (PM) repair in Saccharomyces cerevisiae. From 80 PM, repair proteins that were identified, 72 of them were novel proteins. The use of both proteomic and microscopy approaches provided a spatiotemporal coordination of exocytosis and clathrin-mediated endocytosis (CME) during repair. Interestingly, the authors were able to demonstrate that exocytosis dominates early and CME later, with CME also playing an essential role in trafficking transmembrane-domain (TMD) containing repair proteins between the bud tip and the damage site.

      Weaknesses/limitations:

      (1) Why are the authors saying that Pkc1 is the best characterized repair protein? What is the evidence?

      (2) It is unclear why the authors decided on the C-terminal GFP-tagged library to continue with the laser damage assay, exclusively the C-terminal GFP-tagged library. Potentially, this could have missed N-terminal tag-dependent localizations and functions and may have excluded functionally important repair proteins.

      (3) The use of SDS and laser damage may bias toward proteins responsive to these specific stresses, potentially missing proteins involved in other forms of plasma membrane injuries, such as mechanical, osmotic, etc.). SDS stress is known to indirectly induce oxidative stress and heat-shock responses.

      (4) It is unclear what the scale bars of Figures 3, 5, and 6 are. These should be included in the figure legend.

      (5) Figure 4 should be organized to compare WT vs. mutant, which would emphasize the magnitude of impairment.

      (6) It would be interesting to expand on possible mechanisms for CME-mediated sorting and retargeting of TMD proteins, including a speculative model.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This work aims to understand how cells repair damage to the plasma membrane (PM). This is important, as failure to do so will result in cell lysis and death. Therefore, this is an important fundamental question with broad implications for all eukaryotic cells. Despite this importance, there are relatively few proteins known to contribute to this repair process. This study expands the number of experimentally validated PM from 8 to 80. Further, they use precise laser-induced damage of the PM/cell wall and use live-cell imaging to track the recruitment of repair proteins to these damage sites. They focus on repair proteins that are involved in either exocytosis or clathrin-mediated endocytosis (CME) to understand how these membrane remodeling processes contribute to PM repair. Through these experiments, they find that while exocytosis and CME both occur at the sites of PM damage, exocytosis predominates in the early stages of repairs, while CME predominates in the later stages of repairs. Lastly, they propose that CME is responsible for diverting repair proteins localized to the growing bud cell to the site of PM damage.

      Strengths:

      The manuscript is very well written, and the experiments presented flow logically. The use of laser-induced damage and live-cell imaging to validate the proteome-wide screen using SDS-induced damage strengthens the role of the identified candidates in PM/cell wall repair.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Could the authors estimate the fraction of their candidates that are associated with cell wall repair versus plasma membrane repair? Understanding how many of these proteins may be associated with the repair of the cell wall or PM may be useful for thinking about how these results are relevant to systems that do or do not have a cell wall. Perhaps this is already in their GO analysis, but I don't see it mentioned in the manuscript.

      (2) Do the authors identify actin cable-associated proteins or formin regulators associated with sites of PM damage? Prior work from the senior author (reference 26) shows that the formin Bnr1 relocalizes to sites of PM damage, so it would be interesting if Bnr1 and its regulators (e.g., Bud14, Smy1, etc) are recruited to these sites as well. These may play a role in directing PM repair proteins (see more below).

      (3) Do the authors suspect that actin cables play a role in the relocalization of material from the bud tip to PM damage sites? They mention that TMD proteins are secretory vesicle cargo (lines 134-143) and that Myo2 localizes to damage sites. Together, this suggests a possible role for cable-based transport of repair proteins. While this may be the focus of future work, some additional discussion of the role of cables would strengthen their proposed mechanism (steps 3 and 4 in Figure 7).

      (4) Lines 248-249: I find the rationale for using an inducible Gal promoter here unclear. Some clarification is needed.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      (1) In this study, the authors aimed at characterizing Huntington's Disease (HD) - related microstructural abnormalities in the basal ganglia and thalami as revealed using Soma and Neurite Density Imaging (SANDI) indices (apparent soma density, apparent soma size, extracellular water signal fraction, extracellular diffusivity, apparent neurite density, fractional anisotropy and mean diffusivity).

      (2) The study implements a novel biophysical diffusion model that extends up-to-date methodologies and presents a significant potential for quantifying neurodegenerative processes of the grey matter of the human brain in vivo. The authors comment on the usefulness of this technique in other pathologies, but they exemplify it only with multiple sclerosis. Further development of this, building evidence, should be provided.

      (3) The study found that HD-related neurodegeneration in the striatum accounted significantly for striatal atrophy and correlated with motor impairments. HD was associated with reduced soma density, increased apparent soma size, and extracellular signal fraction in the basal ganglia, but not in the thalami. Additionally, these effects were larger at the manifest stage.

      (4) The results of this work demonstrate the impact of HD on the basal ganglia and thalami, which can be further explored as a non-invasive biomarker of disease progression. Additionally, the study shows that SANDI can be used to explore grey matter microstructure in a variety of neurological conditions.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors aimed to investigate whether advanced microstructural diffusion MRI modeling using the SANDI framework could reveal clinically relevant tissue alterations in the subcortical structures of individuals with Huntington's disease (HD). Specifically, they sought to determine if SANDI-derived parameters-such as soma density, soma size, and extracellular diffusivity-could detect abnormalities in both manifest and premanifest HD stages, complement standard MRI biomarkers (e.g., volume, MD), and correlate with disease burden and motor impairment. Through this, they hoped to demonstrate the feasibility and added biological specificity of SANDI for early detection and characterization of HD pathology.

      Strengths:

      (1) Novelty and relevance:

      This is, to the best of my knowledge, the first clinical deployment of SANDI in HD, offering more biophysically interpretable and specific imaging biomarkers than standard DTI or volumetric features.

      (2) More specific microstructural insight: Traditional approaches have used volumetric features (e.g., striatal volume loss) or DTI metrics (like FA and MD), which are indirect and non-specific markers. They can indicate something is "wrong" but not what is wrong.

      (3) SANDI parameters permit establishing clearer links with microstructure:

      o Apparent soma density (fis): proxy for neuronal/glial cell body density.

      o Apparent soma size (rs): reflects possible gliagl hypertrophy or neuronal shrinkage.

      o Neurite density (fin): linked to dendritic/axonal integrity.

      o Extracellular fraction and diffusivity: sensitive to edema, gliosis, and tissue loss.

      In this way, a decrease in soma density can be related to neural loss (e.g., medium spiny neurons), and an increase in soma size and extracellular fraction could be related to glial reactivity (astrocytes, microglia). This enables differentiating between atrophy due to neuron loss vs reactive gliosis, which volumetrics or DTI cannot do.

      (4) Integration of modalities: The inclusion of motor impairment (Q-Motor), HD-ISS staging, and multi-compartment diffusion modeling is a methodological strength.

      (5) Early detection potential: SANDI metrics showed abnormalities in premanifest HD, sometimes even when volume loss was mild or absent. This suggests the potential for earlier, more sensitive biomarkers of disease progression.

      (6) Predictive power: Regression models showed that SANDI metrics explained up to 63% of the variance in striatal volumes in HD. And this correlated strongly with motor impairment and disease burden (CAP100). This shows they are not just redundant with volume or DTI, but they are complementary and potentially more mechanistically meaningful.

      Weaknesses:

      Certain aspects of the study would benefit from clarification:

      (1) Scanner and acquisition consistency: While HD data are from the WAND study, it is not clear whether controls were scanned on the same scanner or protocol. Given the use of model-derived metrics (especially SANDI), differences in scanner or acquisition could introduce confounds. Also, although it offers novel and biologically informative markers, widespread clinical translation still faces hurdles. For instance, the study used a 3T Connectom scanner (300mT/m gradients), which is not widely available. Reproduction of these results in standard 3T clinical scanners would be a great addition, in scenarios with lower resolution, less precise parameter recovery, and longer scans if SNR needs to be maintained.

      (2) HD-ISS staging and group comparisons:<br /> a) Only 26-27 out of 56 gene-positive participants could be assigned HD-ISS stages, and none were classified into stages 0 or 4.

      b) Visual overlap between stages 1 and 2 in behavioral and imaging features suggests that staging-based group separation may not be robust.

      c) The above may lead to claims based on progression across HD-ISS stages to be overinterpreted or underpowered

      (3) Regression modeling choices:<br /> a) SANDI metrics included in the models differ between HC and HD groups, reducing comparability.

      b) The potential impact of multicollinearity (e.g., between fis and rs) is not discussed.

      c) Beta coefficients could reflect model instability or parameter degeneracy rather than true biological effects.

      These issues do not undermine the study's main conclusions, which effectively demonstrate the feasibility and initial clinical relevance of applying SANDI to HD. Nonetheless, addressing them more thoroughly would enhance the clarity and interpretability of the manuscript.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Ioakeimidis and colleagues studied microstructural abnormalities in N=56 Huntington's disease (HD) patients compared to N=57 normative controls. The authors used a powerful MRI Connectom scanner and applied the SANDI model to estimate the soma size, neurite size, soma density, and extracellular fraction in key subcortical nuclei related to HD. In the striatum, they found decreased soma density and increased soma size, which also seemed to become more pronounced in advanced HD individuals in the final exploratory analyses. The authors conducted important analyses to find whether the SANDI measures correlate with clinical scores (i.e., QMotor) and whether the variance of the striatal volume is explained by the SANDI measures. They found a relationship between SANDI measures for both.

      Strengths:

      The study is both innovative and of high interest for the HD community. The authors provide a rich pool of statistical analyses and results that anticipate the questions that may emerge in the HD research community. Statistics are carefully chosen and image processing is done with state-of-the-art methods and tools. The sample size gives sufficient credibility to the findings. Altogether, I think this study sets a milestone in the attempts of the HD community to understand neuropathological processes with non-invasive methods, and extends the current knowledge of microstructural anomalies identified in HD with diffusion MRI. More importantly, the newly identified anomalies in soma size and soma density open new avenues for studying these biological effects further and perhaps developing these biomarkers for use in clinical trials.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) An important question is whether the SANDI measures, which require an expensive scanner and elaborate processing, are better biomarkers than the more traditional DTI measures. Can the authors compare the effect size of FA/MD with SANDI measures? In some of the plots and tables, FA/MD seem to have comparable, if not higher, correlations with QMotor or CAP scores. On the same vein, it is unclear whether DTI measures were included in hierarchical stepwise regression. I wonder if the stepwise models may have picked up FA/MD instead of SANDI measures if they are given a chance. Overall, I hope the authors can discuss their findings also in this light of cost vs. benefit of adopting SANDI in future studies, which is an important topic for clinical trials.

      (2) Similar to the above point, it is very important to consider how strong the biomarking signal is from SANDI measures compared to the good old striatal volume. Some plots seem to indicate that volumes still have the highest correlation with QMotor and the highest effect size in group comparisons. It would be helpful for the community to know where the new SANDI measures stand compared to the most typically used volumes in terms of effect size.

      (3) The diffusion measures are inevitably correlated to some degree. Please provide a correlation matrix in the supplementary material, including all DWI measures, to enable readers to better understand how similar SANDI measures are to each other or vs. other DTI measures. Perhaps adding volumes to this correlation matrix may also be a good future reference.

      (4) ISS stages:

      a) The online ISS calculator requires cut-offs derived from the longitudinal Freesurfer pipeline, while the authors do not have longitudinal data. Thus, the ISS classification might be inaccurate to some degree if the authors used the FS cross-sectional pipeline. Please review this issue and see if updated cut-offs should be used to classify participants.

      b) Were there really no participants with ISS 0 among the 56 HD individuals? Please clarify in the manuscript.

      (5) A note on terminology that might be confusing to some readers. According to the creators of ISS, the ISS stages are created for research only; they are not used or applied in the clinic. On the other hand, the terms "premanifest" and "manifest" have a clinical meaning, typically based on the diagnostic confidence level. The assignment of ISS0-1 to premanifest and ISS2-3 to manifest may create some non-trivial confusion, if not opposition, in some segments of the HD community. The authors can keep their current terminology, but will need to at least clarify to the reader that this assignment is speculative, does not fully match the clinically-based categories, and should not be confused with similarly named groups in the previous literature.

    1. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      This is a fundamentally important study presenting cryo-EM structures of a human small conductance calcium-activated potassium (SK2) channel in the absence and presence of calcium, or with interesting pharmacological probes bound, including the bee toxin apamin, a small molecule inhibitor, and a small molecule activator. As efforts to solve structures of the wild-type hSK2 channel were unsuccessful, the authors engineered a chimera containing the intracellular domain of the SK4 channel, the subtype of SK channel that was successfully solved in a previous study (reference 13). The authors present many new and exciting findings, including opening of an internal gate (similar to SK4), for the first time resolving the S3-S4 linker sitting atop the outer vestibule of the pore and unanticipated plasticity of the ion selectivity filter, and the binding sites for apamin, one new small molecule inhibitor and another small molecule activator. Appropriate functional data are provided to frame interpretations arising from the structures of the chimeric protein; the data are compelling, the interpretations are sound, and the writing is clear. This high-quality study will be of interest to membrane protein structural biologists, ion channel biophysicists, and chemical biologists, and will be valuable for future drug development targeting SK channels.

      Comments on revisions:

      The authors have done a nice job of revising the manuscript to address the issues raised in the first round of review and I have no further suggestions.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      While the structure of the melibiose permease in both outward and inward-facing forms has been solved previously, there remain unanswered questions regarding its mechanism. Hariharan et al set out to address this with further crystallographic studies complemented with ITC and hydrogen-deuterium exchange (HDX) mass spectrometry. They first report 4 different crystal structures of galactose derivatives to explore molecular recognition, showing that the galactose moiety itself is the main source of specificity. Interestingly, they observe a water-mediated hydrogen bonding interaction with the protein and suggest that this water molecule may be important in binding.

      The results from the crystallography appear sensible, though the resolution of the data is low, with only the structure with NPG better than 3Å. However, it is a bit difficult to understand what novel information is being brought out here and what is known about the ligands. For instance, are these molecules transported by the protein or do they just bind? They measure the affinity by ITC, but draw very few conclusions about how the affinity correlates with the binding modes. Can the protein transport the trisaccharide raffinose?

      The HDX also appears to be well done; however, in the manuscript as written, it is difficult to understand how this relates to the overall mechanism of the protein and the conformational changes that the protein undergoes.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      This manuscript from Hariharan, Shi, Viner, and Guan presents x-ray crystallographic structures of membrane protein MelB and HDX-MS analysis of ligand-induced dynamics. This work improves on the resolution of previously published structures, introduces further sugar-bound structures, and utilises HDX to explore in further depth the previously observed positive cooperatively to cotransported cation Na+. The work presented here builds on years of previous study and adds substantial new details into how Na+ binding facilitates melibiose binding and deepens the fundamental understanding of the molecular basis underlying the symport mechanism of cation-coupled transporters. However, the presentation of the data lacks clarity, and in particular, the HDX-MS data interpretation requires further explanation in both methodology and discussion.

      Comments on Crystallography and biochemical work:

      (1) It is not clear what Figure 2 is comparing. The text suggests this figure is a comparison of the lower resolution structure to the structure presented in this work; however, the figure legend does not mention which is which, and both images include a modelled water molecule that was not assigned due to poor resolution previously, as stated by the authors, in the previously generated structure. This figure should be more clearly explained.

      (2) It is slightly unclear what the ITC measurements add to this current manuscript. The authors comment that raffinose exhibiting poor binding affinity despite having more sugar units is surprising, but it is not surprising to me. No additional interactions can be mapped to these units on their structure, and while it fits into the substrate binding cavity, the extra bulk of additional sugar units is likely to reduce affinity. In fact, from their listed ITC measurements, this appears to be the trend. Additionally, the D59C mutant utilised here in structural determination is deficient in sodium/cation binding. The reported allostery of sodium-sugar binding will likely influence the sugar binding motif as represented by these structures. This is clearly represented by the authors' own ITC work. The ITC included in this work was carried out on the WT protein in the presence of Na+. The authors could benefit from clarifying how this work fits with the structural work or carrying out ITC with the D59C mutant, or additionally, in the absence of sodium.

      Comments on HDX-MS work:

      While the use of HDX-MS to deepen the understanding of ligand allostery is an elegant use of the technique, this reviewer advises the authors to refer to the Masson et al. (2019) recommendations for the HDX-MS article (https://doi.org/10.1038/s41592-019-0459-y) on how to best present this data. For example:

      (1) The Methodology includes a lipid removal step. Based on other included methods, I assumed that the HDX-MS was being carried out in detergent-solubilised protein samples. I therefore do not see the need for a lipid removal step that is usually included for bilayer reconstituted samples. I note that this methodology is the same as previously used for MelB. It should be clarified why this step was included, if it was in fact used, aka, further details on the sample preparation should be included.

      (2) A summary of HDX conditions and results should be given as recommended, including the mean peptide length and average redundancy per state alongside other included information such as reaction temperature, sequence coverage, etc., as prepared for previous publications from the authors, i.e., Hariharan et al., 2024.

      (3) Uptake plots per peptide for the HDX-MS data should be included as supporting information outside of the few examples given in Figure 6.

      (4) A reference should be given to the hybrid significance testing method utilised. Additionally, as stated by Hageman and Weis (2019) (doi:10.1021/acs.analchem.9b01325), the use of P < 0.05 greatly increases the likelihood of false positive ΔD identifications. While the authors include multiple levels of significance, what they refer to as high and lower significant results, this reviewer understands that working with dynamic transporters can lead to increased data variation; a statement of why certain statistical criteria were chosen should be included, and possibly accompanied by volcano plots. The legend of Figure 6 should include what P value is meant by * and ** rather than statistically significant and highly statistically significant.

      (5) Line 316 states a significant difference in seen in dynamics, how is significance measured here? There is no S.D. given in Table S4. Can the authors further comment on the potential involvement in solvent accessibility and buried helices that might influence the overall dynamics outside of their role in sugar vs sodium binding? An expected low rate of exchange suggests that dynamics are likely influenced by solvent accessibility or peptide hydrophobicity? The increased dynamics at peptides covering the Na binding site on overall more dynamic helices suggests that there is no difference between the dynamics of each site.

      (6) Previously stated HDX-MS results of MelB (Hariharan et al., 2024) state that the transmembrane helices are less dynamic than polypeptide termini and loops with similar distributions across all transmembrane bundles. The previous data was obtained in the presence of sodium. Does this remove the difference in dynamics in the sugar-binding helices and the cation-binding helices? Including this comparison would support the statement that the sodium-bound MelB is more stable than the Apo state, along with the lack of deprotection observed in the differential analysis.

      (7) Have the authors considered carrying out an HDX-MS comparison between the WT and the D59C mutant? This may provide some further information on the WT structure (particularly a comparison with sugar-bound). This could be tied into a nice discussion of their structural data.

      (8) Have the authors considered utilising Li+ to infer how cation selectivity impacts the allostery? Do they expect similar stabilisation of a higher-affinity sugar binding state with all cations?

      (9) MD of MelB suggests all transmembrane helices are reorientated during substrate translocation, yet substrate and cotransporter ligand binding only significantly impacts a small number of helices. Can the authors comment on the ensemble of states expected from each HDX experiment? The data presented here instead shows overall stabilisation of the transporter. This data can be compared to that of HDX on MFS sugar cation symporter XylE, where substrate binding induces a transition to OF state. There is no discussion of how this HDX data compares to previous MFS sugar transporter HDX. The manuscript could benefit from this comparison rather than a comparison to LacY. It is unlikely that there are universal mechanisms that can be inferred even from these model proteins. Highlighting differences instead between these transport systems provides broader insights into this protein class. Doi: 10.1021/jacs.2c06148 and 10.1038/s41467-018-06704-1.

      (10) Additionally, the recent publication of SMFS data (by the authors: doi:10.1016/j.str.2022.11.011) states the following: "In the presence of either melibiose or a coupling Na+-cation, however, MelB increasingly populates the mechanically less stable state which shows a destabilized middle-loop C3." And "In the presence of both substrate and co-substrate, this mechanically less stable state of MelB is predominant.". It would benefit the authors to comment on these data in contrast to the HDX obtained here. Additionally, is the C3 loop covered, and does it show the destabilization suggested by these studies? HDX can provide a plethora of results that are missing from the current analysis on ligand allostery. The authors instead chose to reference CD and thermal denaturation methods as comparisons.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The melibiose permease from Salmonella enterica serovar Typhimurium (MelBSt) is a member of the Major Facilitator Superfamily (MFS). It catalyzes the symport of a galactopyranoside with Na⁺, H⁺, or Li⁺, and serves as a prototype model system for investigating cation-coupled transport mechanisms. In cation-coupled symporters, a coupling cation typically moves down its electrochemical gradient to drive the uphill transport of a primary substrate; however, the precise role and molecular contribution of the cation in substrate binding and translocation remain unclear. In a prior study, the authors showed that the binding affinity for melibiose is increased in the presence of Na+ by about 8-fold, but the molecular basis for the cooperative mechanism remains unclear. The objective of this study was to better understand the allosteric coupling between the Na+ and melibiose binding sites. To verify the sugar-recognition specific determinants, the authors solved the outward-facing crystal structures of a uniport mutant D59C with four sugar ligands containing different numbers of monosaccharide units (α-NPG, melibiose, raffinose, or α-MG). The structure with α-NPG bound has improved resolution (2.7 Å) compared to a previously published structure and to those with other sugars. These structures show that the specificity is clearly directed toward the galactosyl moiety. However, the increased affinity for α-NPG involves its hydrophobic phenyl group, positioned at 4 Å-distance from the phenyl group of Tyr26 forms a strong stacking interaction. Moreover, a water molecule bound to OH-4 in the structure with α-NPG was proposed to contribute to the sugar recognition and appears on the pathway between the two specificity-determining pockets. Next, the authors analyzed by hydrogen-to-deuterium exchange coupled to mass spectrometry (HDX-MS) the changes in structural dynamics of the transporter induced by melibiose, Na+, or both. The data support the conclusion that the binding of the coupling cation at a remote location stabilizes the sugar-binding residues to switch to a higher-affinity state. Therefore, the coupling cation in this symporter was proposed to be an allosteric activator.

      Strengths:

      (1) The manuscript is generally well written.

      (2) This study builds on the authors' accumulated knowledge of the melibiose permease and integrates structural and HDX-MS analyses to better understand the communication between the sodium ion and sugar binding sites. A high sequence coverage was obtained for the HDX-MS data (86-87%), which is high for a membrane protein.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) I am not sure that the resolution of the structure (2.7 Å) is sufficiently high to unambiguously establish the presence of a water molecule bound to OH-4 of the α-NPG sugar. In Figure 2, the density for water 1 is not obvious to me, although it is indeed plausible that water mediates the interaction between OH4/OH6 and the residues Q372 and T373.

      (2) Site-directed mutagenesis could help strengthen the conclusions of the authors. Would the mutation(s) of Q372 and/or T373 support the water hypothesis by decreasing the affinity for sugars? Mutations of Thr 121, Arg 295, combined with functional and/or HDX-MS analyses, may also help support some of the claims of the authors regarding the allosteric communication between the two substrate-binding sites.

      (3) The main conclusion of the authors is that the binding of the coupling cation stabilizes those dynamic sidechains in the sugar-binding pocket, leading to a high-affinity state. This is visible when comparing panels c and a from Figure S5. However, there is both increased protection (blue, near the sugar) and decreased protection in other areas (red). The latter was less commented, could the increased flexibility in these red regions facilitate the transition between inward- and outward-facing conformations? The HDX changes induced by the different ligands were compared to the apo form (see Figure S5). It might be worth it for data presentation to also analyze the deuterium uptake difference by comparing the conditions sodium ion+melibiose vs melibiose alone. It would make the effect of Na+ on the structural dynamics of the melibiose-bound transporter more visible. Similarly, the deuterium uptake difference between sodium ion+melibiose vs sodium ion alone could be analyzed too, in order to plot the effect of melibiose on the Na+-bound transporter.

      (4) For non-specialists, it would be beneficial to better introduce and explain the choice of using D59C for the structural analyses.

      (5) In Figure 5a, deuterium changes are plotted as a function of peptide ID number. It is hardly informative without making it clearer which regions it corresponds to. Only one peptide is indicated (213-226), I would recommend indicating more of them in areas where deuterium changes are substantial.

      (6) From prior work of the authors, melibiose binding also substantially increases the affinity of the sodium ion. Can the authors interpret this observation based on the HDX data?

    1. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      General comment:

      This is a very valuable and unique comparative study. An excellent combination of scanning and histological data from three different species is presented. Obtaining the material for such a comparative study is never trivial. The study presents new data and thus provides the basis for an in-depth discussion about chondrichthyan mineralised skeletal tissues.

      Comments on previous revisions:

      The manuscript has been revised and improved and can be published. A very nice manuscript, indeed. My only recommendation (point of discussion, not a requirement) would still be to think about the claim of paedomorphosis in a holocephalan.

      Within the chondrichthyes, how distant holocephali are in relation to elasmobranchii remains uncertain, holocephali are quite a specialised group. Holocephali are also older than Batoidea and Selachii. As paedomorphosis is a derived character, I imagine it is difficult to establish that development in an extant holocephalan is derived compared to development in elasmobranchii. If this type of development would have been typical for the "older" holocephali it would not be paedomorphic. Also, the uncertainty how distant holocephali are from elasmobranchii makes it difficult to identify paedomorphosis with reference to chondrichthyes.

      [Editors note: the authors have made further revisions in response to the previous reviews.]

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Participants learned a graph-based representation, but, contrary to the hypotheses, failed to show neural replay shortly after. This prompted a critical inquiry into temporally delayed linear modeling (TDLM)--the algorithm used to find replay. First, it was found that TDLM detects replay only at implausible numbers of replay events per second. Second, it detects replay-to-cognition correlations only at implausible densities. Third, there are concerning baseline shifts in sequenceness across participants. Fourth, spurious sequences arise in control conditions without a ground truth signal. Fifth, when reframing simulations previously published, similar evidence is apparent.

      Strengths:

      (1) This work is meticulous and meets a high standard of transparency and open science, with preregistration, code and data sharing, external resources such as a GUI with the task and material for the public.

      (2) The writing is clear, balanced, and matter-of-fact.

      (3) By injecting visually evoked empirical data into the simulation, many surface-level problems are avoided, such as biological plausibility and questions of signal-to-noise ratio.

      (4) The investigation of sequenceness-to-cognition correlations is an especially useful add-on because much of the previous work uses this to make key claims about replay as a mechanism.

      Weaknesses:

      Many of the weaknesses are not so much flaws in the analyses, but shortcomings when it comes to interpretation and a lack of making these findings as useful as they could be.

      (1) I found the bigger picture analysis to be lacking. Let us take stock: in other work, during active cognition, including at least one study from the Authors, TDLM shows significance sequenceness. But the evidence provided here suggests that even very strong localizer patterns injected into the data cannot be detected as replay except at implausible speeds. How can both of these things be true? Assuming these analyses are cogent, do these findings not imply something more destructive about all studies that found positive results with TDLM?

      (2) All things considered, TDLM seems like a fairly 'vanilla' and low-assumption algorithm for finding event sequences. It is hard to see intuitively what the breaking factor might be; why do the authors think ground truth patterns cannot be detected by this GLM-based framework at reasonable densities?

      (3) Can the authors sketch any directions for alternative methods? It seems we need an algorithm that outperforms TDLM, but not many clues or speculations are given as to what that might look like. Relatedly, no technical or "internal" critique is provided. What is it about TDLM that causes it to be so weak?

      Addressing these points would make this manuscript more useful, workable, and constructive, even if they would not necessarily increase its scientific breadth or strength of evidence.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Kern et al. investigated whether temporally delayed linear modeling (TDLM) can uncover sequential memory replay from a graph-learning task in human MEG during an 8-minute post-learning rest period. After failing to detect replay events, they conduct a simulation study in which they insert synthetic replay events, derived from each participant's localizer data, into a control rest period prior to learning. The simulations suggest that TDLM only reveals sequences when replay occurs at very high densities (> 80 per minute) and that individual differences in baseline sequenceness may lead to spurious and/or lackluster correlations between replay strength and behavior.

      Strengths:

      The approach is extremely well documented and rigorous. The authors have done an excellent job re-creating the TDLM methodology that is most commonly used, reporting the different approaches and parameters that they used, and reporting their preregistrations. The hybrid simulation study is creative and provides a new way to assess the efficacy of replay decoding methods. The authors remain measured in the scope/applicability of their conclusions, constructive in their discussion, and end with a useful set of recommendations for how to best apply TDLM in future studies. I also want to commend this work for not only presenting a null result but thoroughly exploring the conditions under which such a null result is expected. I think this paper is interesting and will be generally quite useful for the field, but I believe it also has a number of weaknesses that, if addressed, could improve it further.

      Weaknesses:

      The sample size is small (n=21, after exclusions), even for TDLM studies (which typically have somewhere between 25-40 participants). The authors address this somewhat through a power analysis of the relationship between replay and behavioral performance in their simulations, but this is very dependent on the assumptions of the simulation. Further, according to their own power analysis, the replay-behavior correlations are seriously underpowered (~10% power according to Figure 7C), and so if this is to be taken at face value, their own null findings on this point (Figure 3C) could therefore just reflect undersampling as opposed to methodological failure. I think this point needs to be made more clearly earlier in the manuscript. Relatedly, it would be very useful if one of the recommendations that come out of the simulations in this paper was a power analysis for detecting sequenceness in general, as I suspect that the small sample size impacts this as well, given that sequenceness effects reported in other work are often small with larger sample sizes. Further, I believe that the authors' simulations of basic sequenceness effects would themselves still suffer from having a small number of subjects, thereby impacting statistical power. Perhaps the authors can perform a similar sort of bootstrapping analysis as they perform for the correlation between replay and performance, but over sequenceness itself?

      The task paradigm may introduce issues in detecting replay that are separate from TDLM. First, the localizer task involves a match/mismatch judgment and a button press during the stimulus presentation, which could add noise to classifier training separate from the semantic/visual processing of the stimulus. This localizer is similar to others that have been used in TDLM studies, but notably in other studies (e.g., Liu, Mattar et al., 2021), the stimulus is presented prior to the match/mismatch judgment. A discussion of variations in different localizers and what seems to work best for decoding would be useful to include in the recommendations section of the discussion. Second, and more seriously, I believe that the task design for training participants about the expected sequences may complicate sequence decoding. Specifically, this is because two images (a "tuple") are shown together and used for prediction, which may encourage participants to develop a single bound representation of the tuple that then predicts a third image (AB -> C rather than A -> B, B -> C). This would obviously make it difficult to i) use a classifier trained on individual images to detect sequences and ii) find evidence for the intended transition matrix using TDLM. Can the authors rule out this possibility?

      Participants only modestly improved (from 76-82% accuracy) following the rest period (which the authors refer to as a consolidation period). If the authors assume that replay leads to improved performance, then this suggests there is little reason to see much task-related replay during rest in the first place. This limitation is touched on (lines 228-229), but I think it makes the lack of replay finding here less surprising. However, note that in the supplement, it is shown that the amount of forward sequenceness is marginally related to the performance difference between the last block of training and retrieval, and this is the effect I would probably predict would be most likely to appear. Obviously, my sample size concerns still hold, and this is not a significant effect based on the null hypothesis testing framework the authors employ, but I think this set of results should at least be reported in the main text. I was also wondering whether the authors could clarify how the criterion over six blocks was 80% but then the performance baseline they use from the last block is 76%? Is it just that participants must reach 80% within the six blocks *at some point* during training, but that they could dip below that again later?

      Because most of the conclusions come from the simulation study, there are a few decisions about the simulations that I would like the authors to expand upon before I can fully support their interpretations. First, the authors use a state-to-state lag of 80ms and do not appear to vary this throughout the simulations - can the authors provide context for this choice? Does varying this lag matter at all for the results (i.e., does the noise structure of the data interact with this lag in any way?) Second, it seems that the approach to scaling simulated replays with performance is rather coarse. I think a more sensitive measure would be to scale sequence replays based on the participants' responses to *that* specific sequence rather than altering the frequency of all replays by overall memory performance. I think this would help to deliver on the authors' goal of simulating an "increase of replay for less stable memories" (line 246). On the other hand, I was also wondering whether it is actually necessary to use the real memory performance for each participant in these simulations - couldn't similar goals (with a better/more full sampling of the space of performance) be achieved with simulated memory performance as well, taking only the MEG data from the participant? Finally, Figure 7D shows that 70ms was used on the y-axis. Why was this the case, or is this a typo?

      Because this is a re-analysis of a previous dataset combined with a new simulation study on that data aimed at making recommendations about how to best employ TDLM, I think the usefulness of the paper to the field could be improved in a few places. Specifically, in the discussion/recommendation section, the authors state that "yet unknown confounders" (line 295) lead to non-random fluctuations in the simulated correlations between replay detection and performance at different time lags. Because it is a particularly strong claim that there is the potential to detect sequenceness in the baseline condition where there are no ground-truth sequences, the manuscript could benefit from a more thorough exploration of the cause(s) of this bias in addition to the speculation provided in the current version. In addition, to really provide that a realistic simulation is necessary (one of the primary conclusions of the paper), it would be useful to provide a comparison to a fully synthetic simulation performed on this exact task and transition structure (in addition to the recreation of the original simulation code from the TDLM methods paper). Finally, I think the authors could do further work to determine whether some of their recommendations for improving the sensitivity of TDLM pan out in the current data - for example, they could report focusing not just on the peak decoding timepoint but incorporating other moments into classifier training.

      Lastly, I would like the authors to address a point that was raised in a separate public forum by an author of the TDLM method, which is that when replays "happen during rest, they are not uniform or close". Because the simulations in this work assume regularly occurring replay events, I agree that this is an important limitation that should be incorporated into alternative simulations to ensure the lack of findings is not because of this assumption.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Kern et al. critically assess the sensitivity of temporally delayed linear modelling (TDLM), a relatively new method used to detect memory replay in humans via MEG. While TDLM has recently gained traction and been used to report many exciting links between replay and behavior in humans, Kern et al. were unable to detect replay during a post-learning rest period. To determine whether this null result reflected an actual absence of replay or sensitivity of the method, the authors ran a simulation: synthetic replay events were inserted into a control dataset, and TDLM was used to decode them, varying both replay density and its correlation with behavior. The results revealed that TDLM could only reliably detect replay at unrealistically (not-physiological) high replay densities, and the authors were unable to induce strong behavior correlations. These findings highlight important limitations of TDLM, particularly for detecting replay over extended, minutes-long time periods.

      Strengths:

      Overall, I think this is an extremely important paper, given the growing use of TDLM to report exciting relationships between replay and behavior in humans. I found the text clear, the results compelling, and the critique of TDLM quite fair: it is not that this method can never be applied, but just that it has limits in its sensitivity to detect replay during minutes-long periods. Further, I greatly appreciated the authors' efforts to describe ways to improve TDLM: developing better decoders and applying them to smaller time windows.

      The power of this paper comes from the simulation, whereby the authors inserted replay events and attempted to detect them using TDLM. Regarding their first study, there are many alternative explanations or possible analysis strategies that the authors do not discuss; however, none of these are relevant if, under conditions where it is synthetically inserted, replay cannot be detected.

      Additionally, the authors are relatively clear about which parameters they chose, why they chose them, and how well they match previous literature (they seem well matched).

      Finally, I found the application of TDLM to a baseline period particularly important, as it demonstrated that there are fluctuations in sequenceness in control conditions (where no replay would be expected); it is important to contrast/calculate the difference between control (pre-resting state) and target (post-resting state) sequenceness values.

      Weaknesses:

      While I found this paper compelling, I was left with a series of questions.

      (1) I am still left wondering why other studies were able to detect replay using this method. My takeaway from this paper is that large time windows lead to high significance thresholds/required replay density, making it extremely challenging to detect replay at physiological levels during resting periods. While it is true that some previous studies applying TDLM used smaller time windows (e.g., Kern's previous paper detected replay in 1500ms windows), others, including Liu et al. (2019), successfully detected replay during a 5-minute resting period. Why do the authors believe others have nevertheless been able to detect replay during multi-minute time windows?

      For example, some studies using TDLM report evidence of sequenceness as a contrast between evidence of forwards (f) versus backwards (b) sequenceness; sequenceness was defined as ZfΔt - ZbΔt (where Z refers to the sequence alignment coefficient for a transition matrix at a specific time lag). This use case is not discussed in the present paper, despite its prevalence in the literature. If the same logic were applied to the data in this study, would significant sequenceness have been uncovered? Whether it would or not, I believe this point is important for understanding methodological differences between this paper and others.

      (2) Relatedly, while the authors note that smaller time windows are necessary for TDLM to succeed, a more precise description of the appropriate window size would greatly improve the utility of this paper. As it stands, the discussion feels incomplete without this information, as providing explicit guidance on optimal window sizes would help future researchers apply TDLM effectively. Under what window size range can physiological levels of replay actually be detected using TDLM? Or, is there some scaling factor that should be considered, in terms of window size and significance threshold/replay density? If the authors are unable to provide a concrete recommendation, they could add information about time windows used in previous studies (perhaps, is 1500ms as used in their previous paper a good recommendation?).

      (3) In their simulation, the authors define a replay event as a single transition from one item to another (example: A to B). However, in rodents, replay often traverses more than a single transition (example: A to B to C, even to D and E). Observing multistep sequences increases confidence that true replay is present. How does sequence length impact the authors' conclusions? Similarly, can the authors comment on how the length of the inserted events impacts TDLM sensitivity, if at all?

      For example, regarding sequence length, is it possible that TDLM would detect multiple parts of a longer sequence independently, meaning that the high density needed to detect replay is actually not quite so dense? (example: if 20 four-step sequences (A to B to C to D to E) were sampled by TDLM such that it recorded each transition separately, that would lead to a density of 80 events/min).

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors strived for an inventory of GPCRs and GPCR pathway component genes within the genomes of 23 choanoflagellates and other close relatives of metazoans.

      Strengths:

      The authors generated a solid phylogenetic overview of the GPCR superfamily in these species. Intriguingly, they discover novel GPCR families, novel assortments of domain combinations, novel insights into the evolution of those groups within the Opisthokonta clade. A particular focus is laid on adhesion GPCRs, for which the authors discover many hitherto unknown subfamilies based on Hidden Markov Models of the 7TM domain sequences, which were also reflected by combinations of extracellular domains of the homologs. In addition, the authors provide bioinformatic evidence that aGPCRs of choanoflagellates also contained a GAIN domain, which are self-cleavable thereby reflecting the most remarkable biochemical feat of aGPCRs.

      Weaknesses:

      The chosen classification scheme for aGPCRs may require reassessment and amendment by the authors in order to prevent confusion with previously issued classification attempts of this family.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors set out to characterise the GPCR family in choanoflagellates (and other unicellular holozoans). GPCRs are the most abundant gene family in many animal genomes, playing crucial roles in a wide range of physiological processes. Although they are known to evolve rapidly, GPCRs are an ancient feature of eukaryotic biology. Identifying conserved elements across the animal-protist boundary is therefore a valuable goal, and the increasing availability of genomes from non-animal holozoans provides new opportunities to explore evolutionary patterns that were previously obscured by limited taxon sampling. This study presents a comprehensive re-examination of GPCRs in choanoflagellates, uncovering examples of differential gene retention and revealing the dynamic nature of the GPCR repertoire in this group. As GPCRs are typically involved in environmental sensing, understanding how these systems evolved may shed light on how our unicellular ancestors adapted their signalling networks in the transition to complex multicellularity.

      Strengths:

      The paper combines a broad taxonomic scope with the use of both established and recently developed tools (e.g. Foldseek, AlphaFold), enabling a deep and systematic exploration of GPCR diversity. Each family is carefully described, and the manuscript also functions as an up-to-date review of GPCR classification and evolution. Although similar attempts of understanding GPCR evolution were done over the last decade, the authors build on this foundation by identifying new families and applying improved computational methods to better predict structure and function. Notably, the presence of Rhodopsin-like GPCRs in some choanoflagellates and ichthyosporeans is intriguing, even though they do not fall within known animal subfamilies. The computational framework presented here is broadly applicable, offering a blueprint for surveying GPCR diversity in other non-model eukaryotes (and even in animal lineages), potentially revealing novel families relevant to drug discovery or helping revise our understanding of GPCR evolution beyond model systems.

      Weaknesses:

      While the study contributes several interesting observations, it does not radically revise the evolutionary history of the GPCR family. However, in an era increasingly concerned with the reproducibility of scientific findings, this is arguably a strength rather than a weakness. It is encouraging to see that previously established patterns largely hold, and that with expanded sampling and improved methods, new insights can be gained-especially at the level of specific GPCR subfamilies. Then, no functional follow ups are provided in the model system Salpingoeca rosetta, but I am sure functional work on GPCRs in choanoflagellates is set to reveal very interesting molecular adaptations in the future.

      Comments on the latest version:

      The authors have done a good job answering my questions and suggestions.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Rolland and colleagues investigated the interaction between Vibrio bacteria and Alexandrium algae. The authors found a correlation between the abundance of the two in the Thau Lagoon and observed in the laboratory that Vibrio grows to higher numbers in the presence of the algae than in monoculture. Time-lapse imaging of Alexandrium in coculture with Vibrio enabled the authors to observe Vibrio bacteria in proximity to the algae and subsequent algae death. The authors further determine the mechanism of the interaction between the two and point out similarities between the observed phenotypes and predator-prey behaviours across organisms.

      Strengths:

      The study combines field work with mechanistic studies in the laboratory and uses a wide array of techniques ranging from co-cultivation experiments to genetic engineering, microscopy and proteomics. Further, the authors test multiple Vibrio and Alexandria species and claim a wide spread of the observed phenotypes.

      Weaknesses:

      In my view, the presentation of the data is in some cases not ideal. The phrasing of some conclusions (e.g., group-attacks and wolf-pack-hunting by the bacteria) is in my opinion too strong based on the herein provided data.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Goal summary:

      The authors sought to (i) demonstrate correlations between the dynamics of the dinoflagellate Alexandrium pacificum and the bacterim Vibrio atlanticus in natural populations, ii) demonstrate the occurrence of predation in laboratory experiments, iii) claim coordinated action by the predators in the predation process, iv) demonstrate that predation is induced by predator starvation, and v) test for effects of quorum sensing and iron-uptake genes on the predation process.

      Strengths include:

      (1) Data indicating correlated dynamics in a natural environment that increase the motivation for the study of in vitro interactions.

      (2) Experimental design allowing clear inference of predation based on population counts of both prey and predators in addition to microscopy-based evidence.

      (3) Supplementation of population-level data with molecular approaches to test hypotheses regarding possible involvement of quorum sensing and iron uptake in predation.

      Weaknesses include:

      (1) A lack of early, clear definitions for several important terms used in the paper, including 'predation', 'coordination' and 'coordinated action', 'group attack', and 'wolf-pack hunting', along with a corresponding lack of criteria for what evidence would warrant use of some of these labels. (For example, does mere simultaneity of attacks of an A. pacificum cell by many V. atlanticus cells constitute "coordination"? Or, as it seems to us, does coordination require some form of signalling between predator cells?)

      (2) Absence of controls for cell density in the test for starvation effects on predatory behavior; unclear how the length of incubation affects the density of V. atlanticus cells.

      (3) Lack of clarity in some of the methodological descriptions

      Appraisal:

      The authors convincingly achieve their aim of demonstrating that V. atlanticus can prey on A. pacificum, provide strongly suggestive evidence that such predation is induced by starvation, and clearly demonstrate that both iron availability and, correspondingly, the presence of genes involved in iron uptake, strongly influence the efficacy of predation. However, the evidence for starvation-induction of predation can be strengthened with cell-density controls; evidence for a social component to predation - positive interactions between attacking predators - is lacking.

      Discussion of impact:

      This paper will interest those interested in how microbial behaviour responds to environmental fluctuations, in particular predatory behaviour, but will do so more strongly if the evidence of starvation-induction of predation is strengthened. It will also interest those investigating bacteria-algae interactions and potential ecological controls of algal blooms. It has the potential to interest researchers of microbial cooperation, should the authors be able to provide any evidence of coordination between predator cells.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary

      In this manuscript, Singh, Wu and colleagues explore functional links between septins and the exocyst complex. The exocyst in a conserved octameric complex that mediates the tethering of secretory vesicles for exocytosis in eukaryotes. In fission yeast cells, the exocyst is necessary for cell division, where it localizes mostly at the rim of the division plane, but septins, which localize in a similar manner, are non-essential. The main findings of the work are that septins are required for the specific localization of the exocyst to the rim of the division plane, and the likely consequent localization of the glucanase Eng1 at this same location, where it is known to promote cell separation. In absence of septins, the exocyst still localizes to the division plane, but is not restricted to the rim. They also show some defect in the localization of secretory vesicles and glucan synthase cargo. They further show interactions between septins and exocyst subunits through coIP experiments.

      Strengths

      The septin, exocyst and Eng1 localization data are well supported, showing that the septin rim recruits the exocyst and (likely consequently) the Eng1 glucanase at this location. One important finding of the manuscript is that of a physical interaction between septins and exocyst subunits in co-immunoprecipitation experiments.

      Weaknesses

      While interactions are supported by coIP experiments, the AlphaFold-predicted septin-exocyst interactions are not very convincing and the predicted binding interfaces are not supported by mutation analysis. A further open question is whether septins interact with the intact exocyst complex or whether the interactions occur only with individual subunits. The two-hybrid and coIP data only show weak interactions with individual subunits, and some coIPs (for instance Sec3 and Exo70 with Spn1 and Spn4) are negative, suggesting that the exocyst complex may not remain intact in these experiments.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This interesting study implicates the direct interaction between two multi-subunit complexes, known as the exocyst and septin complexes, in the function of both complexes during cytokinesis in fission yeast. While previous work from several labs had implicated roles for the exocyst and septin complexes in cytokinesis and cell separation, this study describes the importance of protein:protein interaction between these complexes in mediating the functions of these complexes in cytokinesis. Previous studies in neurons had suggested interactions between septins and exocyst complexes occur but the functional importance of such interactions was not known. Moreover, in baker's yeast where both of these complexes have been extensively studied - no evidence of such an interaction has been uncovered despite numerous studies which should have detected it. Therefore while exocyst:septin interactions appear to be conserved in several systems, it appears likely that budding yeast are the exception--having lost this conserved interaction.

      Strengths:

      The strengths of this work include the rigorous analysis of the interaction using multiple methods including Co-IP of tagged but endogenously expressed proteins, 2 hybrid interaction, and Alphafold Multimer. Careful quantitative analysis of the effects of loss of function in each complex and the effects on localization and dynamics of each complex was also a strength. Taken together this work convincingly describes that these two complexes do interact and that this interaction plays an important role in post Golgi vesicle targeting during cytokinesis.

      Comments on revisions:

      The authors have added substantial work to the revised manuscript, and it is much improved. In particular, the figures portraying the AlphaFold Multimer model of the exocyst:septin interactions are much clearer. I also appreciate the effort that went into modeling the fission yeast exocyst complex based on the yeast CryoEM structure in order to determine if the predicted interfaces with septins were likely to be surface accessible in the intact exocyst complex.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors make a bold claim that a combination of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (intermittent theta burst-iTBS) and transcranial alternating current stimulation (gamma tACS) causes slight improvements in memory in a face/name/profession task.

      Strengths:

      The idea of stimulating the human brain non-invasively is very attractive because, if it worked, it could lead to a host of interesting applications. The current study aims to evaluate one such exciting application.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript by Borghi and colleagues provides evidence that the combination of intermittent theta burst TMS stimulation and gamma transcranial alternating current stimulation (γtACS) targeting the precuneus increases long-term associative memory in healthy subjects compared to iTBS alone and sham conditions. Using a rich dataset of TMS-EEG and resting-state functional connectivity (rs-FC) maps and structural MRI data, the authors also provide evidence that dual stimulation increased gamma oscillations and functional connectivity between the precuneus and hippocampus. Enhanced memory performance was linked to increased gamma oscillatory activity and connectivity through white matter tracts.

      Strengths:

      The combination of personalized repetitive TMS (iTBS) and gamma tACS is a novel approach to targeting the precuneus, and thereby, connected memory-related regions to enhance long-term associative memory. The authors leverage an existing neural mechanism engaged in memory binding, theta-gamma coupling, by applying TMS at theta burst patterns and tACS at gamma frequencies to enhance gamma oscillations. The authors conducted a thorough study that suggests that simultaneous iTBS and gamma tACS could be a powerful approach for enhancing long-term associative memory. The paper was well-written, clear, and concise.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Borghi and colleagues present results from 4 experiments aimed at investigating the effects of dual γtACS and iTBS stimulation of the precuneus on behavioral and neural markers of memory formation. In their first experiment (n = 20), they find that a 3-minute offline (i.e., prior to task completion) stimulation that combines both techniques leads to superior memory recall performance in an associative memory task immediately after learning associations between pictures of faces, names, and occupation, as well as after a 15-minute delay, compared to iTBS alone (+ tACS sham) or no stimulation (sham for both iTBS and tACS). Performance in a second task probing short-term memory was unaffected by the stimulation condition. In a second experiment (n = 10), they show that these effects persist over 24 hours and up to a full week after initial stimulation. A third (n = 14) and fourth (n = 16) experiment were conducted to investigate neural effects of the stimulation protocol. The authors report that, once again, only combined iTBS and γtACS increases gamma oscillatory activity and neural excitability (as measured by concurrent TMS-EEG) specific to the stimulated area at the precuneus compared to a control region, as well as precuneus-hippocampus functional connectivity (measured by resting state MRI), which seemed to be associated with structural white matter integrity of the bilateral middle longitudinal fasciculus (measured by DTI).

      Strengths:

      Combining non-invasive brain stimulation techniques is a novel, potentially very powerful method to maximize the effects of these kinds of interventions that are usually well-tolerated and thus accepted by patients and healthy participants. It is also very impressive that the stimulation-induced improvements in memory performance resulted from a short (3 min) intervention protocol. If the effects reported here turn out to be as clinically meaningful and generalizable across populations as implied, this approach could represent a promising avenue for treatment of impaired memory functions in many conditions.

      Methodologically, this study is expertly done! I don't see any serious issues with the technical setup in any of the experiments. It is also very commendable that the authors conceptually replicated the behavioral effects of experiment 1 in experiment 2 and then conducted two additional experiments to probe the neural mechanisms associated with these effects. This certainly increases the value of the study and the confidence in the results considerably.

      The authors used a within-subject approach in their experiments, which increases statistical power and allows for stronger inferences about the tested effects. They also used to individualize stimulation locations and intensities, which should further optimize the signal-to-noise ratio.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      In this manuscript, Lau et al reported that KDM5 inhibition in luminal breast cancer cells results in R-loop-mediated DNA damage, reduced cell fitness and an increase in ISG and AP signatures as well as cell surface Major Histocompatibility Complex (MHC) class I, mediated by RNA:DNA hybrid activation of the CGAS/STING pathway.

      Their studies have shown that KDM5 inhibition/loss mediates a viral mimicry and DNA damage response through the generation of R-loops in genomic repeats. This is a different mechanism from the more well studied double-stranded RNA-induced "viral mimicry" response.

      More importantly, they have shown that KDM5 inhibition does not result in DNA damage or activation of the CGAS/STING pathway in normal breast epithelial cells, suggesting that KDM5 inhibitors may enable a wide therapeutic window in this setting, as compared to STING agonists or Type I Interferons.

      Their findings provide new insights into the interplay between epigenetic regulation of genomic repeats, R-loop formation, innate immunity, and cell fitness in the context of cancer evolution and therapeutic vulnerability.

      Comments on revised version:

      The authors have satisfactorily addressed my comments and revised the manuscript accordingly.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, the authors investigated how the type-I interferon response (ISG) and antigen presentation (AP) pathways are repressed in luminal breast cancer cells and how this repression can be overcome. They found that a STING agonist can reactivate these pathways in breast cancer cells, but it also does so in normal cells, suggesting that this is not a good way to create a therapeutic window. Depletion of ADAR and inhibition of KDM5 also activate ISG and AP genes. The activation of ISG and AP genes is dependent on cGAS/STING and the JAK kinase. Interestingly, although both ADAR depletion and KDM5 inhibition activate ISG and AP genes, their effects on cell fitness are different. Furthermore, KDM5 inhibitor selectively activates ISG and AP genes in tumor cells but not normal cells, arguing that it may create a larger therapeutic window than the STING agonist. These results also suggest that KDM5 inhibition may activate ISG and AP genes in a way different from ADAR loss, and this process may affect tumor cell fitness independently of the activation of ISG and AP genes.

      The authors further showed that KDM5 inhibition increases R-loops and DNA damage in tumor cells, and XPF, a nuclease that cuts R-loops, is required for the activation of ISG and AP genes. Using H3K4me3 CUT&RUN, they found that KMD5 inhibition results in increased H3K4me3 not only at genes, but also at repetitive elements including SINE, LINE, LTR, telomeres, and centromeres. Using S9.6 CUT&TAG, they confirmed that R-loops are increased at SINE, LINE, and LTR repeated with increased H3K4me3. Together, the results of this study suggest that KMD5 inhibition leads to H3K4me3 and R-loop accumulation in repetitive elements, which induces DNA damage and cGAS/STING activation and subsequently activates AP genes. This provides an exciting approach to stimulate the anti-tumor immunity against breast tumors.

      KDM5 inhibition activates interferon and antigen presentation genes through R-loops.

      Strengths:

      A new approach to make breast tumors "hot" for anti-tumor immunity.

      Weaknesses:

      Future in vivo studies are needed to show the effects of KDM5 inhibitors on the immunotherapy responses of breast tumors.

      Comments on revised version:

      The authors have adequately addressed my comments.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Shen et al. have improved upon the mitotic clone analysis tool MAGIC that their lab previously developed. MAGIC uses CRISPR/Cas9-mediated double-stranded breaks to induce mitotic recombination. The authors have replaced the sgRNA scaffold with a more effective scaffold to increase clone frequency. They also introduced modifications to positive and negative clonal markers to improve signal-to-noise and mark the cytoplasm of the cells instead of the nuclei. The changes result in increase in clonal frequencies and marker brightness. The authors also generated the MAGIC transgenics to target all chromosome arms and tested the clone induction efficacy.

      Strengths:

      MAGIC is a mitotic clone generation tool that works without prior recombination to special chromosomes (e.g., FRT). It can also generate mutant clones for genes for which the existing FRT lines could not be used (e.g., the genes that are between the FRT transgene and the centromere).

      This manuscript does a thorough job in describing the method and provides compelling data that support improvement over the existing method.

      Weaknesses:

      It would be beneficial to have a greater variety of clonal markers for nMAGIC. Currently, the only marker is BFP, which may clash with other genetic tools (e.g., some FRET probes) depending on the application. It would be nice to have far-red clonal markers.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, the authors present the latest improvement of their previously published methods, pMAGIC and nMAGIC, which can be used to engineer mosaic gene expression in wild-type animals and in a tissue-specific manner. They address the main limitation of MAGIC, the lack of gRNA-marker transgenes, which has hampered the broader adoption of MAGIC in the fly community. To do so, they create an entire toolkit of gRNA markers for every Drosophila chromosome and test them across a range of different tissues and in the context of making Drosophila species hybrid mosaic animals. The study provides a significant and broadly useful improvement compared to earlier versions, as it broadens the use-cases for transgenic manipulation with MAGIC to virtually any subfield of Drosophila cell biology.

      Strengths:

      Major improvements to MAGIC were made in terms of clone induction efficiency and usability across the Drosophila model system, including wild-type genotypes and the use in non-melanogaster species.

      Notably, mosaic mutants can now be created for genes residing on the 4th chromosome, which is exciting and possibly long-awaited by 4th chromosome gene enthusiasts.

      Selection of the standard set of gRNA markers was done thoughtfully, using non-repetitive conserved and unique sequences.

      The authors demonstrate that MAGIC can be used easily in the context of interspecific hybrids. I believe this is a great advancement for the Drosophila community, especially for evolutionary biologists, because this may allow for easy access to mechanistic, tissue-specific insight into the process of a range of hybrid incompatibilities, an important speciation process that is normally difficult to study at the level of molecular and cell biology.

      In the same way, because it is not limited to usage in any particular genetic background, genome-wide MAGIC can be potentially used in wild-type genotypes relatively easily. This is exciting, especially because natural genetic diversity is rarely investigated more mechanistically and at the scale/resolution of cells or specific tissues. Now, one can ask how a particular naturally occurring allele influences cell physiology compared to another (control) while keeping the global physiological context of the particular genetic background largely intact.

      Weaknesses:

      It is not entirely clear how functionally non-critical regions were evaluated, besides that they are selected based on conservation of sequence between species. It may be useful to directly test the difference in viability or other functionally relevant phenotype for flies carrying different markers. Similarly, the frequency of off-targets could be investigated or documented in a bit more detail, especially if one of the major use-cases is meant for naturally derived, diverse genetic backgrounds. It is, at the moment, unclear how consistently the clones are induced for each new gRNA marker across different WT genetic backgrounds, for example, a set of DGRP genotypes, which could be highly useful information for future users.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In the manuscript by Shen, Yeung, and colleagues, the authors generate an improved and expanded Mosaic analysis by gRNA-induced crossing-over (MAGIC) toolkit for use in making mosaic clones in Drosophila. This is a clever method by which mitotic clones can be induced in dividing cells by using CRISPR/Cas9 to generate double-strand breaks at specific locations that induce crossing over at those locations. This is conceptually similar to previous mosaic methods in flies that utilized FRT sites that had been inserted near centromeres along with heat-shock inducible FLPase. The advantage of the MAGIC system is that it can be used along with chromosomes lacking FRT sites already introduced, such as those found in many deficiency collections or in EMS mutant lines. It may also be simpler to implement than FRT-based mosaic systems. There are two flavors of the MAGIC system: nMAGIC and pMAGIC. In nMAGIC, the main constituents are a transgene insertion that contains gRNAs that target DNA near the centromere, along with a fluorescent marker. In pMAGIC, the main constituents are a transgenic insertion that contains gRNAs that target DNA near the centromere, along with ubiquitous expression of GAL80. As such, nMAGIC can be used to generate clones that are not labelled, whereas pMAGIC (along with a GAL4 line and UAS-marker) can be used much like MARCM to positively label a clone of cells. This manuscript introduces MAGIC transgenic reagents that allow all 4 chromosomes to be targeted. They demonstrate its use in a variety of tissues, including with mutants not compatible with current FLP/FRT methods, and also show it works well in tissues that prove challenging for FLP/FRT mosaic analyses (such as motor neurons). They further demonstrate that it can be used to generate mosaic clones in non-melanogaster hybrid tissues. Overall, this work represents a valuable improvement to the MAGIC method that should promote even more widespread adoption of this powerful genetic technique.

      Strengths:

      (1) Improves the design of the gRNA-marker by updating the gRNA backbone and also the markers used. GAL80 now includes a DE region that reduces the perdurance of the protein and thus better labeling of pMAGIC clones. The data presented to demonstrate these improvements is rigorous and of high quality.

      (2) Introduces a toolkit that now covers all chromosome arms in Drosophila. In addition, the efficiency of 3 target different sites is characterized for each chromosome arm (e.g., 3 different gRNA-Marker combinations), which demonstrate differences in efficiency. This could be useful to titrate how many clones an experimenter might want (e.g., lower efficiency combinations might prove advantageous).

      (3) The manuscript is well written and easy to follow. The authors achieved their aims of creating and demonstrating MAGIC reagents suitable for mosaic analysis of any Drosophila chromosome arm.

      (4) The MAGIC method is a valuable addition to the Drosophila genetics toolkit, and the new reagents described in this manuscript should allow it to become more widely adopted.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The MAGIC method might not be well known to most readers, and the manuscript could have benefited from schematics introducing the technique.

      (2) Traditional mosaic analyses using the FLP/FRT system have strongly utilized heat-shock FLPase for inducible temporal control over mitotic clones, as well as a way to titrate how many clones are induced (e.g., shorter heat shocks will induce fewer clones). This has proven highly valuable, especially for developmental studies. A heat-shock Cas9 is available, and it would have been beneficial to determine the efficiency of inducing MAGIC clones using this Cas9 source.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The study by Bobola et al reports single-nucleus expression analysis with some supporting spatial expression data of human embryonic and fetal cardiac outflow tracts compared to adult aortic valves. The transcription factor GATA6 is identified as a top regulator of one of the mesenchymal subpopulations, and potential interacting factors and downstream target genes are identified bioinformatically. Additional bioinformatic tools are used to describe cell lineage relationships and trajectories for developmental and adult cardiac cell types.

      Strengths:

      The studies of human tissue and extensive gene expression data will be valuable to the field.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The expression data are largely confirmatory of previous studies in humans and mice. Thus, it is not clear what novel biological insights are being reported. While there is some novelty and impact in using human tissue, there are extensive existing publications and data sets in this area.

      (2) Major conclusions regarding spatial localization, differential gene expression, or cell lineage relationships based on bioinformatic data are not validated in the context of intact tissues.

      (3) The conclusions regarding lineage relationships are based on common gene expression in the current study and may not reflect cellular origins or lineage relationships that have previously been reported in genetic mouse models.

      (4) An additional limitation is the exclusive examination of adult aortic valve leaflets that represent only a subset of outflow tract derivatives in the mature heart. The conclusion, as stated in the title regarding adult derivatives of the outflow tract, is not accurate based on the limited adult tissue evaluated, exclusive bioinformatic approach, and lack of experimental lineage analysis of cell origins.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript by Leshem et al. presents a transcriptomic analysis of the developing human outflow tract (OFT) at embryonic and fetal stages using snRNAseq and spatial transcriptomics. Additionally, the authors analyze transcriptomic data from the adult aortic valve to compare embryonic and adult cell populations, aiming to identify persistent embryonic transcriptional signatures in adult cells. A total of 15 clusters were identified from the embryonic and fetal OFT samples, including three mesenchymal and four endothelial clusters. Using SCENIC analysis on the embryonic snRNAseq data, the authors identified GATA6 as a key regulator of valve precursor cells. Spatial transcriptomic analysis of four fetal OFT sections further revealed the spatial distribution of mesenchymal nuclei, smooth muscle cells, and valvular interstitial cells. Trajectory analysis identified two distinct developmental origins of fetal mesenchymal cells: the neural crest and the second heart field. Finally, the authors used snRNAseq data from the adult aortic valve to propose that embryonic transcriptional signatures persist in a subset of adult cells.

      Strengths:

      (1) The study offers a rich and detailed dataset, combining snRNA-seq and spatial transcriptomics in human embryonic and fetal OFT, which are challenging to obtain.

      (2) The use of SCENIC and trajectory analysis adds mechanistic insight into cell lineage and regulatory programs during valve development.

      (3) This study confirms GATA6 as a key regulator of valve precursor cells.

      (4) Comparison between embryonic/fetal and adult datasets represents a novel attempt to trace persistence of developmental transcriptional programs.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) A major limitation is the lack of experimental validation to support key conclusions, particularly the claim of persistent embryonic transcriptional signatures in adult cells.

      (2) The manuscript would benefit from a clearer discussion of how these results advance beyond previous studies in human heart and valve development.

      (3) The comparison between embryonic and adult data is interesting, but would be more convincing with additional evidence supporting the proposed persistence of embryonic transcriptional signatures in adult cells.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Leshem et al have generated a transcriptional cell atlas of the human outflow tract at two developmental timepoints and its adult valvular derivatives. This carefully performed study provides a useful resource for the study of known genes implicated in outflow tract defects and potentially also for discovering new disease genes. The authors reveal neural crest and mesodermal contributions to different outflow tract components and show that GATA6, known to play a role in arterial valve development, controls a set of genes expressed in endocardium-derived cells during valve development. Interestingly, the results suggest lineage persistence of expression of certain genes through to the adult timepoint, a main new finding of this study.

      The following points should be addressed to reinforce the conclusions and emphasize the novel features of this study.

      (1) It would be helpful to clarify how these new findings confirm or diverge from what is known from analysis of neural crest and mesodermal lineage contributions to different cell populations in the mouse heart. Did the authors identify any human-specific populations of cells, such as the LGR5 population reported by Sahara et al?

      (2) The authors should clarify in the introduction and results that they consider the endocardium to be on the SHF trajectory as indicated in Figure S4C. Please add a reference for this point.

      (3) The GATA6 results are interesting and support this experimental approach. The paper would be reinforced if the authors could provide any functional validation (in addition to their GATA6 genomic occupancy data) that the designated target genes are regulated by GATA6. This might involve looking at mutant mouse embryos or cultured cells. Do the authors consider that GATA6 may regulate the endocardial to mesenchymal transition during the early stages of valve development? Or the valve interstitial cell versus fibroblast fate choice?

      (4) Do the new findings reveal whether human valves have a direct SHF to VIC trajectory (ie, without transiting through endocardium) as has been recently shown in the murine non-coronary valve leaflet? Relevant to this point, Figure 5E appears to show contributions to a single adult aortic valve leaflet - this should be explained, or corrected.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In the study by Wang et al. entitled "Dissecting organoid-bacteria interaction highlights decreased contractile force as a key factor for heart infection", a simple cardiac organoid (CO) model was established, by combining a heterologous mixture of patient-specific human induced pluripotent stem cells (hiPSC)-derived cardiomyocytes (CMs) in combination with primary HUVECs (Human Umbilical Vein Endothelial Cells) and human mesenchymal stem cells (MSCs, representing stromal cells). This model was applied for investigating the interplay of COs' bacterial infections in vitro, aiming at revealing pathological mechanisms of bacterial infections of the heart in vivo, which may induce myocarditis and consequently heart failure in affected patients.

      Strengths:

      The paper is systematic, well written, and easy to follow.

      Based on their results, the authors state that: "In this study, by developing quantitative tools for analyzing bacterial-cardiac organoid interactions in a 3D, dynamic, clinically relevant setting, we discovered the significant role of cardiac contractility in preventing bacterial infection."

      In principle, the idea of establishing a simple yet functionally and physiologically relevant in vitro model and relevant analytical tools for enabling the study of complex pathological mechanisms of cardiovascular diseases is intriguing.

      Weaknesses:

      However, despite the combination of numerous analytical tools established and applied in the study, the work has substantial experimental limitations, indicating that the bold conclusions may represent a misinterpretation or overinterpretation of the findings.

      Key limitations and questions:

      (1) It seems that iPSCs from only one patient ("dilated cardiomyopathy (DCM) cells were derived from a 47-year-old Asian male with an LMNA gene mutation") were used in the study. Moreover, it seems that only one iPSC-line/clone from that DCM patient was used and compared to a single control iPSC line from a "healthy donor". Therefore, despite the different assays and experimental controls used in the study, there is a high risk that the observed phenomena reflect iPSC-line-/ clone-dependent effects, rather than revealing general pathophysiologic mechanisms. Thus, key experiments must be shown by cardiomyocytes/ cardiac organoids derived from additional independent iPSC-lines representing different patients and other non-diseased control lines as well. Moreover, it is established good experimental practice in the iPS cell field to generate and include isogenic iPSC controls i.e. iPSC lines of the same genetic background but with corrections of the hypothesised gene mutation underlying the respective e.g., cardiovascular disease.

      (2) In Figure 1 (A) immunohistochemical staining for cardiomyocytes for the cardiac marker Troponin is shown, apparently indicating successful cardiomyogenic differentiation of the applied hiPSC lines. In supplemental Figure S1, a flow cytometry analysis specific to cTnT is shown to reveal the CMs content resulting from the monolayer differentiation of respective iPSC lines. Already, the exemplified plots indicate that the CMs' content/ purity for DCM-CMs was notably lower compared to healthy cardiomyocytes (CM; control). This is an important issue, since the non-CMs ("contaminating bystander cells") may have a substantial effect on the functional (including contractile) properties of the COs.

      Interestingly, based on the method description, it seems that COs were generated from cryopreserved iPSC-CMs and iPSC-DCMs, including intermediate seeding and culture on Matrigel before COs formation. However, it remains unclear whether the CMs FACS analysis, which is apparently: "Representative FACS plots for analysis of the cell types in DCM monolayer culture after 33 days of differentiation" shows a CMs purity relevant to CO formation, or something different.

      The lineage phenotype of non-CMs in respective differentiations should also be clarified. Moreover, it should be noted in the results that the CMs content in COs is lower than the 6:2:2 (CM:ECs:MSC) ratio indicated by the authors, since the CMs purity is not 100%, and is particularly reduced in the iPSC-DCMs.

      Finally, to investigate the important latter questions of the "real CMs content" in COs, systematic technologies should be applied to quantify the lineage composition in COs (e.g. by IF staining for the 3 lineages plus DAPI, followed by COs clearance, confocal microscopy "3D stags" and automated, ImageJ-based quantitative cell counts for total cell number definition (see e.g. doi: 10.1038/s41596-024-00976-2) per CO, and quantification of respective lineage content as well.

      These questions are of key importance since the presence of non-CMs and their phenotype has profound consequences on the cardiac organoid model, its contractile/ biophysical properties, and, in general, on models' sensitivity to bacterial infections as well.

      (3) Figure 2: (F) Why is this figure (Confocal Observations) showing only healthy cardiac organoids (HCOs) but not DCM-COs?

      The overall quality of these pictures is poor and not informative regarding the structural identity and tissue composition of the COs, which actually is an important topic in the frame of the paper, as the 3D structure and tissue composition - and differences between HCOs and DCM-COs - are of key importance to their contractile properties.

      Moreover, the expective overlay of the cardiac markers alpha-actinin and MHC is not obvious from Figure 2F (see also comments on Figure 7, below).

      In Figure 2E: COs at later stages/days should be shown, in particular at that stage, which was used for the functional assays i.e., bacteria infections and contraction pattern monitoring.

      (4) Figure 7 (A) (B) - In the IF sections, it seems that there is no overlay between the expression of the cardiac marker MHC (seems to be expressed in the centre of COs only) and the cardiac markers alpha-actinin (which seems to be unexpectedly expressed in all cells on the sections) and Troponin (which seems to be vocally expressed on the outside, excluding the area of MHC expression).

      (F) Quantification of the mean area of gene expression, e.g., for MHC indicates a larger area after MHC expression; this seems to entirely contradict the IF pictures (in Figures 7 A-D) of MHC expression before and after infection. This contraction is deemed very critical to this reviewer as it may indicate that the IF staining, data analysis, and/or data interpretation in this part of the manuscript is poor, misleading, or simply wrong.

      (5) Overall, from the perspective of this reviewer, the CO-derived results do not reflect in a meaningful way the contractile and hydrodynamic conditions in the mouse heart or the human heart. Thus, it seems that the conclusions may rather represent a hypothesised outcome bias.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors tried deconvoluting, for the first time, the effect of various components of heart contraction on initial bacterial adhesion, which increases the risk of infective endocarditis. The proposed organoid platform might be used to develop and test novel therapeutic agents for infective endocarditis.

      Strengths:

      (1) Use of a broad range of methods: finite element methods, -omics, particle tracking, animal experiments to investigate the connections between contractility and infective endocarditis.

      (2) Detailed procedure and supportive information, which will allow other groups to replicate the results and extend the application of the proposed organoid platform.

      (3) Despite the complexity of the work reported, the manuscript is rather readable and understandable by non-specialists.

      Weaknesses:

      There is a minor issue with some of the vocabulary (e.g., magnificent amount of bacteria).

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors aimed to investigate the cellular mechanisms underlying place field formation (PFF) in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells by performing in vivo two-photon calcium imaging in head-restrained mice navigating a virtual environment. Specifically, they sought to determine whether BTSP-like (behavioral time scale synaptic plasticity) events, characterized by large calcium transients, are the primary mechanism driving PFFs or if other mechanisms also play a significant role. Through their extensive imaging dataset, the authors found that while BTSP-like events are prevalent, a substantial fraction of new place fields are formed via non-BTSP-like mechanisms. They further observed that large calcium transients, often associated with BTSP-like events, are not sufficient to induce new place fields, indicating the presence of additional regulatory factors (possibly local dendritic spikes).

      Strengths

      The study makes use of a robust and extensive dataset collected from 163 imaging sessions across 45 mice, providing a comprehensive examination of CA1 place cell activity during navigation in both familiar and novel virtual environments. The use of two-photon calcium imaging allows the authors to observe the detailed dynamics of neuronal activity and calcium transients, offering insights into the differences between BTSP-like and non-BTSP-like PFF events. The study's ability to distinguish between these two mechanisms and analyze their prevalence under different conditions is a key strength, as it provides a nuanced understanding of how place fields are formed and maintained. The paper supports the idea that BTSP is not the only driving fore behind PFF, and other mechanisms are likely sufficient to drive PFF, and BTSP events may also be insufficient to drive PFF in some cases. The longer-than-usual virtual track used in the experiment allowed place cells to express multiple place fields, adding a valuable dimension to the dataset that is typically lacking in similar studies. Additionally, the authors took a conservative approach in classifying PFF events, ensuring that their findings were not confounded by noise or ambiguous activity.

      Weaknesses

      The stand out weakness of the paper is the lack of direct measures of BTSP events. Without direct confirmation that large calcium transients correspond to actual BTSP events (including associated complex spikes and calcium plateau potentials), concluding that BTSP is not necessary or sufficient for PFF formation is speculative (although I do believe it).

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors of this manuscript aim to investigate the formation of place fields (PFs) in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells. They focus on the role of behavioral time scale synaptic plasticity (BTSP), a mechanism proposed to be crucial for the formation of new PFs. Using in vivo two-photon calcium imaging in head-restrained mice navigating virtual environments, employing a classification method based on calcium activity to categorize the formation of place cells' place fields into BTSP, non-BTSP-like, and investigated their properties.

      Strengths:

      This work shows that place fields formation could induced by both BSTP and non-BSTP events, and it also provided a new and solid method to classify BTSP and non-BTSP place field formation using calcium image to the field. This work offers novel knowledge and new methods and factual evidence for other researchers in the field.

      The method enabled the authors to reveal that while many PFs are formed by BTSP-like events, a significant number of PFs emerge with calcium dynamics that do not match BTSP characteristics, suggesting a diversity of mechanisms underlying PF formation. The characteristics of place fields under the first two categories are comprehensively described, including aspects such as formation timing, quantity, and width.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors have addressed the weaknesses in the revised version.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Sumegi et al. use calcium imaging in head-fixed mice to test whether new place fields tend to emerge due to events that resemble behavioral time scale plasticity (BTSP) or other mechanisms. An impressive dataset was amassed (163 sessions from 45 mice with 500-1000 neurons per sample) to study spontaneous emergence of new place fields in area CA1 that had the signature of BTSP. The authors observed that place fields could emerge due to BTSP and non-BTSP-like mechanisms. Interestingly, when non-BTSP mechanisms seemed to generate a place field, this tended to occur on a trial with a spontaneous reset in neural coding (a remapping event). Novelty seemed to upregulate non-BTSP events relative to BTSP events. Finally, large calcium transients (presumed plateau potentials) were not sufficient to generate a place field.

      Strengths:

      I found this manuscript to be exceptionally well written, well powered, and timely given the outstanding debate and confusion surrounding whether all place fields must arise from BTSP event. Working at the same institute, Albert Lee (e.g. Epszstein et al., 2011 - which should be cited) and Jeff Magee (e.g. Bittner et al., 2017) showed contradictory results for how place fields arise. These accounts have not fully been put toe-to-toe and reconciled in the literature. This manuscript addresses this gap and shows that both accounts are correct - place fields can emerge due to a pre-existing map and due to BTSP.

      Weaknesses:

      I find only three significant areas for improvement in the present study:

      First, can it be concluded that non-BTSP events occur exclusively due to a global remapping event, as stated in the manuscript "these PFF surges included a high fraction of both non-BTSP- and BTSP-like PFF events, and were associated with global remapping of the CA1 representation"? Global remapping has a precise definition that involves quantifying the stability of all place fields recorded. Without a color scale bar in Figure 3D (which should be added), we cannot know whether the overall representations were independent before and after the spontaneous reset. It would be good to know if some neurons are able to maintain place coding (more often than expected by chance), suggestive of a partial-remapping phenomenon.

      Second, BTSP has a flip side that involves weakening of existing place fields when a novel field emerges. Was this observed in the present study? Presumably place fields can disappear due to this bidirectional-BTSP or due to global remapping. For a full comparison of the two phenomena, the disappearance of place fields must also be assessed.

      Finally, it would be good to know if place fields differ according to how they are born. For example, are there differences in reliability, width, peak rate, out of field firing, etc for those that arise due BTSP vs non-BTSP.

      Comments on revisions:

      The authors have mostly addressed my feedback. Compelling evidence for a fundamental observation.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Lu & Cui et al. observe that adult male zebrafish are more resistant to infection and disease following exposure to Spring Viremia of Carp Virus (SVCV) than female fish. The authors then attempt to identify some of the molecular underpinnings of this apparent sexual dimorphism and focus their investigations on a gene called cytochrome P450, family 17, subfamily A, polypeptide 2 (cyp17a2) because it was among the genes that they found to be more highly expressed in kidney tissue from males than in females. Their investigations lead them to propose a direct connection between cyp17a2 and modulation of interferon signaling as the key underlying driver of the difference between male and female susceptibility to SVCV.

      Strengths:

      Strengths of this study include the interesting observation of a substantial difference between adult male and female zebrafish in their susceptibility to SVCV, and also the breadth of experiments that were performed linking cyp17a2 to infection phenotypes and molecularly to the stability of host and virus proteins in cell lines. The authors place the infection phenotype in an interesting and complex context of many other sexual dimorphisms in infection phenotypes in vertebrates. This study succeeds in highlighting an unexpected factor involved in antiviral immunity that will be an important subject for future investigations of infection, metabolism, and other contexts.

      Weaknesses:

      Weaknesses of this study include an indirect connection between the majority of experiments and the proposed mechanism underlying the sexual dimorphism phenotype, widespread reliance on over-expression when investigating protein-protein interaction and localization, and an insufficient amount of description of the data presented in the figures. Specific examples of areas for clarification or improvement include:

      (1) Figure 10 outlines a mechanistic link between cyp17a2 and the sexual dimorphism the authors report for SVCV infection outcomes. The data presented on increased susceptibility of cyp17a2-/- mutant male zebrafish support this diagram, but this conclusion is fairly weak without additional experimentation in both males and females. The authors justify their decision to focus on males by stating that they wanted to avoid potential androgen-mediated phenotypes in the cpy17a2 mutant background (lines 152-156), but this appears to be speculation. It also doesn't preclude the possibility of testing the effects of increased cyp17a2 expression on viral infection in both males and females. This is of critical importance if the authors intend to focus the study on sexual dimorphism, which is how the introduction and discussion are currently structured.

      (2) The authors present data indicating an unexpected link between cyp17a2 and ubiquitination pathways. It is unclear how a CYP450 family member would carry out such activities, and this warrants much more attention. One brief paragraph in the discussion (starting at line 448) mentions previous implications of CYP450 proteins in antiviral immunity, but given that most of the data presented in the paper attempt to characterize cyp17a2 as a direct interactor of ubiquitination factors, more discussion in the text should be devoted to this topic. For example, are there any known domains in this protein that make sense in this context? Discussion of this interface is more relevant to the study than the general overview of sexual dimorphism that is currently highlighted in the discussion and throughout the text.

      (3) Figures 2-9 contain information that could be streamlined to highlight the main points the authors hope to make through a combination of editing, removal, and movement to supplemental materials. There is a consistent lack of clarity in these figures that could be improved by supplementing them with more text to accompany the supplemental figures. Using Figure 2 and an example, panel (A) could be removed as unnecessary, panel (B) could be exchanged for a volcano plot with examples highlighting why cyp17a2 was selected for further study and also the full dataset could be shared in a supplemental table, panel (C) could be modified to indicate why that particular subset was chosen for plotting along with an explanation of the scaling, panel (D) could be moved to supplemental because the point is redundant with panels (A) and (C), panel (E) could be presented as a heatmap, in panels (G) and (H) data from EPC cells could be moved to supplemental because it is not central to the phenotype under investigation, panels (J) to (L) and (N) to (P) could be moved to supplemental because they are redundant with the main points made in panels (M) and (Q). Similar considerations could be made with Figures 3-9

      (4) The data in Figure 3 (A)-(C) do not seem to match the description in the text. That is, the authors state that cyp17a2 overexpression increases interferon signaling activity in cells, but the figure shows higher increases in vector controls. Additionally, the data in panel (H) are not described. What genes were selected and why, and where are the data on the rest of the genes from this analysis? This should be shared in a supplemental table.

      (5) Some of the reagents described in the methods do not have cited support for the applications used in the study. For example, the antibody for TRIM11 (line 624, data in Figures 6 & 7) was generated for targeting the human protein. Validation for use of this reagent in zebrafish should be presented or cited. Furthermore, the accepted zebrafish nomenclature for this gene would be preferred throughout the text, which is bloodthirsty-related gene family, member 32.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      The manuscript identified Cyp17a2 as a master regulator of male-biased antiviral immunity in a sex chromosome-free model (zebrafish) challenging established immunological paradigms.

      Strengths:

      (1) The bifunctional role of Cyp17a2 (host-directed STING stabilization and virus-directed P degradation) represents a significant conceptual advance.

      (2) First demonstration of K33 chains as a critical regulatory switch for both host defense proteins and viral substrates.

      (3) Comprehensive validation across biological scales: organismal (survival, histopathology), cellular (transcriptomics, Co-IPs), and molecular (ubiquitination assays, site-directed mutagenesis).

      (4) Functional conservation in cyprinids (zebrafish and gibel carp) strengthens biological significance.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Colocalization analyses (Figures 4G, 6I, 9D) require quantitative metrics (e.g., Pearson's coefficients) rather than representative images alone.

      (2) Figure 1 survival curves need annotated statistical tests (e.g., "Log-rank test, p=X.XX")

      (3) Figure 2P GSEA should report exact FDR-adjusted *p*-values (not just "*p*<0.05").

      (4) Section 2 overextends on teleost sex-determination diversity, condensing to emphasize relevance to immune dimorphism would strengthen narrative cohesion.

      (5) Limited discussion on whether this mechanism extends beyond Cyprinidae and its implications for teleost adaptation.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In their manuscript, Metz Reed and colleagues present an exceptionally thorough analysis of three-dimensional genome reorganization during breast cancer progression using the well-characterized MCF10 model system. The integration of high-resolution Micro-C contact maps with multi-omics profiling provides compelling insights into stage-specific dynamics of chromatin compartments, TAD boundaries, and looping events. The discovery that stable chromatin loops enable epigenetic reprogramming of cancer genes, while structural changes selectively drive metastasis-associated pathways, represents a significant conceptual advance. This work substantially deepens our understanding of genome topology in malignancy. To further enhance this impactful study, we offer the following constructive suggestions.

      Strengths:

      This work sets a benchmark for integrative 3D genomics in oncology. Its methodological sophistication and conceptual advances establish a new paradigm for studying nuclear architecture in disease.

      Weaknesses:

      Major Issues

      (1) Functional tests would strengthen the observed links between structure and gene changes. For example, the COL12A1 gene loop formation correlates with its increased expression. Disrupting this loop using CRISPR-dCas9 at chr6 position 75280 kb could prove whether the loop causes COL12A1 activation. Such experiments would turn strong correlations into clear mechanisms.

      (2) The H3K27ac looping idea needs deeper validation. Data suggests H3K27ac loss weakens loops without affecting CTCF. Testing how cohesin proteins interact with H3K27ac-modified sites would clarify this process. Degron systems could rapidly remove H3K27ac to observe real-time effects. Also, the AP-1 motifs found at dynamic loop sites deserve functional tests. Knocking down AP-1 factors might show if they control loop formation.

      (3) Connecting findings to patient data would boost clinical relevance. The MCF10 model is excellent for controlled studies. Checking if TAD boundary weakening occurs in actual patient metastases would show real-world importance. Comparing primary and metastatic tumor samples from the same patients could reveal new structural biomarkers. If tissue is scarce, testing cancer cells with added stroma cells might mimic tumor environment effects.

      Minor Issues

      Adding a clear definition for static loops would help readers. For example, state that static loops show less than 10 percent contact change across replicates. In the ABC model analysis, removing promoter regions from the enhancer list would focus results on true long-range interactions. Briefly noting why this study sees TAD weakening while other cancer types show different patterns would provide useful context.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Employing the MCF10 breast-cancer progression series, the authors integrate high-resolution Micro-C chromatin-conformation capture with RNA-seq and ChIP-seq to delineate the sequential reorganization of compartments, topologically associated domains (TADs), and long-range loops across benign, pre-neoplastic, and metastatic states, and couple these 3D alterations to gene expression and enhancer activity. Four principal findings emerge: (i) largely static chromatin frameworks still gate differential gene output, with up-regulated loci most affected; (ii) enhancer-promoter contact strength covaries with transcriptional amplitude; (iii) 127 genes gain expression concomitant with increased chromatin contacts; and (iv) progression-associated genes acquire altered histone marks at distal enhancers that remain tethered by stable loops. While the conclusions are broadly supported, methodological and analytical refinements are required.

      (1) Model representativeness.<br /> The long-term culture-adapted MCF10 genome harbours extensive aneuploidies and translocations. Validation of key COL12A1/WNT5A loop dynamics in an independent breast-cancer line (e.g., MDA-MB-231, T47D) or in patient-derived organoids/PDX models would strengthen generalizability.

      (2) The study remains purely correlative; no perturbation experiments are conducted to demonstrate causal roles of chromatin loops on gene expression. CRISPR interference (CRISPR-Cas9-KRAB/HDAC) or enhancer deletion/inversion should be applied to 3-5 pivotal loops (e.g., COL12A1, WNT5A) to test their impact on target-gene expression and cellular phenotypes (e.g., proliferation, migration).

      (3) The manuscript lacks integration with clinical datasets. Integrate TCGA-BRCA data to assess whether elevated COL12A1/WNT5A expression associates with overall survival (OS) or distant metastasis-free survival (DMFS).

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors tackle an important problem: defining the topological changes that occur during tumorigenesis. To study this, they use an established stepwise cell model of breast cancer. A strength of their study is a careful, robust differential analysis of topological features across each cell state, which is presented clearly and rigorously. They define changes in compartmentalization, TAD structure, and chromatin looping. Intriguingly, when the authors integrate differential gene expression with chromatin looping, they see that most differentially regulated genes are not involved in loop changes, suggesting that changes in promoter or enhancer chromatin marks may play a bigger role in regulating transcription than differential loops. The differential topology analysis and its integration with transcription is very well done- one of the best versions of this I have read in the 3D genome field! However, the paper is framed largely as a cancer biology study, and it teaches us much less about this. I am worried that some of the trends for each topologic feature are not going to be consistent across the pre-malignant-malignant-metastatic spectrum and would like the authors to soften some of their claims a bit regarding how this clarifies our understanding of cancer evolution.

      Weaknesses:

      Major Concerns:

      (1) The integration of gene expression and chromatin loops is intriguing. The authors' differential analysis, however, omits consideration of genes that are on and simply further upregulated versus genes that transition on/off or off/on. It would be nice to see the authors break out looping patterns for these two different patterns of regulation, as it may be instructive regarding the rules for how EP loops govern transcription.

      (2) Given the paucity of differential loops at the majority of genes whose expression changes, the authors should examine chromatin subcompartments, as these may associate more with differential transcription.

      (3) The authors could push their TAD analysis further by integrating it with transcription. Can they look at genes and their enhancers that span these altered boundaries to see if these shifts impact transcription?

      (4) The progression of cancer critically goes from a benign -> pre-malignant -> malignant -> metastatic series of steps. The AT1 line is described as 'premalignant' and thus the authors' series omits a malignant line. While I think adding such a sample is an unreasonable request at this point (as it would have had to have been studied in 'batch' with these other samples), the authors should acknowledge that they omit this step and spend some time discussing the genetic, morphologic, and phenotypic features for their 3 conditions. The images in Figure 1S aren't particularly useful- they don't tell the reader that these cells are malignant/benign. The karyotypic data are intriguing but not fully analyzed, so it is hard to know what true phenotype these cells represent. For example, malignant means DCIS/invasive carcinoma - so then what does this pre-malignant cell model represent? The described alteration in the AT1 line is a Ras oncogene, so in some sense, the transition to this line really is just +/- Ras. The authors could spend some time thinking about the effects of Ras specifically on the 3D genome.

    1. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Ueno et al. described substantial changes in the Afadin knockout retina. These changes include decreased numbers of rods and cones, an increased number of bipolar cells, and disrupted somatic and synaptic organization of the outer limiting membrane, outer nuclear layer, outer plexiform layer. In contrast, the number and organization of amacrine cells and retinal ganglion cells remain relatively intact. They also observed changes in ERG responses, RGC receptive fields and functions, and visual behaviors. The morphological and function characterization of retinal cell types and laminations is detailed and relatively comprehensive.

    2. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The question of how central nervous system lamination defects affect functional integrity is an interesting yet debated topic. The authors investigated the role of afadin, a key adherens junction scaffolding protein, in retinal lamination and function using a retina-specific conditional knockout mouse model. Their findings show that the loss of Afadin caused severe outer retinal lamination defects, disrupting photoreceptor morphology, synapse numbers, and cell positioning, as demonstrated by histological analysis. Despite these structural impairments, retinal function was partially preserved: mERG detected small a- and b-waves, retinal ganglion cells responded to light, and behavioral tests confirmed residual visual function. This research offers new insights into the relationship between retinal lamination and neural circuit function, suggesting that altered retinal morphology does not completely eliminate the capacity for visual information processing.

      Strengths:

      The study effectively employs the well-organized laminar structure of the retina as an accessible model for investigating afadin's role in lamination within the central nervous system. High-quality histological, immunostaining, and electron microscopy images clearly reveal structural defects in the conditional knockout mice. The revised manuscript significantly enhances the findings by incorporating robust quantitative analyses of cell positioning, retinal thickness, and cell numbers, as well as new assessments of developmental defects. Additionally, new behavioral tests, including the optomotor response and visual cliff tests, have been introduced. Together with electrophysiological recordings, these additions compellingly demonstrate the partial preservation of visual function despite severe structural disruptions.

      Weaknesses:

      Overall, the study of the mechanisms remains weak. While the authors addressed concerns about molecular mechanisms by examining cell proliferation potentially related to Notch and Wnt signaling (Figure S6C, lines 868-870), the findings are largely negative (no significant changes in progenitor cell numbers), and the discussion of alternative pathways remains speculative.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      I applaud the authors' for providing a thorough response to my comments from the first round of review. The authors' have addressed the points I raised on the interpretation of the behavioral results as well as the validation of the model (fit to the data) by conducting new analyses, acknowledging the limitations where required and providing important counterpoints. As a result of this process, the manuscript has considerably improved. I have no further comments and recommend this manuscript for publication.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript proposes that the use of a latent cause model for assessment of memory-based tasks may provide improved early detection in Alzheimer's Disease as well as more differentiated mapping of behavior to underlying causes. To test the validity of this model, the authors use a previously described knock-in mouse model of AD and subject the mice to several behaviors to determine whether the latent cause model may provide informative predictions regarding changes in the observed behaviors. They include a well-established fear learning paradigm in which distinct memories are believed to compete for control of behavior. More specifically, it's been observed that animals undergoing fear learning and subsequent fear extinction develop two separate memories for the acquisition phase and the extinction phase, such that the extinction does not simply 'erase' the previously acquired memory. Many models of learning require the addition of a separate context or state to be added during the extinction phase and are typically modeled by assuming the existence of a new state at the time of extinction. The Niv research group, Gershman et al. 2017, have shown that the use of a latent cause model applied to this behavior can elegantly predict the formation of latent states based on a Bayesian approach, and that these latent states can facilitate the persistence of the acquisition and extinction memory independently. The authors of this manuscript leverage this approach to test whether deficits in production of the internal states, or the inference and learning of those states, may be disrupted in knock-in mice that show both a build-up of amyloid-beta plaques and a deterioration in memory as the mice age.

      Strengths:

      I think the authors' proposal to leverage the latent cause model and test whether it can lead to improved assessments in an animal model of AD is a promising approach for bridging the gap between clinical and basic research. The authors use a promising mouse model and apply this to a paradigm in which the behavior and neurobiology are relatively well understood - an ideal situation for assessing how a disease state may impact both the neurobiology and behavior. The latent cause model has the potential to better connect observed behavior to underlying causes and may pave a road for improved mapping of changes in behavior to neurobiological mechanisms in diseases such as AD.<br /> The authors also compare the latent cause model to the Rescorla-Wagner model and a latent state model allowing for better assessment of the latent cause model as a strong model for assessing reinstatement.

      Weaknesses:

      I have several substantial concerns which I've detailed below. These include important details on how the behavior was analyzed, how the model was used to assess the behavior, and the interpretations that have been made based on the model.<br /> (1) There is substantial data to suggest that during fear learning in mice separate memories develop for the acquisition and extinction phases, with the acquisition memory becoming more strongly retrieved during spontaneous recovery and reinstatement. The Gershman paper, cited by the authors, shows how the latent causal model can predict this shift in latent causes by allowing for the priors to decay over time, thereby increasing the posterior of the acquisition memory at the time of spontaneous recovery. In this manuscript, the authors suggest a similar mechanism of action for reinstatement, yet the model does not appear to return to the acquisition memory after reinstatement, at least based on the simulation and examples shown in figures 1 and 3. More specifically, in figure 1, the authors indicate that the posterior probability of the latent cause, z<sub>A</sub> (the putative acquisition memory), increases, partially leading to reinstatement. This does not appear to be the case as test 3 (day 36) appears to have similar posterior probabilities for z<sub>A</sub> as well as similar weights for the CS as compared to the last days of extinction. Rather, the model appears to mainly modify the weights in the most recent latent cause, z<sub>B</sub> - the putative the 'extinction state', during reinstatement. The authors suggest that previous experimental data have indicated that spontaneous recovery or reinstatement effects are due to an interaction of the acquisition and extinction memory. These studies have shown that conditioned responding at a later time point after extinction is likely due to a balance between the acquisition memory and the extinction memory, and that this balance can shift towards the acquisition memory naturally during spontaneous recovery, or through artificial activation of the acquisition memory or inhibition of the extinction memory (see Lacagnina et al. for example). Here the authors show that the same latent cause learned during extinction, z<sub>B</sub>, appears to dominate during the learning phase of reinstatement, with rapid learning to the context - the weight for the context goes up substantially on day 35 - in z<sub>B</sub>. This latent cause, z<sub>B</sub>, dominates at the reinstatement test, and due to the increased associative strength between the context and shock, there is a strong CR. For the simulation shown in figure 1, it's not clear why a latent cause model is necessary for this behavior. This leads to the next point.

      (2) The authors compared the latent cause model to the Rescorla-Wagner model. This is very commendable, particularly since the latent cause model builds upon the RW model, so it can serve as an ideal test for whether a more simplified model can adequately predict the behavior. The authors show that the RW model cannot successfully predict the increased CR during reinstatement (Appendix figure 1). Yet there are some issues with the way the authors have implemented this comparison:<br /> (2A) The RW model is a simplified version of the latent cause model and so should be treated as a nested model when testing, or at a minimum, the number of parameters should be taken into account when comparing the models using a method such as the Bayesian Information Criterion, BIC.<br /> (2B) The RW model provides the associative strength between stimuli and does not necessarily require a linear relationship between V and the CR. This is the case in the original RW model as well as in the LCM. To allow for better comparison between the models, the authors should be modeling the CR in the same manner (using the same probit function) in both models. In fact, there are many instances in which a sigmoid has been applied to RW associative strengths to predict CRs. I would recommend modeling CRs in the RW as if there is just one latent cause. Or perhaps run the analysis for the LCM with just one latent cause - this would effectively reduce the LCM to RW and keep any other assumptions identical across the models.<br /> (2C) In the paper, the model fits for the alphas in the RW model are the same across the groups. Were the alphas for the two models kept as free variables? This is an important question as it gets back to the first point raised. Because the modeling of the reinstatement behavior with the LCM appears to be mainly driven by latent cause z<sub>B</sub>, the extinction memory, it may be possible to replicate the pattern of results without requiring a latent cause model. For example, the 12-month-old App NL-G-F mice behavior may have a deficit in learning about the context. Within the RW model, if the alpha for context is set to zero for those mice, but kept higher for the other groups, say alpha_context = 0.8, the authors could potentially observe the same pattern of discrimination indices in figure 2G and 2H at test. Because the authors don't explicitly state which parameters might be driving the change in the DI, the authors should show in some way that their results cannot simply be due to poor contextual learning in the 12 month old App NL-G-F mice, as this can presumably be predicted by the RW model. The authors' model fits using RW don't show this, but this is because they don't consider this possibility that the alpha for context might be disrupted in the 12-month-old App NL-G-F mice. Of course, using the RW model with these alphas won't lead to as nice of fits of the behavior across acquisition, extinction, and reinstatement as the authors' LCM, the number of parameters are substantially reduced in the RW model. Yet the important pattern of the DI would be replicated with the RW model (if I'm not mistaken), which is the important test for assessment of reinstatement.

      (3) As stated by the authors in the introduction, the advantage of the fear learning approach is that the memory is modified across the acquisition-extinction-reinstatement phases. Although perhaps not explicitly stated by the authors, the post-reinstatement test (test 3) is the crucial test for whether there is reactivation of a previously stored memory, with the general argument being that the reinvigorated response to the CS can't simply be explained by relearning the CS-US pairing, because re-exposure the US alone leads to increase response to the CS at test. Of course there are several explanations for why this may occur, particularly when also considering the context as a stimulus. This is what I understood to be the justification for the use of a model, such as the latent cause model, that may better capture and compare these possibilities within a single framework. As such, it is critical to look at the level of responding to both the context alone and to the CS. It appears that the authors only look at the percent freezing during the CS, and it is not clear whether this is due to the contextual-US learning during the US re-exposure or to increased responding to the CS - presumably caused by reactivation of the acquisition memory. The authors do perform a comparison between the preCS and CS period, but it is not clear whether this is taken into account in the LCM. For example, the instance of the model shown in figure 1 indicates that the 'extinction cause', or cause z6, develops a strong weight for the context during the reinstatement phase of presenting the shock alone. This state then leads to increased freezing during the final CS probe test as shown in the figure. If they haven't already, I think the authors must somehow incorporate these different phases (CS vs ITI) into their model, particularly since this type of memory retrieval that depends on assessing latent states is specifically why the authors justified using the latent causal model. In more precise terms, it's not clear whether the authors incorporate a preCS/ITI period each day the cue is presented as a vector of just the context in addition to the CS period in which the vector contains both the context and the CS. Based on the description, it seemed to me that they only model the CRs during the CS period on days when the CS is presented, and thereby the context is only ever modeled on its own (as just the context by itself in the vector) on extinction days when the CS is not presented. If they are modeling both timepoints each day that the CS I presented, then I would recommend explicitly stating this in the methods section.

      (4) The authors fit the model using all data points across acquisition and learning. As one of the other reviewers has highlighted, it appears that there is a high chance for overfitting the data with the LCM. Of course, this would result in much better fits than models with substantially fewer free parameters, such as the RW model. As mentioned above, the authors should use a method that takes into account the number of parameters, such as the BIC.

      (5) The authors have stated that they do not think the Barnes maze task can be modeled with the LCM. Whether or not this is the case, if the authors do not model this data with the LCM, the Barnes maze data doesn't appear valuable to the main hypothesis. The authors suggest that more sophisticated models such as the LCM may be beneficial for early detection of diseases such as Alzheimer's, so the Barnes maze data is not valuable for providing evidence of this hypothesis. Rather, the authors make an argument that the memory deficits in the Barnes maze mimic the reinstatement effects providing support that memory is disrupted similarly in these mice. Although, the authors state that the deficits in memory retrieval are similar across the two tasks, the authors are not explicit as to the precise deficits in memory retrieval in the reinstatement task - it's a combination of overgeneralizing latent causes during acquisition, poor learning rate, over differentiation of the stimuli.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This paper seeks to identify underlying mechanisms contributing to memory deficits observed in Alzheimer's disease (AD) mouse models. By understanding these mechanisms, they hope to uncover insights into subtle cognitive changes early in AD to inform interventions for early-stage decline.

      Strengths:

      The paper provides a comprehensive exploration of memory deficits in an AD mouse model, covering early and late stages of the disease. The experimental design was robust, confirming age-dependent increases in Aβ plaque accumulation in the AD model mice and using multiple behavior tasks that collectively highlighted difficulties in maintaining multiple competing memory cues, with deficits most pronounced in older mice.

      In the fear acquisition, extinction, and reinstatement task, AD model mice exhibited a significantly higher fear response after acquisition compared to controls, as well as a greater drop in fear response during reinstatement. These findings suggest that AD mice struggle to retain the fear memory associated with the conditioned stimulus, with the group differences being more pronounced in the older mice.

      In the reversal Barnes maze task, the AD model mice displayed a tendency to explore the maze perimeter rather than the two potential target holes, indicating a failure to integrate multiple memory cues into their strategy. This contrasted with the control mice, which used the more confirmatory strategy of focusing on the two target holes. Despite this, the AD mice were quicker to reach the target hole, suggesting that their impairments were specific to memory retrieval rather than basic task performance.

      The authors strengthened their findings by analyzing their data with a leading computational model, which describes how animals balance competing memories. They found that AD mice showed somewhat of a contradiction: a tendency to both treat trials as more alike than they are (lower α) and similar stimuli as more distinct than they are (lower σx) compared to controls.

      Weaknesses:

      While conceptually solid, the model struggles to fit the data and to support the key hypothesis about AD mice's inability to retain competing memories. These issues are evident in Figure 3:

      (1) The model misses trends in the data, including the gradual learning of fear in all groups during acquisition, the absence of a fear response at the start of the experiment, and the faster return of fear during reinstatement compared to the gradual learning of fear during acquisition. It also underestimates the increase in fear at the start of day 2 of extinction, particularly in controls.

      (2) The model explains the higher fear response in controls during reinstatement largely through a stronger association to the context formed during the unsignaled shock phase, rather than to any memory of the conditioned stimulus from acquisition (as seen in Figure 3C). In the experiment, however, this memory does seem to be important for explaining the higher fear response in controls during reinstatement (as seen in Author Response Figure 3). The model does show a necessary condition for memory retrieval, which is that controls rely more on the latent causes from acquisition. But this alone is not sufficient, since the associations within that cause may have been overwritten during extinction. The Rescorla-Wagner model illustrates this point: it too uses the latent cause from acquisition (as it only ever uses a single cause across phases) but does not retain the original stimulus-shock memory, updating and overwriting it continuously. Similarly, the latent cause model may reuse a cause from acquisition without preserving its original stimulus-shock association.

      These issues lead to potential overinterpretation of the model parameters. The differences in α and σx are being used to make claims about cognitive processes (e.g., overgeneralization vs. over differentiation), but the model itself does not appear to capture these processes accurately.

      The authors could benefit from a model that better matches the data and captures the retention and retrieval of fear memories across phases. While they explored alternatives, including the Rescorla-Wagner model and a latent state model, these showed no meaningful improvement in fit. This highlights a broader issue: these models are well-motivated but may not fully capture observed behavior.

      Conclusion:

      Overall, the data support the authors' hypothesis that AD model mice struggle to retain competing memories, with the effect becoming more pronounced with age. While I believe the right computational model could highlight these differences, the current models fall short in doing so.

    1. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Septin caging has emerged as one of the innate immune response of eukaryotic cells to infections by intracellular bacteria. This fascinating assembly of eukaryotic proteins into complex structures restricts bacteria motility within the cytoplasm of host cells, thereby facilitating recognition by cytosolic sensors and components of the autophagy machinery. Given the different types of septin caging that have been described thus far, a single cell, unbiased approach to quantify and characterise septin recruitment at bacteria is important to fully grasp the role and function of caging. Thus, the authors have developed an automated image analysis pipeline allowing bacterial segmentation and classification of septin cages that will be very useful in the future, applied to study the role of host and bacterial factors, compare different bacterial strains or even compare infections by clinical isolates.

      Strengths:

      The authors developed a solid pipeline that has been thoroughly validated. When tested on infected cells, automated analysis corroborated previous observations and allowed the unbiased quantification of the different types of septin cages as well as the correlation between caging and bacterial metabolic activity. This approach will prove an essential asset in the further characterisation of septin cages for future studies.

      Weaknesses:

      As the main aim of the manuscript is to described the newly developed analysis pipeline, the results illustrated in the manuscript are essentially descriptive. The developed pipeline seems exceptionally efficient in recognising septin cages in infected cells but its application for a broader purpose or field of study remains limited.

    2. Reviewer #4 (Public review):

      Summary

      In this study, López-Jiménez and colleagues demonstrate the utility of using high-content microscopy in dissecting host and bacterial determinants that play a role in the establishment of infection using Shigella flexneri as a model. The manuscript nicely identifies that infection with Shigella results in a block to DNA replication and protein synthesis. At the same time, the host responds, in part, via the entrapment of Shigella in septin cages.

      Strengths:

      The main strength of this manuscript is its technical aspects. They nicely demonstrate how an automated microscopy pipeline coupled with artificial intelligence can be used to gain new insights regarding elements of bacterial pathogenesis, using Shigella flexneri as a model system. Using this pipeline enabled the investigators to enhance the field's general understanding regarding the role of septin cages in responding to invading Shigella. This platform should be of interest to those who study a variety of intracellular microbial pathogens.

      Another strength of the manuscript is the demonstration - using cell biology-based approaches- that infection with Shigella blocks DNA replication and protein synthesis. These observations nicely dovetail with the prior findings of other groups. Nevertheless, their clever click-chemistry-based approaches provide visual evidence of these phenomena and should interest many.

      Weaknesses:

      There are two main weaknesses of this work. First, the studies are limited to findings obtained using a single immortalized cell line. It is appreciated that HeLa cells serve as an excellent model for studying aspects of Shigella pathogenesis and host responses. However, it would be nice to see that similar observations are observed with an epithelial cell line of intestinal, preferably colonic origin, and eventually, with a non-immortalized cell line, although it is appreciated that the latter studies are beyond the scope of this work.

      The other weakness is that the studies are minimally mechanistic. For example, the investigators have data to suggest that infection with Shigella leads to an arrest in DNA replication and protein synthesis; however, no follow-up studies have been conducted to determine how these host cell processes are disabled. Interestingly, Zhang and colleagues recently identified that the Shigella OspC effectors target eukaryotic translation initiation factor 3 to block host cell translation (PMID: 38368608).

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, the authors attempt to devise general rules for aptamer design based on structure and sequence features. The main system they are testing is an aptamer targeting a viral sequence.

      Strengths:

      The method combines a series of well-established protocols, including docking, MD, and a lot of system-specific knowledge, to design several new versions of the Ta aptamer with improved binding affinity.

      Weaknesses:

      The approach requires a lot of existing knowledge and, importantly, an already known aptamer, which presumably was found with Selex. In addition, although the aptamer may have a stronger binding affinity, it is not clear if any of it has any additional useful properties such as stability, etc.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript proposes a workflow for discovering and optimizing RNA aptamers, with application in the optimization of a SARS-CoV-2 RBD. The authors took a previously identified RNA aptamer, computationally docked it into one specific RBD structure, and searched for variants with higher predicted affinity. The variants were subsequently tested for RBD binding using gel retardation assays and competition with antibodies, and one was found to be a stronger binder by about three-fold than the founding aptamer.

      Overall, this would be an interesting study if it were performed with truly high-affinity aptamers, and specificity was shown for RBD or several RBD variants.

      Strengths:

      The computational workflow appears to mostly correctly find stronger binders, though not de novo binders.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Antibody competition assays are reported with RBD at 40 µM, aptamer at 5 µM, and a titration of antibody between 0 and 1.2 µg. This approach does not make sense. The antibody concentration should be reported in µM. An estimation of the concentration is 0-8 pmol (from 0-1.2 µg), but that's not a concentration, so it is unknown whether enough antibody molecules were present to saturate all RBD molecules, let alone whether they could have displaced all aptamers.

      (2) These are not by any means high-affinity aptamers. The starting sequence has an estimated (not measured, since the titration is incomplete) KD of 110 µM. That's really the same as non-specific binding for an interaction between an RNA and a protein. This makes the title of the manuscript misleading. No high-affinity aptamer is presented in this study. If the docking truly presented a bound conformation of an aptamer to a protein, a sub-micromolar Kd would be expected, based on the number of interactions that they make.

      (3) The binding energies estimated from calculations and those obtained from the gel-shift experiments are vastly different, as calculated from the Kd measurements, making them useless for comparison, except for estimating relative affinities.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      This is my first review of this manuscript. The authors included previous reviews for a different journal with a length of 90 and 39 pages; I did not review this reply in my assessment of the paper itself. Influenza prediction is not my area of expertise.

      A major concern is that the model is trained in the midst of the COVID-19 pandemic and its associated restrictions and validated on 2023 data. The situation before, during, and after COVID is fluid, and one may not be representative of the other. The situation in 2023 may also not have been normal and reflective of 2024 onward, both in terms of the amount of testing (and positives) and measures taken to prevent the spread of these types of infections. A further worry is that the retrospective prospective split occurred in October 2020, right in the first year of COVID, so it will be impossible to compare both cohorts to assess whether grouping them is sensible.

      The outcome of interest is the number of confirmed influenza cases. This is not only a function of weather, but also of the amount of testing. The amount of testing is also a function of historical patterns. This poses the real risk that the model confirms historical opinions through increased testing in those higher-risk periods. Of course, the models could also be run to see how meteorological factors affect testing and the percentage of positive tests. The results only deal with the number of positive (only the overall number of tests is noted briefly), which means there is no way to assess how reasonable and/or variable these other measures are. This is especially concerning as there was massive testing for respiratory viruses during COVID in many places, possibly including China.

      (1) Although the authors note a correlation between influenza and the weather factors. The authors do not discuss some of the high correlations between weather factors (e.g., solar radiation and UV index). Because of the many weather factors, those plots are hard to parse.

      (2) The authors do not actually compare the results of both methods and what the LSTM adds.

      Minor comments:

      (3) The methods are long and meandering. They could be cleaned up and shortened. E.g., there is no need for 30 lines on PCR testing; the study area should come before the study design. The authors discuss similar elements in multiple places; this whole section can be shortened considerably without affecting the content.

      (4) How reliable is the "Our Word in Data" website for subnational coverage of restrictions? Some of the authors are from Putian and should be able to confirm the accuracy for both studied areas.

      (5) Figure 2A is hard to parse; it would make more sense to plot these as line plots (y=count, x=month).

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The study aimed to assess the associations between meteorological drivers and influenza is important although not new. The authors used only 6 years of surveillance data and deep learning models, combining distributed lag non-linear models (DLNM) with Bayesian-optimized LSTM neural networks for predictive modeling. The key interest in this area is to explore the subtropical locations, where influenza is less common and circulates year-round. The authors further claimed that such an association could be able to provide an early warning in the community. In this direction, the current manuscript has several scopes of improvements and clarification of the claims, as I list here.

      Strengths:

      Study design based on a prospective cohort to analyse the data for retrospective outcomes.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The rationale of the study is not clearly stated.

      (2) Several issues with methodological and data integration should be clarified.

      (3) Validation of the models is not presented clearly.

      (4) The claim for providing tools for 'early warning' was not validated by analysis and results.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Nahas et al. investigated the roles of herpes simplex virus 1 (HSV-1) structural proteins using correlative cryo-light microscopy and soft X-ray tomography. The authors generated nine viral variants with deletions or mutations in genes encoding structural proteins. They employed a chemical fixation-free approach to study native-like events during viral assembly, enabling observation of a wider field of view compared to cryo-ET. The study effectively combined virology, cell biology, and structural biology to investigate the roles of viral proteins in virus assembly and budding.

      Strengths:

      (1) The study presented a novel approach to studying viral assembly in cellulo.

      (2) The authors generated nine mutant viruses to investigate the roles of essential proteins in nuclear egress and cytoplasmic envelopment.

      (3) The use of correlative imaging with cryoSIM and cryoSXT allowed for the study of viral assembly in a near-native state and in 3D.

      (4) The study identified the roles of VP16, pUL16, pUL21, pUL34, and pUS3 in nuclear egress.

      (5) The authors demonstrated that deletion of VP16, pUL11, gE, pUL51, or gK inhibits cytoplasmic envelopment.

      (6) The manuscript is well-written, clearly describing findings, methods, and experimental design.

      (7) The figures and data presentation are of good quality.

      (8) The study effectively correlated light microscopy and X-ray tomography to follow virus assembly, providing a valuable approach for studying other viruses and cellular events.

      (9) The research is a valuable starting point for investigating viral assembly using more sophisticated methods like cryo-ET with FIB-milling.

      (10) The study proposes a detailed assembly mechanism and tracks the contributions of studied proteins to the assembly process.

      (11) The study includes all necessary controls and tests for the influence of fluorescent proteins.

      Weaknesses:

      Overall, the manuscript does not have any major weaknesses, just a few minor comments, which were mostly solved in the revised version of the manuscript.

      Comments on the latest version:

      I reviewed the responses and the updated manuscript, and I am very pleased with how the authors have revised it. The manuscript was already strong, but with the addition of the summary table and the separated images, it is now excellent.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      For centuries, humans have been developing methods to see ever smaller objects, such as cells and their contents. This has included studies of viruses and their interactions with host cells during processes extending from virion structure to the complex interactions between viruses and their host cells: virion entry, virus replication and virion assembly, and release of newly constructed virions. Recent developments have enabled simultaneous application of fluorescence-based detection and intracellular localization of molecules of interest in the context of sub-micron resolution imaging of cellular structures by electron microscopy.

      The submission by Nahas et al., extends the state-of-the-art for visualization of important aspects of herpesvirus (HSV-1 in this instance) virion morphogenesis, a complex process that involves virus genome replication, and capsid assembly and filling in the nucleus, transport of the nascent nucleocapsid and some associated tegument proteins through the inner and outer nuclear membranes to the cytoplasm, orderly association of several thousand mostly viral proteins with the capsid to form the virion's tegument, envelopment of the tegumented capsid at a virus-tweaked secretory vesicle or at the plasma membrane, and release of mature virions at the plasma membrane.

      In this groundbreaking study, cells infected with HSV-1 mutants that express fluorescently tagged versions of capsid (eYFP-VP26) and tegument (gM-mCherry) proteins were visualized with 3D correlative structured illumination microscopy and X-ray tomography. The maturation and egress pathways thus illuminated were studied further in infections with fluorescently tagged viruses lacking one of nine viral proteins.

      Strengths:

      This outstanding paper meets the journal's definitions of Landmark, Fundamental, Important, Valuable, and Useful. The work is also Exceptional, Compelling, Convincing, and Solid. The work is a tour de force of classical and state-of-the-art molecular and cellular virology. Beautiful images accompanied by appropriate statistical analyses and excellent figures. The numerous complex issues addressed are explained in a clear and coordinated manner; the sum of what was learned is greater than the sum of the parts. Impacts go well beyond cytomegalovirus and the rest of the herpesviruses, to other viruses and cell biology in general.

      Comments on the latest version:

      This is a very nice paper. The authors responded affirmatively to the suggestions and questions of the reviewers.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Kamal L. Nahas et al. demonstrated that pUL16, pUL21, pUL34, VP16, and pUS3 are involved in the egress of the capsids from the nucleous, since mutant viruses ΔpUL16, ΔpUL21, ΔUL34, ΔVP16, and ΔUS3 HSV-1 show nuclear egress attenuation determined by measuring the nuclear:cytoplasmic ratio of the capsids, the dfParental, or the mutants. Then, they showed that gM-mCherry+ endomembrane association and capsid clustering were different in pUL11, pUL51, gE, gK, and VP16 mutants. Furthermore, the 3D view of cytoplasmic budding events suggests an envelopment mechanism where capsid budding into spherical/ellipsoidal vesicles drives the envelopment.

      Strengths:

      The authors employed both structured illumination microscopy and cellular ultrastructure analysis to examine the same infected cells, using cryo-soft-X-ray tomography to capture images. This combination, set here for the first time, enabled the authors to obtain holistic data regarding a biological process, as a viral assembly. Using this approach, the researchers studied various stages of HSV-1 assembly. For this, they constructed a dual-fluorescently labelled recombinant virus, consisting of eYFP-tagged capsids and mCherry-tagged envelopes, allowing for the independent identification of both unenveloped and enveloped particles. They then constructed nine mutants, each targeting a single viral protein known to be involved in nuclear egress and envelopment in the cytoplasm, using this dual-fluorescent as the parental one. The experimental setting, both the microscopic and the virological, is robust and well-controlled. The manuscript is well-written, and the data generated is robust and consistent with previous observations made in the field.

      I congratulate the authors. The work is robust, and I personally highlight the way they managed to include others' results merged among their own, providing a complete view of the story.

      Comments on the latest version:

      I reviewed the responses and the updated manuscript, and I agree with the reviewer's #1 words: "The manuscript was already strong, but with the addition of the summary table and the separated images, it is now excellent."

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Filamentous fungi are established work horses in biotechnology with Aspergillus oryzae as a prominent example with a thousand-year of history. Still the cell biology and biochemical properties of the production strains is not well understood. The paper of the Takeshita group describes the change in nuclear numbers and correlate it to different production capacities. They used microfluidic devices to really correlate the production with nuclear numbers. In addition, they used microdissection to understand expression profile changes and found an increase of ribosomes. The analysis of two genes involved in cell volume control in S. pombe did not reveal conclusive answers to explain the phenomenon. It appears that it is a multi-trait phenotype. Finally, they identified SNPs in many industrial strains and tried to correlate them to the capability of increasing their nuclear numbers.

      The methods used in the paper range from high quality cell biology, Raman spectroscopy to atomic force and electron microscopy and from laser microdissection to the use of microfluidic devices to study individual hyphae.

      This is a very interesting, biotechnologically relevant paper with the application of excellent cell biology.

      Comments on revised version:

      The authors addressed all suggestions satisfactorily.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In the study presented by Itani and colleagues it is shown that some strains of Aspergillus oryzae - especially those used industrially for the production of sake and soy sauce - develop hyphae with a significantly increased number of nuclei and cell volume over time. These thick hyphae are formed by branching from normal hyphae and grow faster and therefore dominate the colonies. The number of nuclei positively correlates with the thicker hyphae and also the amount of secreted enzymes. The addition of nutrients such as yeast extract or certain amino acids enhanced this effect. Genome and transcriptome analyses identified genes, including rseA, that are associated with the increased number of nuclei and enzyme production. The authors conclude from their data involvement of glycosyltransferases, calcium channels and the tor regulatory cascade in regulation of cell volume and number of nuclei. Thicker hyphae and an increased number of nuclei was also observed in high-production strains of other industrially used fungi such as Trichoderma reesei and Penicillium chrysogenum, leading to the hypothesis that the mentioned phenotypes are characteristic of production strains which is of significant interest for fungal biotechnology.

      Strengths:

      The study is very comprehensive and involves application of divers state-of-the-art cell biological, biochemical and genetic methods. Overall, the data are properly controlled and analyzed, and the figures and movies are of excellent quality.The results are particularly interesting with regard to the elucidation of molecular mechanisms that regulate the size of fungal hyphae and the number of nuclei. For this, the authors have discovered a very good model: (regular) strains with a low number of nuclei and strains with high number of nuclei. Also, the results can be expected to be of interest for the further optimization of industrially relevant filamentous fungi.

      In the revision the authors addressed all my comments and as a result produced an even stronger study.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors seek to determine the underlying traits that support the exceptional capacity of Aspergillus oryzae to secrete enzymes and heterologous proteins. To do so, they leverage the availability of multiple domesticated isolates of A. oryzae along with other Aspergillus species to perform comparative imaging and genomic analysis.

      Strengths:

      The strength of this study lies in the use of multifaceted approaches to identify significant differences in hyphal morphology that correlate with enzyme secretion, which is then followed by the use of genomics to identify candidate functions that underlie these differences.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors addressed all suggestions satisfactorily.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Overall, the manuscript reveals the role for actin polymerization to drive fusion of myoblasts during adult muscle regeneration. This pathway regulates fusion in many contexts, but whether it was conserved in adult muscle regeneration remained unknown. Robust genetic tools and histological analyses were used to convincingly support the claims.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      To fuse, differentiated muscle cells must rearrange their cytoskeleton and assemble actin-enriched cytoskeletal structures. These actin foci are proposed to generate mechanical forces necessary to drive close membrane apposition and the fusion pore formation. While the study of these actin-rich structures has been conducted mainly in drosophila and in vertebrate embryonic development, the present manuscript present clear evidence this mechanism is necessary for fusion of adult muscle stem cells in vivo, in mice. The data presented here clearly demonstrate that ARP2/3 and SCAR/WAVE complexes are required for differentiating satellite cells fusion into multinucleated myotubes, during skeletal muscle regeneration.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      The authors have satisfactorily addressed my inquiries. However, I had to look quite hard to find where they responded to my final comment regarding the potential role of Arpc2 post-fusion during myofiber growth and/or maintenance, which I eventually located on page 7. I would appreciate it if the authors could state this point more explicitly, perhaps by adding a sentence such as "However, we cannot rule out the possibility that Arpc2 may also play a role in....." to improve clarity of communication.

      While I understood from the original version that this issue falls beyond the immediate scope of the study, I believe it is important to adopt a more cautious and rigorous interpretative framework, especially given the widespread use of this experimental approach. In particular, when a gene could potentially have additional roles in myofibers, it may be helpful to explicitly acknowledge that possibility. Even if Arpc2 may not necessarily be one of them, such roles cannot be fully excluded without direct testing.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This computational modeling study builds on multiple previous lines of experimental and theoretical research to investigate how a single neuron can solve a nonlinear pattern classification task. The authors construct a detailed biophysical and morphological model of a single striatal medium spiny neuron, and endow excitatory and inhibitory synapses with dynamic synaptic plasticity mechanisms that are sensitive to (1) the presence or absence of a dopamine reward signal, and (2) spatiotemporal coincidence of synaptic activity in single dendritic branches. The latter coincidence is detected by voltage-dependent NMDA-type glutamate receptors, which can generate a type of dendritic spike referred to as a "plateau potential." In the absence of inhibitory plasticity, the proposed mechanisms result in good performance on a nonlinear classification task when specific input features are segregated and clustered onto individual branches, but reduced performance when input features are randomly distributed across branches. Interestingly, adding inhibitory plasticity improves classification performance even when input features are randomly distributed.

      Strengths:

      The integrative aspect of this study is its major strength. It is challenging to relate low-level details such as electrical spine compartmentalization, extrasynaptic neurotransmitter concentrations, dendritic nonlinearities, spatial clustering of correlated inputs, and plasticity of excitatory and inhibitory synapses to high-level computations such as nonlinear feature classification. Due to high simulation costs, it is rare to see highly biophysical and morphological models used for learning studies that require repeated stimulus presentations over the course of a training procedure. The study aspires to prove the principle that experimentally-supported biological mechanisms can explain complex learning.

      Weaknesses:

      The high level of complexity of each component of the model makes it difficult to gain an intuition for which aspects of the model are essential for its performance, or responsible for its poor performance under certain conditions. Stripping down some of the biophysical detail and comparing it to a simpler model may help better understand each component in isolation.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The study explores how single striatal projection neurons (SPNs) utilize dendritic nonlinearities to solve complex integration tasks. It introduces a calcium-based synaptic learning rule that incorporates local calcium dynamics and dopaminergic signals, along with metaplasticity to ensure stability for synaptic weights. Results show SPNs can solve the nonlinear feature binding problem and enhance computational efficiency through inhibitory plasticity in dendrites, emphasizing the significant computational potential of individual neurons. In summary, the study provides a more biologically plausible solution to single-neuron learning and gives further mechanical insights into complex computations at the single-neuron level.

      Strengths:

      The paper introduces a novel learning rule for training a single multicompartmental neuron model to perform nonlinear feature binding tasks (NFBP), highlighting two main strengths: the learning rule is local, calcium-based, and requires only sparse reward signals, making it highly biologically plausible, and it applies to detailed neuron models that effectively preserve dendritic nonlinearities, contrasting with many previous studies that use simplified models.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      This revision of the computational study by Mondal et al addresses several issues that I raised in the previous round of reviews and, as such, is greatly improved. The manuscript is more readable, its findings are more clearly described, and both the introduction and the discussion sections are tighter and more to the point. And thank you for addressing the three timescales of half activation/inactivation parameters. It makes the mechanism clearer.

      Some issues remain that I bring up below.

      Comment:

      I still have a bone to pick with the claim that "activity-dependent changes in channel voltage-dependence alone are insufficient to attain bursting". As I mentioned in my previous comment, this is also the case for the gmax values (channel density). If you choose the gmax's to be in a reasonable range, then the statement above is simply cannot be true. And if, in contrast, you choose the activation/inactivation parameters to be unreasonable, then no set of gmax's can produce proper activity. So I remain baffled what exactly is the point that the authors are trying to make.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, Mondal and co-authors present the development of a computational model of homeostatic plasticity incorporating activity-dependent regulation of gating properties (activation, inactivation) of ion channels. The authors show that, similar to what has been observed for activity-dependent regulation of ion channel conductances, implementing activity-dependent regulation of voltage sensitivity participates in the achievement of a target phenotype (bursting or spiking). The results however suggest that activity-dependent regulation of voltage sensitivity is not sufficient to allow this and needs to be associated with the regulation of ion channel conductances in order to reliably reach target phenotype. Although the implementation of this biologically relevant phenomenon is undeniably relevant, a few important questions are left unanswered.

      Strengths:

      (1) Implementing activity-dependent regulation of gating properties of ion channels is biologically relevant.

      (2) The modeling work appears to be well performed and provides results that are consistent with previous work performed by the same group.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The main question not addressed in the paper is the relative efficiency and/or participation of voltage-dependence regulation compared to channel conductance in achieving the expected pattern of activity. Is voltage-dependence participating to 50% or 10%. Although this is a difficult question to answer (and it might even be difficult to provide a number), it is important to determine whether channel conductance regulation remains the main parameter allowing the achievement of a precise pattern of activity (or its recovery after perturbation).

      (2) Another related question is whether the speed of recovery is significantly modified by implementing voltage-dependence regulation (it seems to be the case looking at Figure 3). More generally, I believe it would be important to give insights into the overall benefit of implementing voltage-dependence regulation, beyond its rather obvious biological relevance.

      (3) Along the same line, the conclusion about how voltage-dependence regulation and channel conductance regulation interact to provide the neuron with the expected activity pattern (summarized and illustrated in Figure 6) is rather qualitative. Consistent with my previous comments, one would expect some quantitative answers to this question, rather than an illustration that approximately places a solution in parameter space.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Mondal et al. use computational modeling to investigate how activity-dependent shifts in voltage-dependent (in)activation curves can complement changes in ion channel conductance to support homeostatic plasticity. While it is well established that the voltage-dependent properties of ion channels influence neuronal excitability, their potential role in homeostatic regulation, alongside conductance changes, has remained largely unexplored. The results presented here demonstrate that activity-dependent regulation of voltage dependence can interact with conductance plasticity to enable neurons to attain and maintain target activity patterns, in this case, intrinsic bursting. Notably, the timescale of these voltage-dependent shifts influences the final steady-state configuration of the model, shaping both channel parameters and activity features such as burst period and duration. A major conclusion of the study is that altering this timescale can seamlessly modulate a neuron's intrinsic properties, which the authors suggest may be a mechanism for adaptation to perturbations.

      While this conclusion is largely well-supported, additional analyses could help clarify its scope. For instance, the effects of timescale alterations are clearly demonstrated when the model transitions from an initial state that does not meet the target activity pattern to a new stable state. However, Fig. 6 and the accompanying discussion appear to suggest that changing the timescale alone is sufficient to shift neuronal activity more generally. It would be helpful to clarify that this effect primarily applies during periods of adaptation, such as neurodevelopment or in response to perturbations, and not necessarily once the system has reached a stable, steady state. As currently presented, the simulations do not test whether modifying the timescale can influence activity after the model has stabilized. In such conditions, changes in timescale are unlikely to affect network dynamics unless they somehow alter the stability of the solution, which is not shown here. That said, it seems plausible that real neurons experience ongoing small perturbations which, in conjunction with changes in timescale, could allow gradual shifts toward new solutions. This possibility is not discussed but could be a fruitful direction for future work.

      Editor's note: The authors have adequately addressed the concerns raised in the public reviews above, as well as the previous recommendations, and revised the manuscript where necessary.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      In this work, Ligneul and coauthors implemented diffusion-weighted MRS in young rats to follow longitudinally and in vivo the microstructural changes occurring during brain development. Diffusion-weighted MRS is here instrumental in assessing microstructure in a cell-specific manner, as opposed to the claimed gold-standard (manganese-enhanced MRI) that can only probe changes in brain volume. Differential microstructure and complexification of the cerebellum and the thalamus during rat brain development were observed non-invasively. In particular, lower metabolite ADC with increasing age were measured in both brain regions, reflecting increasing cellular restriction with brain maturation. Higher sphere (representing cell bodies) fraction for neuronal metabolites (total NAA, glutamate) and total creatine and taurine in the cerebellum compared to the thalamus were estimated, reflecting the unique structure of the cerebellar granular layer with a high density of cell bodies. Decreasing sphere fraction with age was observed in the cerebellum, reflecting the development of the dendritic tree of Purkinje cells and Bergmann glia. From morphometric analyses, the authors could probe non-monotonic branching evolution in the cerebellum, matching 3D representations of Purkinje cells expansion and complexification with age. Finally, the authors highlighted taurine as a potential new marker of cerebellar development.

      From a technical standpoint, this work clearly demonstrates the potential of diffusion-weighted MRS at probing microstructure changes of the developing brain non-invasively, paving the way for its application in pathological cases. Ligneul and coauthors also show that diffusion-weighted MRS acquisitions in neonates are feasible, despite the known technical challenges of such measurements, even in adult rats. They also provide all necessary resources to reproduce and build upon their work, which is highly valuable for the community.

      From a biological standpoint, claims are well supported by the microstructure parameters derived from advanced biophysical modelling of the diffusion MRS data.

      Specific strengths:

      (1) The interpretation of dMRS data in terms of cell-specific microstructure through advanced biophysical modelling (e.g. the sphere fraction, modelling the fraction of cell bodies versus neuronal or astrocytic processes) is a strong asset of the study, going beyond the more commonly used signal representation metrics such as the apparent diffusion coefficient, which lacks specificity to biological phenomena.

      (2) The fairly good data quality despite the complexity of the experimental framework should be praised: diffusion-weighted MRS was acquired in two brain regions (although not in the same animals) and longitudinally, in neonates, including data at high b-values and multiple diffusion times, which altogether constitutes a large-scale dataset of high value for the diffusion-weighted MRS community.

      (3) The authors have shared publicly data and codes used for processing and fitting, which will allow one to reproduce or extend the scope of this work to disease populations, and which goes in line with the current effort of the MR(S) community for data sharing.

      Specific weaknesses:

      Ligneul and coauthors have convincingly addressed and included my comments from the first and second round in their revised manuscript.

      I believe the following conceptual concerns, which are inherent to the nature of the study and do not require further adjustments of the manuscript, remain:

      (1) Metabolite compartmentation in one cell type or the other has often been challenged and is currently impossible to validate in vivo. Here, Ligneul and coauthors did not use this assumption a priori and supported their claims also with non-MR literature (eg. for Taurine), but the interpretation of results in that direction should be made with care.

      (2) Longitudinal MR studies of the developing brain make it difficult to extract parameters with an "absolute" meaning. Indirect assumptions used to derive such parameters may change with age and become confounding factors (brain structure, cell distribution, concentrations normalizing metabolites (here macromolecules), relaxation times...). While findings of the manuscript are convincing and supported with literature, the true underlying nature of such changes might be difficult to access.

      (3) Diffusion MRI in addition to diffusion MRS would have been complementary and beneficial to validate some of the signal contributions, but was unfeasible in the time constraints of experiments on young animals.

    1. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      To summarize: The authors' overfilling hypothesis depends crucially on the premise that the very-quickly reverting paired-pulse depression seen after unusually short rest intervals of << 50 ms is caused by depletion of release sites whereas Dobrunz and Stevens (1997) concluded that the cause was some other mechanism that does not involve depletion. The authors now include experiments where switching extracellular Ca2+ from 1.2 to 2.5 mM increases synaptic strength on average, but not by as much as at other synapse types. They contend that the result supports the depletion hypothesis. I didn't agree because the model used to generate the hypothesis had no room for any increase at all, and because a more granular analysis revealed a mixed population with a subset where: (a) synaptic strength increased by as much as at standard synapses; and yet (b) the quickly reverting depression for the subset was the same as the overall population.

      The authors raise the possibility of additional experiments, and I do think this could clarify things if they pre-treat with EGTA as I recommended initially. They've already shown they can do this routinely, and it would allow them to elegantly distinguish between pv and pocc explanations for both the increases in synaptic strength and the decreases in the paired pulse ratio upon switching Ca2+ to 2.5 mM. Plus/minus EGTA pre-treatment trials could be interleaved and done blind with minimal additional effort.

      Showing reversibility would be a great addition too, because, in our experience, this does not always happen in whole-cell recordings in ex-vivo tissue even when electrical properties do not change. If the goal is to show that L2/3 synapses are less sensitive to changes in Ca2+ compared to other synapse types - which is interesting but a bit off point - then I would additionally include a positive control, done by the same person with the same equipment, at one of those other synapse types using the same kind of presynaptic stimulation (i.e. ChRs).

      Specific points (quotations are from the Authors' rebuttal)

      (1) Regarding the Author response image 1, I was instead suggesting a plot of PPR in 1.2 mM Ca2+ versus the relative increase in synaptic strength in 2.5 versus in 1.2 mM. This continues to seem relevant.

      (2) "Could you explain in detail why two-fold increase implies pv < 0.2?"

      a. start with power((2.5/(1 + (2.5/K1) + 1/2.97)),4) = 2*power((1.3/(1 + (1.3/K1) + 1/2.97)),4);

      b. solve for K1 (this turns out to be 0.48);

      c. then implement the premise that pv -> 1.0 when Ca2+ is high by calculating Max = power((C/(1 + (C/K1) + 1/2.97)),4) where C is [Ca] -> infinity.

      d. pv when [Ca] = 1.3. mM must then be power((1.3/(1 + (1.3/K1) + 1/2.97)),4)/Max, which is <0.2.

      Note that modern updates of Dodge and Rahamimoff typically include a parameter that prevents pv from approaching 1.0; this is the gamma parameter in the versions from Neher group.

      (3) "If so, we can not understand why depletion-dependent PPD should lead to PPF."

      When PPD is caused by depletion and pv < 0.2, the number of occupied release sites should not be decreased by more than one-fifth at the second stimulus so, without facilitation, PPR should be > 0.8. The EGTA results then indicate there should be strong facilitation, driving PPR to something like 1.2 with conservative assumptions. And yet, a value of < 0.4 is measured, which is a large miss.

      (4) Despite the authors' suggestion to the contrary, I continue to think there is a substantial chance that Ca2+-channel inactivation is the mechanism underlying the very quickly reverting paired-pulse depression. However, this is only one example of a non-depletion mechanism among many, with the main point being that any non-depletion mechanism would undercut the reasoning for overfilling. And, this is what Dobrunz and Stevens claimed to show; that the mechanism - whatever it is - does not involve depletion. The most effective way to address this would be affirmative experiments showing that the quickly reverting depression is caused by depletion after all. Attempting to prove that Ca2+-channel inactivation does not occur does not seem like a worthwhile strategy because it would not address the many other possibilities.

      (5) True that Kusick et al. observed morphological re-docking, but then vesicles would have to re-prime and Mahfooz et al. (2016) showed that re-priming would have to be slower than 110 ms (at least during heavy use at calyx of Held).

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors investigate the effects of aging on auditory system performance in understanding temporal fine structure (TFS), using both behavioral assessments and physiological recordings from the auditory periphery, specifically at the level of the auditory nerve. This dual approach aims to enhance understanding of the mechanisms underlying observed behavioral outcomes. The results indicate that aged animals exhibit deficits in behavioral tasks for distinguishing between harmonic and inharmonic sounds, which is a standard test for TFS coding. However, neural responses at the auditory nerve level do not show significant differences when compared to those in young, normal-hearing animals. The authors suggest that these behavioral deficits in aged animals are likely attributable to dysfunctions in the central auditory system, potentially as a consequence of aging.To further investigate this hypothesis, the study includes an animal group with selective synaptic loss between inner hair cells and auditory nerve fibers, a condition known as cochlear synaptopathy (CS). CS is a pathology associated with aging and is thought to be an early indicator of hearing impairment. Interestingly, animals with selective CS showed physiological and behavioral TFS coding similar to that of the young normal-hearing group, contrasting with the aged group's deficits. Despite histological evidence of significant synaptic loss in the CS group, the study concludes that CS does not appear to affect TFS coding, either behaviorally or physiologically.

      Strengths:

      This study addresses a critical health concern, enhancing our understanding of mechanisms underlying age-related difficulties in speech intelligibility, even when audiometric thresholds are within normal limits. A major strength of this work is the comprehensive approach, integrating behavioral assessments, auditory nerve (AN) physiology, and histology within the same animal subjects. This approach enhances understanding of the mechanisms underlying the behavioral outcomes and provides confidence in the actual occurrence of synapse loss and its effects.The study carefully manages controlled conditions by including five distinct groups: young normal-hearing animals, aged animals, animals with CS induced through low and high doses, and a sham surgery group. This careful setup strengthens the study's reliability and allows for meaningful comparisons across conditions. Overall, the manuscript is well-structured, with clear and accessible writing that facilitates comprehension of complex concepts.

      Weakness:

      The stimulus and task employed in this study are very helpful for behavioral research, and using the same stimulus setup for physiology is advantageous for mechanistic comparisons. However, I have some concerns about the limitations in auditory nerve (AN) physiology. Due to practical constraints, it is not feasible to record from a large enough population of fibers that covers a full range of best frequencies (BFs) and spontaneous rates (SRs) within each animal. This raises questions about how representative the physiological data are for understanding the mechanism in behavioral data. I am curious about the authors' interpretation of how this stimulus setup might influence results compared to methods used by Kale and Heinz (2010), who adjusted harmonic frequencies based on the characteristic frequency (CF) of recorded units. While, the harmonic frequencies in this study are fixed across all CFs, meaning that many AN fibers may not be tuned closely to the stimulus frequencies. If units are not responsive to the stimulus further clarification on detecting mistuning and phase locking to TFS effects within this setup would be valuable. Given the limited number of units per condition-sometimes as few as three for certain conditions-I wonder if CF-dependent variability might impact the results of the AN data in this study and discussing this factor can help with better understanding the results. While the use of the same stimuli for both behavioral and physiological recordings is understandable, a discussion on how this choice affects interpretation would be beneficial. In addition a 60 dB stimulus could saturate high spontaneous rate (HSR) AN fibers, influencing neural coding and phase-locking to TFS. Potentially separating SR groups, could help address these issues and improve interpretive clarity.

      A deeper discussion on the role of fiber spontaneous rate could also enhance the study. How might considering SR groups affect AN results related to TFS coding? While some statistical measures are included in the supplement, a more detailed discussion in the main text could help in interpretation.

      Although Figure S2 indicates no change in median SR, the high-dose treatment group lacks LSR fibers, suggesting a different distribution based on SR for different animal groups, as seen in similar studies on other species. A histogram of these results would be informative, as LSR fiber loss with CS-whether induced by ouabain in gerbils or noise in other animals-is well documented (e.g., Furman et al., 2013).

      Although ouabain effects on gerbils have been explored in previous studies, since these data is already seems to be recorded for the animal in this study, a brief description of changes in auditory brainstem response (ABR) thresholds, wave 1 amplitudes, and tuning curves for animals with cochlear synaptopathy (CS) in this study would be beneficial. This would confirm that ouabain selectively affects synapses without impacting outer hair cells (OHCs). For aged animals, since ABR measurements were taken, comparing hearing differences between normal and aged groups could provide insights into the pathologies besides CS in aged animals. Additionally, examining subject variability in treatment effects on hearing and how this correlates with behavior and physiology would yield valuable insights. If limited space maybe a brief clarification or inclusion in supplementary could be good enough.

      Another suggestion is to discuss the potential role of MOC efferent system and effect of anesthesia in reducing efferent effects in AN recordings. This is particularly relevant for aged animals, as CS might affect LSR fibers, potentially disrupting the medial olivocochlear (MOC) efferent pathway. Anesthesia could lessen MOC activity in both young and aged animals, potentially masking efferent effects that might be present in behavioral tasks. Young gerbils with functional efferent systems might perform better behaviorally, while aged gerbils with impaired MOC function due to CS might lack this advantage. A brief discussion on this aspect could potentially enhance mechanistic insights.

      Lastly, although synapse counts did not differ between the low-dose treatment and NH I sham groups, separating these groups rather than combining them with the sham might reveal differences in behavior or AN results, particularly regarding the significance of differences between aged/treatment groups and the young normal-hearing group.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Using a gerbil model, the authors tested the hypothesis that loss of synapses between sensory hair cells and auditory nerve fibers (which may occur due to noise exposure or aging) affects behavioral discrimination of the rapid temporal fluctuations of sounds. In contrast to previous suggestions in the literature, their results do not support this hypothesis; young animals treated with a compound that reduces the number of synapses did not show impaired discrimination compared to controls. Additionally, their results from older animals showing impaired discrimination suggest that age-related changes aside from synaptopathy are responsible for the age-related decline in discrimination.

      Strengths:

      (1) The rationale and hypothesis are well-motivated and clearly presented.

      (2) The study was well conducted with strong methodology for the most part, and good experimental control. The combination of physiological and behavioral techniques is powerful and informative. Reducing synapse counts fairly directly using ouabain is a cleaner design than using noise exposure or age (as in other studies), since these latter modifiers have additional effects on auditory function.

      (3) The study may have a considerable impact on the field. The findings could have important implications for our understanding of cochlear synaptopathy, one of the most highly researched and potentially impactful developments in hearing science in the past fifteen years.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) I have concerns that the gerbils may not have been performing the behavioral task using temporal fine structure information.

      Human studies using the same task employed a filter center frequency that was (at least) 11 times the fundamental frequency (Marmel et al., 2015; Moore and Sek, 2009). Moore and Sek wrote: "the default (recommended) value of the centre frequency is 11F0." Here, the center frequency was only 4 or 8 times the fundamental frequency (4F0 or 8F0). Hence, relative to harmonic frequency, the harmonic spacing was considerably greater in the present study. However, gerbil auditory filters are thought to be broader than those in human. In the revised version of the manuscript, the authors provide modelling results suggesting that the excitation patterns were discriminable for the 4F0 conditions, but may not have been for the 8F0 conditions. These results provide some reassurance that the 8F0 discriminations were dependent on temporal cues, but the description of the model lacks detail. Also, the authors state that "thus, for these two conditions with harmonic number N of 8 the gerbils cannot rely on differences in the excitation patterns but must solve the task by comparing the temporal fine structure." This is too strong. Pulsed tone intensity difference limens (the reference used for establishing whether or not the excitation pattern cues were usable) may not be directly comparable to profile-analysis-like conditions, and it has been argued that frequency discrimination may be more sensitive to excitation pattern cues than predicted from a simple comparison to intensity difference limens (Micheyl et al. 2013, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003336).

      I'm also somewhat concerned that the masking noise used in the present study was too low in level to mask cochlear distortion products. Based on their excitation pattern modelling, the authors state (without citation) that "since the level of excitation produced by the pink noise is less than 30 dB below that produced by the complex tones, distortion products will be masked." The basis for this claim is not clear. In human, distortion products may be only ~20 dB below the levels of the primaries (referenced to an external sound masker / canceller, which is appropriate, assuming that the modelling reported in the present paper did not include middle-ear effects; see Norman-Haignere and McDermott, 2016, doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.050). Oxenham et al. (2009, doi: 10.1121/1.3089220) provide further cautionary evidence on the potential use of distortion product cues when the background noise level is too low (in their case the relative level of the noise in the compromised condition was only a little below that used in the present study). The masking level used in the present study may have been sufficient, but it would be useful to have some further reassurance on this point.

      (2) The synapse reductions in the high ouabain and old groups were relatively small (mean of 19 synapses per hair cell compared to 23 in the young untreated group). In contrast, in some mouse models of the effects of noise exposure or age, a 50% reduction in synapses is observed, and in the human temporal bone study of Wu et al. (2021, https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3238-20.2021) the age-related reduction in auditory nerve fibres was ~50% or greater for the highest age group across cochlear location. It could be simply that the synapse loss in the present study was too small to produce significant behavioral effects. Hence, although the authors provide evidence that in the gerbil model the age-related behavioral effects are not due to synaptopathy, this may not translate to other species (including human).

      (3) The study was not pre-registered, and there was no a priori power calculation, so there is less confidence in replicability than could have been the case. Only three old animals were used in the behavioral study, which raises concerns about the reliability of comparisons involving this group. Statistical analyses on very small samples can be unreliable due to problems of power, generalisability, and susceptibility to outliers.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      This study is a part of the ongoing series of rigorous work from this group exploring neural coding deficits in the auditory nerve, and dissociating the effects of cochlear synaptopathy from other age-related deficits. They have previously shown no evidence of phase-locking deficits in the remaining auditory nerve fibers in quiet-aged gerbils. Here, they study the effects of aging on the perception and neural coding of temporal fine structure cues in the same Mongolian gerbil model.

      They measure TFS coding in the auditory nerve using the TFS1 task which uses a combination of harmonic and tone-shifted inharmonic tones which differ primarily in their TFS cues (and not the envelope). They then follow this up with a behavioral paradigm using the TFS1 task in these gerbils. They test young normal hearing gerbils, aged gerbils, and young gerbils with cochlear synaptopathy induced using the neurotoxin ouabain to mimic synapse losses seen with age.

      In the behavioral paradigm, they find that aging is associated with decreased performance compared to the young gerbils, whereas young gerbils with similar levels of synapse loss do not show these deficits. When looking at the auditory nerve responses, they find no differences in neural coding of TFS cues across any of the groups. However, aged gerbils show an increase in the representation of periodicity envelope cues (around f0) compared to young gerbils or those with induced synapse loss. The authors hence conclude that synapse loss by itself doesn't seem to be important for distinguishing TFS cues, and rather the behavioral deficits with age are likely having to do with the misrepresented envelope cues instead.

      The manuscript is well written, and the data presented are robust. Some of the points below will need to be considered while interpreting the results of the study, in its current form. These considerations are addressable if deemed necessary, with some additional analysis in future versions of the manuscript.

      Spontaneous rates - Figure S2 shows no differences in median spontaneous rates across groups. But taking the median glosses over some of the nuances there. Ouabain (in the Bourien study) famously affects low spont rates first, and at a higher degree than median or high spont rates. It seems to be the case (qualitatively) in figure S2 as well, with almost no units in the low spont region in the ouabain group, compared to the other groups. Looking at distributions within each spont rate category and comparing differences across the groups might reveal some of the underlying causes for these changes. Given that overall, the study reports that low-SR fibers had a higher ENV/TFS log-z-ratio, the distribution of these fibers across groups may reveal specific effects of TFS coding by group.

      [Update: The revised manuscript has addressed these issues]

      Threshold shifts - It is unclear from the current version if the older gerbils have changes in hearing thresholds, and whether those changes may be affecting behavioral thresholds. The behavioral stimuli appear to have been presented at a fixed sound level for both young and aged gerbils, similar to the single unit recordings. Hence, age-related differences in behavior may have been due to changes in relative sensation level. Approaches such as using hearing thresholds as covariates in the analysis will help explore if older gerbils still show behavioral deficits.

      [Update: The issue of threshold shifts with aging gerbils is still unresolved in my opinion. From the revised manuscript, it appears that aged gerbils have a 36dB shift in thresholds. While the revised manuscript provides convincing evidence that these threshold shifts do not affect the auditory nerve tuning properties, the behavioral paradigm was still presented at the same sound level for young and aged animals. But a potential 36 dB change in sensation level may affect behavioral results. The authors may consider adding thresholds as covariates in analyses or present any evidence that behavioral thresholds are plateaued along that 30dB range].

      Task learning in aged gerbils - It is unclear if the aged gerbils really learn the task well in two of the three TFS1 test conditions. The d' of 1 which is usually used as the criterion for learning was not reached in even the easiest condition for aged gerbils in all but one condition for the aged gerbils (Fig. 5H) and in that condition, there doesn't seem to be any age-related deficits in behavioral performance (Fig. 6B). Hence dissociating the inability to learn the task from the inability to perceive TFS 1 cues in those animals becomes challenging.

      [Update: The revised manuscript sufficiently addresses these issues, with the caveat of hearing threshold changes affecting behavioral thresholds mentioned above].

      Increased representation of periodicity envelope in the AN - the mechanisms for increased representation of periodicity envelope cues is unclear. The authors point to some potential central mechanisms but given that these are recordings from the auditory nerve what central mechanisms these may be is unclear. If the authors are suggesting some form of efferent modulation only at the f0 frequency, no evidence for this is presented. It appears more likely that the enhancement may be due to outer hair cell dysfunction (widened tuning, distorted tonotopy). Given this increased envelope coding, the potential change in sensation level for the behavior (from the comment above), and no change in neural coding of TFS cues across any of the groups, a simpler interpretation may be -TFS coding is not affected in remaining auditory nerve fibers after age-related or ouabain induced synapse loss, but behavioral performance is affected by altered outer hair cell dysfunction with age.

      [Update: The revised manuscript has addressed these issues]

      Emerging evidence seems to suggest that cochlear synaptopathy and/or TFS encoding abilities might be reflected in listening effort rather than behavioral performance. Measuring some proxy of listening effort in these gerbils (like reaction time) to see if that has changed with synapse loss, especially in the young animals with induced synaptopathy, would make an interesting addition to explore perceptual deficits of TFS coding with synapse loss.

      [Update: The revised manuscript has addressed these issues]

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors present a nanobody-based pulse-labeling system to track yeast NPCs. Transient expression of a nanobody targeting Nup84 (fused to NeonGreen or an affinity tag) permits selective visualization and biochemical capture of NPCs. Short induction effectively labels NPCs, and the resulting purifications match those from conventional Nup84 tagging. Crucially, when induction is repressed, dilution of the labeled pool through successive cell cycles allows the visualization of "old" NPCs (and potentially individual NPCs), providing a powerful view of NPC lifespan and turnover without permanently modifying a core scaffold protein.

      Strengths:

      (1) A brief expression pulse labels NPCs, and subsequent repression allows dilution-based tracking of older (and possibly single) NPCs over multiple cell cycles.

      (2) The affinity-purified complexes closely match known Nup84-associated proteins, indicating specificity and supporting utility for proteomics.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Reliance on GAL induction introduces metabolic shifts (raffinose → galactose → glucose) that could subtly alter cell physiology or the kinetics of NPC assembly. Alternative induction systems (e.g., β-estradiol-responsive GAL4-ER-VP16) could be discussed as a way to avoid carbon-source changes.

      (2) While proteomics is solid, a comprehensive supplementary table listing all identified proteins (with enrichment and statistics) would enhance transparency.

      (3) Importantly, the authors note that the method is particularly useful "in conditions where direct tagging of Nup84 interferes with its function, while sub-stoichiometric nanobody binding does not." After this sentence, it would be valuable to add concrete examples, such as experiments examining NPC integrity in aging or stress conditions where epitope tags can exacerbate phenotypes. These examples will help readers identify situations in which this approach offers clear advantages.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This preprint describes a practical and useful approach for labeling and tracking NPCs in situ. While useful applications including timelapse imaging, affinity purification, or proximity labeling are envisioned, addressing some outstanding technical questions would give a clearer picture of the sensitivity and temporal resolution of this approach.

      Strengths:

      Clever use of a fluorescently conjugated nanobody that binds directly to the core scaffold nucleoporin Nup84 with nanomolar affinity.

      Weaknesses:

      The decrease in nanobody labeling over 8 hours of chase period is interpreted to indicate that NPCs turn over during this time. However, it is also possible that the nanobody:Nup84 association is disrupted during mitosis by phosphorylation, other PTMs, or structural remodeling.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Submitted to the Tools and Resources series, this study reports on the use of a single-domain antibody targeting the nucleoporin Nup84 to probe and track NPCs in budding yeast. The authors demonstrate their ability to rapidly label or pull down NPCs by inducing the expression of a tagged version of the nanobody (Figure 1).

      Strengths:

      This tool's main strength is its versatility as an inexpensive, easy-to-set-up alternative to metabolic labelling or optical switching. This same rationale could, in principle, be applied to the study of other multiprotein complexes using similar strategies, provided that single-chain antibodies are available.

      Weaknesses:

      This approach has no inherent weaknesses, but it would be useful for the authors to verify that their pulse labelling strategy can also be used to detect assembly intermediates, structural variants, or damaged NPCs.

      Overall, the data clearly show that Nup84 nanobodies are a valuable tool for imaging NPC dynamics and investigating their interactomes through affinity purification.

    1. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this contribution, the authors investigate the degree of alternative splicing across the evolutionary tree, and identify a trend of increasing alternative splicing as you move from the base of the tree (here, only prokaryotes are considered) towards the tips of the tree. In particular, the authors investigate how the degree of alternative splicing (roughly speaking, the number of different proteins made from a single ORF (open reading frame) via alternative splicing) relates to three genomic variables: the genome size, the gene content (meaning the fraction of the genome composed of ORFs), and finally, the coding percentage of ORFs, meaning the ratio between exons and total DNA in the ORF.

      The revised manuscript addresses the problems identified in the first round of reviews and now serves as a guide to understand how alternative splicing has evolved within different phyla, as opposed to making unsubstantiated claims about overall trends.

    2. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In "Alternative Splicing Across the Tree of Life: A Comparative Study," the authors use rich annotation features from nearly 1,500 high-quality NCBI genome assemblies to develop a novel genome-scale metric, the Alternative Splicing Ratio, that quantifies the extent to which coding sequences generate multiple mRNA transcripts via alternative splicing (AS). This standardized metric enables cross-species comparisons and reveals clear phylogenetic patterns: minimal AS in prokaryotes and unicellular eukaryotes, moderate AS in plants, and high AS in mammals and birds. The study finds a strong negative correlation between AS and coding content, with genomes containing approximately 50% intergenic DNA exhibiting the highest AS activity. By integrating diverse lines of prior evidence, the study offers a cohesive evolutionary framework for understanding how alternative splicing varies and evolves across the tree of life.

      Strengths:

      By studying alternative splicing patterns across the tree of life, the authors systematically address an important yet historically understudied driver of functional diversity, complexity, and evolutionary innovation. This manuscript makes a valuable contribution by leveraging standardized, publicly available genome annotations to perform a global survey of transcriptional diversity, revealing lineage-specific patterns and evolutionary correlates. The authors have done an admirable job in this revised version, thoroughly addressing prior reviewer comments. The updated manuscript includes more rigorous statistical analyses, careful consideration of potential methodological biases, expanded discussion of regulatory mechanisms, and acknowledgment of non-adaptive alternatives. Overall, the work presents an intriguing view of how alternative splicing may serve as a flexible evolutionary strategy, particularly in lineages with limited capacity for coding expansion (e.g., via gene duplication). Notably, the identification of genome size and genic coding fraction thresholds (~20 Mb and ~50%, respectively) as tipping points for increased splicing activity adds conceptual depth and potential generalizability.

      Weaknesses:

      While the manuscript offers a broad comparative view of alternative splicing, its central message becomes diffuse in the revised version. The focus of the study is unclear, and the manuscript comes across as largely descriptive without a well-articulated hypothesis or explanatory evolutionary model. Although the discussion gestures toward adaptive and non-adaptive mechanisms, these interpretations are not developed early or prominently enough to anchor the reader. The negative correlation between alternative splicing and coding content is compelling, but the biological significance of this pattern remains ambiguous: it is unclear whether it reflects functional constraint, genome organization, or annotation bias. This uncertainty weakens the manuscript's broader evolutionary inferences.

      Sections of the Introduction, particularly lines 72-90, lack cohesion and logical flow, shifting abruptly between topics without a clear structure. A more effective approach may involve separating discussions of coding and non-coding sequence evolution to clarify their distinct contributions to splicing complexity. Furthermore, some interpretive claims lack nuance. For example, the assertion that splicing in plants "evolved independently" seems overstated given the available evidence, and the citation regarding slower evolution of highly expressed genes overlooks counterexamples from the immunity and reproductive gene literature.

      Presentation of the results is occasionally vague. For instance, stating "we conducted comparisons of mean values" (line 146) without specifying the metric undercuts interpretability. The authors should clarify whether these comparisons refer to the Alternative Splicing Ratio or another measure. Additionally, the lack of correlation between splicing and coding region fraction in prokaryotes may reflect a statistical power issue, particularly given their limited number of annotated isoforms, rather than a biological absence of pattern.

      Finally, the assessment of annotation-related bias warrants greater methodological clarity. The authors note that annotations with stronger experimental support yield higher splicing estimates, yet the normalization strategy for variation in transcriptomic sampling (e.g., tissue breadth vs sequencing depth) is insufficiently described. As these factors can significantly influence splicing estimates, a more rigorous treatment is essential. While the authors rightly acknowledge that splicing represents only one layer of regulatory complexity, the manuscript would benefit from a more integrated consideration of additional dimensions, such as 3D genome architecture, e.g., the potential role of topologically associating domains in constraining splicing variation.

    3. Reviewer #4 (Public review):

      The manuscript reports on a large-scale study correlating genomic architecture with splicing complexity over almost 1,500 species. We still know relatively little about alternative splicing functional consequences and evolution, and thus, the study is relevant and timely. The methodology relies on annotations from NCBI for high-quality genomes and a main metric proposed by the authors and named Alternative Splicing Ratio (ASR). It quantifies the level of redundancy of each coding nucleotide in the annotated isoforms.

      According to the authors' response to the first reviewers' comments, the present version of the manuscript seems to be a profoundly revised version compared to the original submission. I did not have access to the reviewers' comments.

      Although the study addresses an important question and the authors have visibly made an important effort to make their claims more statistically robust, I have a number of major concerns regarding the methodology and its presentation.

      (1) A large part of the manuscript is speculative and vague. For instance, the Discussion is very long (almost longer than the Results section) and the items discussed are sometimes not in direct connection with the present work. I would suggest merging the last 2 paragraphs, for instance, since the before last paragraph is essentially a review of the literature without direct connection to the present work.

      (2) The Methods section lacks clarity and precision. A large part is devoted to explaining the biases in the data without any reference or quantification. The definition of ASR is very confusing. It is first defined in equation 2, with a different name, and then again in the next subsection from a different perspective on lines 512-518. Why build matrices of co-occurrences if these are, in practice, never used? It seems the authors exploit only the trace. A major revision, if I understood correctly, was the correction/normalisation of the ASR metric. This normalisation is not explained. The authors argue that they will write another paper about it, I do not think this is acceptable for the publication of the present manuscript. Furthermore, there is no information about the technical details of the implementation: which packages did the authors use?

      (3) Could the authors motivate why they do not directly focus on the MC permutation test? They motivate the use of permutations because the data contains extreme outliers and are non normal in most cases. Hence, it seems the Welch's ANOVA is not adapted. "To further validate our findings, we also conducted<br /> 148 a Monte Carlo permutation test, which supported the conclusions (see Methods)." Where is the comparison shown? I did not see any report of the results for the non-permuted version of the Welch's ANOVA.

      (4) What are the assumptions for the Phylogenetic Generalized Least Squares? Which evolution model was chosen and why? What is the impact of changing the model? Could the authors define more precisely (e.g. with equations) what is lambda? Is it estimated or fixed?

      (5) I think the authors could improve their account of recent literature on the topic. For instance, the paper https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.93629.3, published in the same journal last year, should be discussed. It perfectly fits in the scope of the subsection "Evidence for the adaptive role of alternative splicing". Methods and findings reported in https://doi.org/10.1186/s13059-021-02441-9 and https://www.genome.org/cgi/doi/10.1101/gr.274696.120 directly concern the assessment of AS evolutionary conservation across long evolutionary times and/or across many species. These aspects are mentioned in the introduction on p.3. but without pointing to such works. Can we really qualify a work published in 2011 as "recent" (line 348-350)?

      The generated data and codes are available on Zenodo, which is a good point for reproducibility and knowledge sharing with the community.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors investigate how methicillin-resistant (MRSA) and sensitive (MSSA) Staphylococcus aureus adapt to a new host (C. elegans) in the presence or absence of a low dose of the antibiotic oxacillin. Using an "Evolve and Resequence" design with 48 independently evolving populations, they track changes in virulence, antibiotic resistance, and other fitness-related traits over 12 passages. Their key finding is that selection from both the host and the antibiotic together, rather than either pressure alone, results in the evolution of the most virulent pathogens. Genomically, they find that this adaptation repeatedly involves mutations in a small number of key regulatory genes, most notably codY, agr, and saeRS.

      Strengths:

      The main advantage of the research lies in its strong and thoroughly replicated experimental framework, enabling significant conclusions to be drawn based on the concept of parallel evolution. The study successfully integrates various phenotypic assays (virulence, growth, hemolysis, biofilm formation) with whole-genome sequencing, offering an extensive perspective on the adaptive landscape. The identification of certain regulatory genes as common targets of selection across distinct lineages is an important result that indicates a level of predictability in how pathogens adapt.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The main limitation of the paper is that its findings on the function of specific genes are based on correlation, not cause-and-effect evidence. While the parallel evolution evidence is strong, the authors have not yet performed the definitive tests (i.e., reconstruction of ancestral genes) to ensure that the mutations identified in isolation are enough to account for the virulence or resistance changes observed. This makes the conclusions more like firm hypotheses, not confirmed facts.

      (2) In some instances, the claims in the text are not fully supported by the visual data from the figures or are reported with vagueness. For example, the display of phenotypic clusters in the PCA (Figure 6A) and the sweeping generalization about the effect of antibiotics on the mutation rates (Figure S5) can be more precise and nuanced. Such small deviations dilute the overall argument somewhat and must be corrected.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript describes the results of an evolution experiment where Staphylococcus aureus was experimentally evolved via sequential exposure to an antibiotic followed by passaging through C. elegans hosts. Because infecting C. elegans via ingestion results in lysis of gut cells and an immune response upon infection, the S. aureus were exposed separately across generations to antibiotic stress and host immune stress. Interestingly, the dual selection pressure of antibiotic exposure and adaptation to a nematode host resulted in increased virulence of S. aureus towards C. elegans.

      Strengths:

      The data presented provide strong evidence that in S. aureus, traits involved in adaptation to a novel host and those involved in antibiotic resistance evolution are not traded off. On the contrary, they seem to be correlated, with strains adapted to antibiotics having higher virulence towards the novel host. As increased virulence is also associated with higher rates of haemolysis, these virulence increases are likely to reflect virulence levels in vertebrate hosts.

      Weaknesses:

      Right now, the results are presented in the context of human infections being treated with antibiotics, which, in my opinion, is inappropriate. This is because<br /> (1) exposure to the host and antibiotics was sequential, not simultaneous, and thus does not reflect the treatment of infection, and<br /> (2) because the site of infection is different in C. elegans and human hosts.

      Nevertheless, the results are of interest; I just think the interpretation and framing should be adjusted.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Su et al. sought to understand how the opportunistic pathogen Staphylococcus aureus responds to multiple selection pressures during infection. Specifically, the authors were interested in how the host environment and antibiotic exposure impact the evolution of both virulence and antibiotic resistance in S. aureus. To accomplish this, the authors performed an evolution experiment where S. aureus was fed to Caenorhabditis elegans as a model system to study the host environment and then either subjected to the antibiotic oxacillin or not. Additionally, the authors investigated the difference in evolution between an antibiotic-resistant strain, MRSA, and an isogenic susceptible strain, MSSA. They found that MRSA strains evolved in both antibiotic and host conditions became more virulent, and that strains evolved outside these conditions lost virulence. Looking at the strains evolved in just antibiotic conditions, the authors found that S. aureus maintained its ability to lyse blood cells. Mutations in codY, gdpP, and pbpA were found to be associated with increased virulence. Additionally, these mutations identified in these experiments were found in S. aureus strains isolated from human infections.

      Strengths:

      The data are well-presented, thorough, and are an important addition to the understanding of how certain pathogens might adapt to different selective pressures in complex environments.

      Weaknesses:

      There are a few clarifications that could be made to better understand and contextualize the results. Primarily, when comparing the number of mutations and selection across conditions in an evolution experiment, information about population sizes is important to be able to calculate the mutation supply and number of generations throughout the experiment. These calculations can be difficult in vivo, but since several steps in the methodology require plating and regrowth, those population sizes could be determined. There was also no mention of how the authors controlled the inoculation density of bacteria introduced to each host. This would need to be known to calculate the generation time within the host. These caveats should be addressed in the manuscript.

      Another concern is the number of generations the populations of S. aureus spent either with relaxed selection in rich media or under antibiotic pressure in between the host exposure periods. It is probable then that the majority of mutations were selected for in these intervening periods between host infection. Again, a more detailed understanding of population sizes would contribute to the understanding of which phase of the experiment contributed to the mutation profile observed.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript presents an extensive body of work and an outstanding contribution to our understanding of the IFN type I and III system in chickens. The research started with the innovative approach of generating KO chickens that lack the receptor for IFNα/β (IFNAR1) or IFN-λ (IFNLR1). The successful deletion and functional loss of these receptors was clearly and comprehensively demonstrated in comparison to the WT. Moreover, the homozygous KO lines (IFNAR1-/- or IFNLR1-/- ) were found to have similar body weights, and normal egg production and fertility compared to their WT counterparts. These lines are a major contribution to the toolbox for the study of avian/chicken immunology.

      The significance of this contribution is further demonstrated by the use of these lines by the authors to gain insight into the roles of IFN type I and IFN-type III in chickens, by conducting in ovo and in vivo studies examining basic aspects of immune system development and function, as well as the responses to viral challenges conducted in ovo and in vivo.

      Based on solid, state-of the-art methods and convincing evidence from studies comparing various immune system related functions in the IFNAR1-/- or IFNLR1-/- lines to the WT, revealed that the deletion of IFNAR1 and/or IFNLR1 resulted in:<br /> (1) impaired IFN signaling and induction of anti-viral state;<br /> (2) modulation of immune cell profiles in the peripheral blood circulation and spleen;<br /> (3) modulation of the cecum microbiome;<br /> (4) reduced concentrations of IgM and IgY in the blood plasma before and following immunization with model antigen KLH, whereby also line differences in the time-course of the antibody production were observed;<br /> (5) decrease in MHCII+ macrophages and B cells in the spleen of IFNAR1 KO chickens, although the MHCII-expression per cell was not affected in this line; and<br /> (6) reduction in the response of αβ1 TCR+ T cells of IFNAR1 KO chickens as suggested by clonal repertoire analyses.

      These studies were then followed by examination of the role of type I and type III IFN in virus infection, using different avian influenza A virus strains as well as an avian gamma corona virus (IBV) in in ovo challenge experiments. These studies revealed: viral titers that reflect virus-species and strain-specific IFN responses; no differences in the secretion of IFN-α/β in both KO compared to the WT lines; a predominant role of type I IFN in inducing the interferon-stimulated gene (ISG) Mx; and that an excessive and unbalanced type I IFN response can harm host fitness (survival rate, length of survival) and contribute to immunopathology.

      Based on guidance from the in ovo studies, comprehensive in vivo studies were conducted on host-pathogen interactions in hens from the three lines (WT, IFNAR1 KO, or IFNLR1 KO). These studies revealed the early appearance of symptoms and poor survival of hens from the IFNR1 KO line challenged with H3N1 avian influenza A virus; efficient H#N1 virus replication in IFNAR1 KO hens, increased plasma concentrations of IFNα/β and mRNA expression of IFN-λ in spleens of the IFNAR1 KO hens; a pro-inflammatory role of IFN-λ in the oviduct of hens infected with H3N1 virus; increased proinflammatory cytokine expression in spleens of IFNAR1 KO hens, and Impairment of negative feedback mechanisms regulating IFN-α/β secretion in IFNAR1-KO hens and a significant decrease in this group's antiviral state; additionally it was demonstrated that IFN-α/β can compensate IFN-λ to induce an adequate antiviral state in the spleen during H3N1 infection, but IFN-λ cannot compensate for IFN-α/β signaling in the spleen.

      Strengths:

      (1) Both the methods and results from the comprehensive, well-designed, and well-executed experiments are considered excellent. The results are well and correctly described in the result narrative and well presented in both the manuscript and supplement Tables and Figures. Excellent discussion/interpretation of results.

      (2) The successful generation of the type I and type III IFN KO lines offers unprecedented insight and opens multiple new venues for exploring the IFN system in chickens. The new knowledge reported here is direct evidence of the high impact of this model system on effectively addressing a critical knowledge gap in avian immunology.

      (3) The thoughtful selection of highly relevant viruses to poultry and human health for the in ovo and in vivo challenge studies to examine and assess host-pathogen interactions in the IFNR KO and WT lines.

      (4) Making use of the unique opportunities in the chicken model to examine and evaluate the host's IFN system responses to various viral challenges in ovo, before conducting challenge studies in hens.

      (5) The new knowledge gained from the IFNAR1 and IFNLR1 KO lines will find much-needed application in developing more effective strategies to prevent health challenges like avian influenza and its devastating effects on poultry, humans, and other mammals.

      (6) The excellent cooperation and contributions of the co-authors and institutions.

      Weaknesses:

      No weaknesses were identified by this reviewer.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study attempts to dissect the contributions of type I and type III IFNs to the antiviral response in chickens. The first part of the study characterises the generation of IFNAR and IFNLR KO chicken strains and describes basic differences. Four different viruses are then tested in chicken embryos, while the subsequent analysis of the antiviral response in vivo is performed with one influenza H3N1 strain.

      Strengths:

      Having these two KO chicken strains as a tool is a great achievement. The initial analysis is solid. Clear effect of IFNAR deficiency in in vivo infection, less so for IFNLR deficiency.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The antibody induction by KLH immunisation: No data indicated whether or not this vaccination induces IFN responses in wt mice, so the effects observed may be due to steady-state differences or to differential effects of IFN induced during the vaccination phase. No pre-immune results are shown. The differences are relatively small and often found at only one plasma dilution - the whole of Figure 4 could be condensed into one or two panels by proper calculation of Ab titers - would these titres be significantly different? This, as all of the other in vivo experiments, has not been repeated, if I understand the methods section correctly.

      (2) The basic conundrum here and in later figures is never addressed by the authors: Situations where IFN type 1 and 3 signalling deficiency each have an independent effect (i.e., Figure 4d) suggest that they act by separate, unrelated mechanisms. However, all the literature about these IFN families suggests that they show almost identical signalling and gene induction downstream of their respective receptors. How can the same signalling, clearly active here downstream of the receptors for IFN type 1 or type 3, be non-redundant, i.e., why does the unaffected IFN family not stand in? This is a major difference from the mouse studies, which showed a rather subtle phenotype when only one of the two IFN systems was missing, but a massive reduction in virus control in double KO mice (the correct primary paper should be quoted here, not only the review by McNab). Reasons could be a direct effect of IFNab on B cells and an indirect effect of IFNL through non-B cells, timing issues, and many other scenarios can be envisaged. The authors do not address this question, which limits the depth of analysis.

      (3) In the one in vivo experiment performed with chickens, only one virus was tested; more influenza strains should be included, as well as non-influenza viruses.

      (4) The basic conundrum of point 2 applies equally to Figure 6a; both KOs have a phenotype. Again in 6d, both IFNs appear to be separately required for Mx induction. An explanation is needed.

      (5) Line 308, where are the viral titers you refer to in the text? The statement that the results demonstrate that excessive IFNab has a negative impact is overstretched, as no IFN measurements of the infected embryos are shown here.

      (6) The in vivo infection is the most interesting experiment, and the key outcome here is that IFN type 1 is crucial for anti-H3N1 protection in chickens, while type 3 is less impactful. However, this experiment suffers from the different time points when chickens were culled, so many parameters are impossible to compare (e.g., weight loss, histopathology, IFN measurements, and more). Many of these phenomena are highly dynamic in acute virus infections, so disparate time points do not allow a meaningful comparison between different genotypes. What are the stats in 7b? Is the median rather than the mean indicated by the line? Otherwise, the lines appear in surprising places. SD must be shown, and I find it difficult to believe that there is a significant difference in weight, for e.g., IFNAR KO, unless maybe with a paired t test. What is the statistical test?

      (7) Figures 7e,f: these comparisons are very difficult to interpret as the virus loads at these time points already differ significantly, so any difference could be secondary to virus load differences.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, the authors reveal that the availability of extracellular asparagine (Asn) represents a metabolic vulnerability for the activation and differentiation of naive CD4+ T cells. To deplete extracellular Asn, they employed two orthogonal approaches: activating naive CD4+ T cells in either PEGylated asparaginase (PEG-AsnASE)-treated medium or custom-formulated RPMI medium specifically lacking Asn. Importantly, they demonstrate that Asn depletion not only impaired metabolic reprogramming associated with CD4+ T cell activation but also reduced CD4+ helper T cell lineage-specific cytokine production, thereby ameliorating the severity of experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis.

      Strengths:

      The experiments presented here are comprehensive and well-designed, providing compelling evidence for the conclusions. The conclusions will be important to the field.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) EAE is the prototypic T cell-mediated autoimmune disease model, and both Th1 and Th17 cells are implicated in its pathogenesis. In contrast, Th2 and Treg cells and their associated cytokines (such as IL-4 and IL-10) have been shown to play a role in the resolution of EAE, and potentially in the modulation of disease progression. Thus, it will be important to determine whether Asn depletion affects the differentiation of naive CD4+ T cells into corresponding subsets under Th2 and Treg polarization conditions, as well as the expression of lineage-specific transcription factors and cytokine production.

      (2) EAE is characterized by inflammation and demyelination in the central nervous system (CNS), leading to neurological deficits. Myelin destruction is directly correlated with the severity of the disease. For Figure 6, did the authors perform spinal cord histological analysis by hematoxylin and eosin (H&E) or Luxol fast blue (LFB) staining? This is important to rigorously examine pathological EAE symptoms.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      While the importance of asparagine in the differentiation and activation of CD8 T cells has been previously reported, its role in CD4 T cells remained unclear. Using culture media containing specific amino acids, the authors demonstrated that extracellular asparagine promotes CD4 T cell proliferation. Consistent with this, depletion of extracellular asparagine using PEG-AsnASE suppressed CD4 T cell activation. Proteomic analysis focusing on asparagine content revealed that, during the early phase of T cell activation, most asparagine incorporated into proteins is derived from extracellular sources. The authors further confirmed the importance of extracellular asparagine in vivo, demonstrating improved EAE pathology.

      While the data are well organized and convincing, the mechanism by which asparagine deficiency leads to altered T cell differentiation remains unclear. It is also necessary to investigate the transporters involved in asparagine uptake. In particular, elucidating whether different T cell subsets utilize the same or distinct transport mechanisms would provide important insight into the immunoregulatory role of asparagine.

      (1) The finding that asparagine supplementation promotes T cell proliferation under various amino acid conditions is highly significant. However, the concentration at which this effect occurs remains unclear. A titration analysis would be necessary to determine the dose-dependency of asparagine.

      (2) The effects of asparagine deficiency occur during the early phase of T cell activation. Thus, it is likely that the transporters responsible for asparagine uptake are either rapidly induced upon activation or already expressed in the resting state. Since this is central to the focus of the manuscript, it is interesting to identify the transporter responsible for asparagine uptake during early T cell activation. A recent paper (DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.ads350) reported that macrophages utilize Slc6a14 to use extracellular asparagine. Is this also true for CD4+ T cells?

      (3) Given that depletion of extracellular asparagine impairs differentiation of Th1 and Th17 cells, it is possible that TCR signaling is compromised under these conditions. This point should be investigated by targeting downstream signaling molecules such as Lck, ZAP70, or mTOR. Also, does it affect the protein stability of master transcription factors such as T-bet and RORgt?

      (4) Is extracellular asparagine also important for the differentiation of helper T cell subsets other than Th1 and Th17, such as Th2, Th9, and iTreg?

      (5) Asparagine taken up from outside the cell has been shown to be used for de novo protein synthesis (Figure 3E), but are there any proteins that are particularly susceptible to asparagine deficiency? This can be verified by performing proteome analysis, and the effects on Th1/17 subset differentiation mentioned above should also be examined.

      (6) While the importance of extracellular asparagine is emphasized, Asns expression is markedly induced during early T cell activation. Nevertheless, the majority of asparagine incorporated into proteins appears to be derived from extracellular sources. Does genetic deletion of Asns have any impact on early CD4+ T cell activation? The authors indicated that newly synthesized Asns have little impact on CD8+ T cells in the Discussion section, but is this also true for CD4+ T cells? This could be verified through experiments using CRISPR-mediated Asns gene targeting or pharmacological inhibition.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript by Xu et al. reported base-resolution mapping of RNA pseudouridylation in five bacterial species, utilizing recently developed BID-seq. They detected pseudouridine (Ψ) in bacterial rRNA, tRNA, and mRNA, and found growth phase-dependent Ψ changes in tRNA and mRNA. They then focused on mRNA and conducted a comparative analysis of Ψ profiles across different bacterial species. Finally, they developed a deep learning model to predict Ψ sites based on RNA sequence and structure.

      Strengths:

      This is the first comprehensive Ψ map across multiple bacterial species, and systematically reveals Ψ profiles in rRNA, tRNA, and mRNA under exponential and stationary growth conditions. It provides a valuable resource for future functional studies of Ψ in bacteria.

      Weaknesses:

      Ψ is highly abundant on non-coding RNA such as rRNA and tRNA, while its level on mRNA is very low. The manuscript focuses primarily on mRNA, which raises questions about the data quality and the rigor of the analysis. Many conclusions in the manuscript are speculative, based solely on the sequencing data but not supported by additional experiments.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, Xu et al. present a transcriptome-wide, single-base resolution map of RNA pseudouridine modifications across evolutionarily diverse bacterial species using an adapted form of BID-Seq. By optimizing the method for bacterial RNA, the authors successfully mapped modifications in rRNA, tRNA, and, importantly, mRNA across both exponential and stationary growth phases. They uncover evolutionarily conserved Ψ motifs, dynamic Ψ regulation tied to bacterial growth state, and propose functional links between pseudouridylation and bacterial transcript stability, translation, and RNA-protein interactions. To extend these findings, they develop a deep learning model that predicts pseudouridine sites from local sequence and structural features.

      Strengths:

      The authors provide a valuable resource: a comprehensive Ψ atlas for bacterial systems, spanning hundreds of mRNAs and multiple species. The work addresses a gap in the field - our limited understanding of bacterial epitranscriptomics, by establishing both the method and datasets for exploring post-transcriptional modifications.

      Weaknesses:

      The main limitation of the study is that most functional claims (i.e., translation efficiency, mRNA stability, and RNA-binding protein interactions) are based on correlative evidence. While suggestive, these inferences would be significantly strengthened by targeted perturbation of specific Ψ synthases or direct biochemical validation of proposed RNA-protein interactions (e.g., with Hfq). Additionally, the GNN prediction model is a notable advance, but methodological details are insufficient to reproduce or assess its robustness.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study aimed to investigate pseudouridylation across various RNA species in multiple bacterial strains using an optimized BID-seq approach. It examined both conserved and divergent modification patterns, the potential functional roles of pseudouridylation, and its dynamic regulation across different growth conditions.

      Strengths:

      The authors optimized the BID-seq method and applied this important technique to bacterial systems, identifying multiple pseudouridylation sites across different species. They investigated the distribution of these modifications, associated sequence motifs, their dynamics across growth phases, and potential functional roles. These data are of great interest to researchers focused on understanding the significance of RNA modifications, particularly mRNA modifications, in bacteria.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The reliability of BID-seq data is questionable due to a lack of experimental validations.

      (2) The manuscript is not well-written, and the presented work shows a major lack of scientific rigor, as several key pieces of information are missing.

      (3) The manuscript's organization requires significant improvement, and numerous instances of missing or inconsistent information make it difficult to understand the key objectives and conclusions of the study.

      (4) The rationale for selecting specific bacterial species is not clearly explained, and the manuscript lacks a systematic comparison of pseudouridylation among these species.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Alveolar macrophages (AMs) are key sentinel cells in the lungs, representing the first line of defense against infections. There is growing interest within the scientific community in the metabolic and epigenetic reprogramming of innate immune cells following an initial stress, which alters their response upon exposure to a heterologous challenge. In this study, the authors show that exposure to extracellular ATP can shape AM functions by activating the P2X7 receptor. This activation triggers the relocation of the potassium channel TWIK2 to the cell surface, placing macrophages in a heightened state of responsiveness. This leads to the activation of the NLRP3 inflammasome and, upon bacterial internalization, to the translocation of TWIK2 to the phagosomal membrane, enhancing bacterial killing through pH modulation. Through these findings, the authors propose a mechanism by which ATP acts as a danger signal to boost the antimicrobial capacity of AMs.

      Strengths:

      This is a fundamental study in a field of great interest to the scientific community. A growing body of evidence has highlighted the importance of metabolic and epigenetic reprogramming in innate immune cells, which can have long-term effects on their responses to various inflammatory contexts. Exploring the role of ATP in this process represents an important and timely question in basic research. The study combines both in vitro and in vivo investigations and proposes a mechanistic hypothesis to explain the observed phenotype.

      Weaknesses:

      First, the concept of training or trained immunity refers to long-term epigenetic reprogramming in innate immune cells, resulting in a modified response upon exposure to a heterologous challenge. The investigations presented demonstrate phenotypic alterations in AMs seven days after ATP exposure; however, they do not assess whether persistent epigenetic remodeling occurs with lasting functional consequences. Therefore, a more cautious and semantically precise interpretation of the findings would be appropriate.

      Furthermore, the in vivo data should be strengthened by additional analyses to support the authors' conclusions. The authors claim that susceptibility to Pseudomonas aeruginosa infection differs depending on the ATP-induced training effect. Statistical analyses should be provided for the survival curves, as well as additional weight curves or clinical assessments. Moreover, it would be appropriate to complement this clinical characterization with additional measurements, such as immune cell infiltration analysis (by flow cytometry), and quantification of pro-inflammatory cytokines in bronchoalveolar lavage fluid and/or lung homogenates.

      Moreover, the authors attribute the differences in resistance to P. aeruginosa infection to the ATP-induced training effect on AMs, based on a correlation between in vivo survival curves and differences in bacterial killing capacity measured in vitro. These are correlative findings that do not establish a causal role for AMs in the in vivo phenotype. ATP-mediated effects on other (i.e., non-AM) cell populations are omitted, and the possibility that other cells could be affected should be, at least, discussed. Adoptive transfer experiments using AMs would be a suitable approach to directly address this question.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Thompson et al. investigate the impact of prior ATP exposure on later macrophage functions as a mechanism of immune training. They describe that ATP training enhances bactericidal functions, which they connect to the P2x7 ATP receptor, Nlrp3 inflammasome activation, and TWIK2 K+ movement at the cell surface and subsequently at phagosomes during bacterial engulfment. With stronger methodology, these findings could provide useful insight into how ATP can modulate macrophage immune responses, though they are generally an incremental addition to existing literature. The evidence supporting their conclusions is currently inadequate. Gaps in explaining methodology are substantial enough to undermine trust in much of the data presented. Some assays may not be designed rigorously enough for interpretation.

      Strengths:

      The authors demonstrate two novel findings that have sufficient rigor to assess:

      (1) prolonged persistence of TWIK2 at the macrophage plasma membrane following ATP, and can translocate to the phagosome during particle engulfment, which builds upon their prior report of ATP-driven 'training' of macrophages.

      (2) administering mice intra-nasal ATP to 'train' lungs to protect mice from otherwise fatal bacterial infection.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Missing details from methods/reported data: Substantial sections of key methods have not been disclosed (including anything about animal infection models, RNA-sequencing, and western blotting), and the statistical methods, as written, only address two-way comparisons, which would mean analysis was improperly performed. In addition, there is a general lack of transparency - the methods state that only representative data is included in the manuscript, and individual data points are not shown for assays.

      (2) Poor experimental design including missing controls: Particularly problematic are the Seahorse assay data (requires normalization to cell numbers to interpret this bulk assay - differences in cell growth/loss between conditions would confound data interpretation) and bacterial killing assays (as written, this method would be heavily biased by bacterial initial binding/phagocytosis which would confound assessment of killing). Controls need to be included for subcellular fractionating to confirm pure fractions and for dye microscopy to show a negative background. Conclusions from these assays may be incorrect, and in some cases, the whole experiment may be uninterpretable.

      (3) The conclusions overstate what was tested in the experiments: Conceptually, there are multiple places where the authors draw conclusions or frame arguments in ways that do not match the experiments used. Particularly:<br /> a) The authors discuss their findings in the context of importance for AM biology during respiratory infection but in vitro work uses cells that are well-established to be poor mimics of resident AMs (BMDM, RAW), particularly in terms of glycolytic metabolism.<br /> b) In vivo work does not address whether immune cell recruitment is triggered during training.<br /> c) Figure 3 is used to draw conclusions about K+ in response to bacterial engulfment, but actually assesses fungal zymosan particles.<br /> d) Figure 5 is framed in bacterial susceptibility post-viral infection, but the model used is bacterial post-bacterial.<br /> e) In their discussion, the authors propose to have shown TWIK2-mediated inflammasome activation. They link these separately to ATP, but their studies do not test if loss of TWIK2 prevents inflammasome activation in response to ATP (Figure 4E does not use TWIK2 KO).

      In summary, this work contains some useful data showing how ATP can 'train' macrophages. However, it largely lacks the expected level of rigor. For this work to be valuable to the field, it is likely to need substantial improvement in methods reporting, inclusion of missing assay controls, may require repeating key experiments that were run with insufficient methodology (or providing details and supplemental data to prove that methodology was sufficient), and should either add additional experiments that properly test their experimental question or rewrite their conclusions.

    1. Joint Public Review:

      Summary from an earlier round of review:

      This paper summarises responses from a survey completed by around 5,000 academics on their manuscript submission behaviours. The authors find several interesting stylised facts, including (but not limited to):- Women are less likely to submit their papers to highly influential journals (e.g., Nature, Science and PNAS).

      - Women are more likely to cite the demands of co-authors as a reason why they didn’t submit to highly influential journals.

      - Women are also more likely to say that they were advised not to submit to highly influential journals.

      The paper highlights an important point, namely that the submission behaviours of men and women scientists may not be the same (either due to preferences that vary by gender, selection effects that arise earlier in scientists’ careers or social factors that affect men and women differently and also influence submission patterns). As a result, simply observing gender differences in acceptance rates - or a lack thereof - should not be automatically interpreted as as evidence for or against discrimination (broadly defined) in the peer review process.

      Editor’s note: This is the third version of this article.

      Comments made during the peer review of the second version, along with author’s responses to these comments, are available below. Revisions made in response to these comments include changing the colour scheme used for the figures to make the figures more accessible for readers with certain forms of colour blindness.

      Comments made during the peer review of the first version, along with author’s responses to these comments, are available with previous versions of the article.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Chao et al. produced an updated version of the SpliceAI package using modern deep learning frameworks. This includes data preprocessing, model training, direct prediction, and variant effect prediction scripts. They also added functionality for model fine-tuning and model calibration. They convincingly evaluate their newly trained models against those from the original SpliceAI package and investigate how to extend SpliceAI to make predictions in new species. Their comparisons to the original SpliceAI models are convincing on the grounds of model performance and their evaluation of how well the new models match the original's understanding of non-local mutation effects. However, their evaluation of the new calibration functionality would benefit from a more nuanced discussion of the limitations of calibration.

      Strengths

      (1) They provide convincing evidence that their new implementation of SpliceAI matches the performance and mutation effect estimation capabilities of the original model on a similar dataset while benefiting from improved computational efficiencies. This will enable faster prediction and retraining of splicing models for new species as well as easier integration with other modern deep learning tools.

      (2) They produce models with strong performance on non-human model species and a simple well well-documented pipeline for producing models tuned for any species of interest. This will be a boon for researchers working on splicing in these species and make it easy for researchers working on new species to generate their own models.

      (3) Their documentation is clear and abundant. This will greatly aid the ability of others to work with their code base.

      Weaknesses

      (1) Their discussion of their package's calibration functionality does not adequately acknowledge the limitations of model calibration. This is problematic as this is a package intended for general use and users who are not experienced in modeling broadly and the subfield of model calibration specifically may not already understand these limitations. This could lead to serious errors and misunderstandings down the road. A model is not calibrated or uncalibrated in and of itself, only with respect to a specific dataset. In this case they calibrated with respect to the training dataset, a set of canonical transcript annotations. This is a perfectly valid and reasonable dataset to calibrate against. However, this is unlikely to be the dataset the model is applied to in any downstream use case, and this calibration is not guaranteed or expected to hold for any shift in the dataset distribution. For example, in the next section they use ISM based approaches to evaluate which sequence elements the model is sensitive to and their calibration would not be expected to hold for this set of predictions. This issue is particularly worrying in the case of their model because annotation of canonical transcript splice sites is a task that it is unlikely their model will be applied to after training. Much more likely tasks will be things such as predicting the effects of mutations, identification of splice sites that may be used across isoforms beyond just the canonical one, identification of regulatory sequences through ISM, or evaluation of human created sequences for design or evaluation purposes (such as in the context of an MPSA or designing a gene to splice a particular way), we would not expect their calibration to hold in any of these contexts. To resolve this issue, the authors should clarify and discuss this limitation in their paper (and in the relevant sections of the package documentation) to avoid confusing downstream users.

      (2) The clarity of their analysis of mutation effects could be improved with some minor adjustments. While they report median ISM importance correlation it would be helpful to see a histogram of the correlations they observed. Instead of displaying (and calculating correlations using) importance scores of only the reference sequence, showing the importance scores for each nucleotide at each position provides a more informative representation. This would also likely make the plots in 6B clearer.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The paper by Chao et al offers a reimplantation of the SpliceAI algorithm in PyTorch so that the model can more easily/efficiently be retrained. They apply their new implementation of the SpliceAI algorithm, which they call OpenSpliceAI, to several species and compare it against the original model, showing that the results are very similar and that in some small species pre-training on other species helps improve performance.

      Strengths:

      On the upside, the code runs fine and it is well documented.

      Weaknesses:

      The paper itself does not offer much beyond reimplementing SpliceAI. There is no new algorithm, new analysis, new data, or new insights into RNA splicing. There is not even any comparison to many of the alternative methods that have since been published to surpass SpliceAI. Given that some of the authors are well known with a long history of important contributions, our expectations were admittedly different. Still, we hope some readers will find the new implementation useful.

      Update for the revised version:

      The update includes mostly clarifications for tech questions/comments raised by the other two reviewers. There is no additional analysis/results that changes our above initial assessment of this paper's contribution.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      This is an important, interesting, and in-depth study examining the role of Sp5/8 transcription factors in maintaining the neuromesodermal progenitor (NMP) niche. The authors first used Sp5/8 double conditional KO mouse embryos to establish that these factors function in the NMP niche to promote trunk elongation. They then conducted extensive single-cell analyses on embryos of various genetic mutant backgrounds to unravel the complex and intricate interactions between Wnt signaling and Sp5/8. The key conclusion from these experiments is that Sp5/8 function within an autoregulatory loop crucial for maintaining the NMP niche. The authors went on to identify and characterize a novel enhancer element downstream of the Wnt3a coding sequence, which mediates the effects of Sp5/8 on Wnt3a expression. Overall, the data presented are compelling and of high quality, and the study offers a prime example of how a relatively small set of signaling pathways and transcription factors can function in concert to impart robustness to developmental processes.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Chalamalasetty et al. investigate the regulatory circuit of signaling molecules and transcription factors that drive the fate of neuromesodermal competent progenitors (NMCs). NMCs contribute to Sox2-positive spinal cord and Tbxt/Bra-expressing somitic mesoderm, and this choice is governed by the interplay between Wnt3a and Fgf signaling. The authors discovered that the transcription factors SP5 and SP8 participate in this process. Mouse genetics, in vivo development, and transcription factors profiling point to a model where SP5 and SP8 directly regulate Wnt3a expression to foster Tbxt-marked mesoderm formation at the expense of Sox2-marked neural ectoderm. Mechanistically, SP5/8 bind to an enhancer which the authors characterize: its activity depends on the presence of SP5, CDX2, TCF7, and TBXT binding sites, and it is activated only in primitive streak cells at E7.5, in NMP, and in caudal and somitic mesoderm, underscoring the tissue and stage-specific nature of this Wnt3a enhancer.

      Moreover, the authors find that SP5/8 likely regulate the TCF7 association with the chromatin and compete for its binding to the TLE repressor.

      The study is extensive, compelling, and well written. The combination of in vivo evidence with single-cell transcriptomics, transcription factors profiling, and in vitro regulatory element characterization is notable and builds a convincing picture of the action of SP5/SP8.

      Here, I provide a series of comments and questions that, if addressed and clarified, could, in my opinion, improve the study.

      (1) While Sp5 and Sp8 are both present in NMCs, their expression does not fully overlap. Sp5 is also detected in caudal and presomitic mesoderm, notochord and gut, while Sp8 overlaps with Sox2 in neural progenitors of the spinal cord and brain (Fig. 1D). Accordingly, Sp8 expression is also activated by the neural-promoting RA+Fgf. It is not easy for me to reconcile this non-fully overlapping expression pattern - and in particular the overlap of Sp8 and Sox2 - with the presumed redundancy (or similarity of function) described later. Sp5/8 dko NMCs show reduced Tbxt and expanded Sox2, indicating that SP8 also represses Sox2 or neural fate, an observation confirmed by Sp8 overexpression (Figure 4c). What is the explanation for this, and is the function of SP8 in Sox2-positive neural progenitors different from its Wnt3a-sustaining role in NMCs? Or what am I missing?

      (2) I suggest that the authors show relevant ChIP-seq peaks in Figure 3 to lend credibility to the complicated overlapping Venn diagrams. I consider visual inspection of peak tracks as primary quality control of this type of experiment. A good choice could be the cis-regulatory elements at Sp5, Sp8, Tbxt, Cdx1, 2, 4 bound by TBXT and either CDX2, SP5, or SP8 (now referring to the Venn diagrams and the annotated peak table). On ChIP-seq visualization, in reference to Figures 5 and 7, I also suggest that the authors show the tracks of a negative control (IgG, non-related antibody, or better anti-flag in Sp5/8 dko). While I do not doubt the validity of these experiments, there are peaks in these figures bound by all factors tested that could be suspicious (even though, admittedly, they look like genuinely good TF peaks). A negative track would clearly show beyond any doubt that these are not suspect regions of positive unspecific signal caused by open chromatin, excessive cross-linking, or antibody cross-reaction.

      (3) SP5 here is found as a direct inducer of Wnt3a expression, and accordingly positive regulator of Tbxt and mesoderm, caudal development. I find this in partial contradiction with a finding by the Willert group (PMID: 29044119). They show that "genes with an associated SP5 peak, such as SP5 itself, AXIN2, AMOTL2, GPR37, GSC, MIXL1, NODAL, and T, show significant upregulation in expression upon Wnt3a treatment in SP5 mutant cells". There, essentially, SP5 inhibits Wnt target genes. While the authors are aware of this and cite Huggins et al., I find that this deserves a better discussion addressing how opposite functions could be sustained in different contexts, if these really are different cellular contexts in the first place, or if this could result from different methodologies.

      (4) The gastruloid experiment is nice, but I wonder whether there is any marker that the authors can use to show that other features of the gastruloids respond accordingly. For example, is the Sox2 expression domain expanded? And is there any unaffected marker to emphasize the specificity of the decreased Tbxt and Cdx2?

      (5) SP5/8 seems to enhance the TCF7 occupancy at WRE. And then, SP5/8 appears to counteract the presence of TLE repressor associated with TCF7. While these two mechanisms are interesting, they are not necessarily interconnected. According to the still-established view, TCF7 should be associated with WRE even in the absence of the Wnt signal, when TLEs are also present on the locus. One could expect that SP5 competes with TLE, to decrease its presence on TCF7-bound loci, leaving the abundance of TCF7 binding unchanged. Yet, the authors also observe that the TCF7 association changes. What is the mechanism implied? Do they perhaps consider a TCF7L1 > TCF7 switch, and if so, what evidence exists for this?

      (6) Along the same line as above, I wonder whether beta-catenin binding is also enhanced at these sites? Any TCF/LEF would require beta-catenin for gene upregulation.

      (7) The authors write that "Small Tle peaks were identified at these WREs in WT cells, demonstrating that both repressive Tle and activating Tcf7 could be detected at active genes". However, ChIP-seq is a population assay, and it is possible - more plausible, in fact - that cells displaying TLE binding are not expressing the target genes.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This is a well-done study. It shows, in a comprehensive manner, that Sp5 and Sp8 play essential roles in maintaining the complicated positive feedback circuitry needed for specification of neuromesodermal competent progenitors (NMCs) in caudal mesodermal development in murine embryos.

      Strengths:

      The developmental genetics, transcriptomic, and genomic survey of TF binding are all satisfactory and make a compelling story. The CRISPR deletion of the Wnt3a downstream enhancer clearly demonstrates that it plays an important role in the positive feedback circuit.

      Weaknesses:

      My only concerns are some of the language surrounding the mechanistic interpretation of the Wnt3a downstream enhancer and the relationship between TCF and TLE binding.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Using a combination of EEG and behavioural measurements, the authors investigate the degree to which processing of spatially-overlapping targets (coherent motion) and distractors (affective images) are sampled rhythmically and how this affects behaviour. They found that both target processing (via measurement of amplitude modulations of SSVEP amplitude to target frequency) and distractor processing (via MVPA decoding accuracy of bandpassed EEG relative to distractor SSVEP frequency) displayed a pronounced rhythm at ~1Hz, time-locked to stimulus onset. Furthermore, the relative phase of this target/distractor sampling predicted accuracy of coherent motion detection across participants.

      Strengths:

      - The authors are addressing a very interesting question with respect to sampling of targets and distractors, using neurophysiological measurements to their advantage in order to parse out target and distractor processing.<br /> - The general EEG analysis pipeline is sensible and well-described.<br /> - The main result of rhythmic sampling of targets and distractors is striking and very clear even on a participant-level.<br /> - The authors have gone to quite a lot of effort to ensure the validity of their analyses, especially in the Supplementary Material.<br /> - It is incredibly striking how the phase of both target and distractor processing are so aligned across trials for a given participant. I would have thought that any endogenous fluctuation in attention or stimulus processing like that would not be so phase aligned. I know there is literature on phase resetting in this context, the results seem very strong here and it is worth noting. The authors have performed many analyses to rule out signal processing artifacts, e.g. the sideband and beating frequency analyses.

      Weaknesses:

      - In general, the representation of target and distractor processing is a bit of a reach. Target processing is represented by SSVEP amplitude, which is going to most likely be related to the contrast of the dots, as opposed to representing coherent motion energy which is the actual target. These may well be linked (e.g. greater attention to the coherent motion task might increase SSVEP amplitude) but I would call it a limitation of the interpretation. Decoding accuracy of emotional content makes sense as a measure of distractor processing, and the supplementary analysis comparing target SSVEP amplitude to distractor decoding accuracy is duly noted. Overall, this limitation remains and has been noted in the Limitations section.<br /> - Then comparing SSVEP amplitude to emotional category decoding accuracy feels a bit like comparing apples with oranges. They have different units and scales and reflect probably different neural processes. Is the result the authors find not a little surprising in this context? This relationship does predict performance and is thus intriguing, but I think this methodological aspect needs to be discussed further. For example, is the phase relationship with behaviour a result of a complex interaction between different levels of processing (fundamental contrast vs higher order emotional processing)? Again, this has been noted in the Limitations section, but changing the data to z-scores doesn't really take care of the conceptual issue, i.e. that on-screen contrast changes would necessarily be distracting during emotional category decision-making.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      In this study, Xiong et al. investigate whether rhythmic sampling - a process typically observed in the attended processing of visual stimuli - extends to task-irrelevant distractors. By using EEG with frequency tagging and multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA), they aimed to characterize the temporal dynamics of both target and distractor processing and examine whether these processes oscillate in time. The central hypothesis is that target and distractor processing occur rhythmically, and the phase relationship between these rhythms correlates with behavioral performance.

      Major Strengths<br /> (1) The extension of rhythmic attentional sampling to include distractors is a novel and interesting question.<br /> (2) The decoding of emotional distractor content using MVPA from SSVEP signals is an elegant solution to the problem of assessing distractor engagement in the absence of direct behavioral measures.<br /> (3) The finding that relative phase (between 1 Hz target and distractor processes) predicts behavioral performance is compelling.

      Major Weaknesses and Limitations<br /> (1) The central claim of 1 Hz rhythmic sampling is insufficiently validated. The windowing procedure (0.5s windows with 0.25s step) inherently restricts frequency resolution, potentially biasing toward low-frequency components like 1 Hz. Testing different window durations or providing controls would significantly strengthen this claim.<br /> (2) The study lacks a baseline or control condition without distractors. This makes it difficult to determine whether the distractor-related decoding signals or the 1 Hz effect reflect genuine distractor processing or more general task dynamics.<br /> (3) The pairwise decoding accuracies for distractor categories hover close to chance (~55%), raising concerns about robustness. While statistically above chance, the small effect sizes need careful interpretation, particularly when linked to behavior.<br /> (4) Neither target nor distractor signal strength (SSVEP amplitude) correlates with behavioral accuracy. The study instead relies heavily on relative phase, which-while interesting-may benefit from additional converging evidence.<br /> (5) Phase analysis is performed between different types of signals hindering their interpretability (time-resolved SSVEP amplitude and time-resolved decoding accuracy).

      The authors largely achieved their stated goal of assessing rhythmic sampling of distractors. However, the conclusions drawn - particularly regarding the presence of 1 Hz rhythmicity - rest on analytical choices that should be scrutinized further. While the observed phase-performance relationship is interesting and potentially impactful, the lack of stronger and convergent evidence on the frequency component itself reduces confidence in the broader conclusions.

      If validated, the findings will advance our understanding of attentional dynamics and competition in complex visual environments. Demonstrating that ignored distractors can be rhythmically sampled at similar frequencies to targets has implications for models of attention and cognitive control. However, the methodological limitations currently constrain the paper's impact.

      Additional Considerations<br /> • The use of EEG-fMRI is mentioned but not leveraged. If BOLD data were collected, even exploratory fMRI analyses (e.g., distractor modulation in visual cortex) could provide valuable converging evidence.<br /> • In turn, removal of fMRI artifacts might introduce biases or alter the data. For instance, the authors might consider investigating potential fMRI artifact harmonics around 1 Hz to address concerns regarding induced spectral components.

      Comments on revisions:

      The authors have addressed my previous points, and the manuscript is substantially improved. The key methodological clarifications have been incorporated, and the interpretation of findings has been appropriately moderated. I have no further major concerns.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This work presents an interesting circuit dissection of the neural system allowing a ctenophore to keep its balance and orientation in its aquatic environment by using a fascinating structure called the statocyst. By combining serial-section electron microscopy with behavioral recordings, the authors found a population of neurons that exists as a syncytium and could associate these neurons with specific functions related to controlling the beating of cilia located in the statocyst. The type A ANN neurons participate in arresting cilia beating, and the type B ANN neurons participate in resuming cilia beating and increasing their beating frequency.

      Moreover, the authors found that bridge cells are connected with the ANN neurons, giving them the role of rhythmic modulators.

      From these observations, the authors conclude that the control is coordination instead of feedforward sensory-motor function, a hypothesis that had been put forth in the past but could not be validated until now. They also compare it to the circuitry implementing a similar behavior in a species that belongs to a different phylum, where the nervous system is thought to have evolved separately.

      Therefore, this work significantly advances our knowledge of the circuitry implementing the control of the cilia that participate in statocyst function, which ultimately allows the animal to correct its orientation. It represents an example of systems neuroscience explaining how the nervous system allows an animal to solve a specific problem and puts it in an evolutionary perspective, showing a convincing case of convergent evolution.

      Strengths:

      The evidence for how the circuitry is connected is convincing. Pictures of synapses showing the direction of connectivity are clear, and there are good reasons to believe that the diagram inferred is valid, even though we can always expect that some connections are missing.

      The evidence for how the cilia change their beating frequency is also convincing, and the paradigm and recording methods seem pretty robust.

      The authors achieved their aims, and the results support their conclusions. This work impacts its field by presenting a mechanism by which ctenophores correct their balance, which will provide a template for comparison with other sensory systems.

      Weaknesses:

      The evidence supporting the claim that the neural circuitry presented here controls the cilia beating is more correlational because it only relies on the fact that the location of the two types of ANN neurons coincides with the quadrants that are affected in the behavioral recordings. Discussing ways by which causality could be established might be helpful.

      The explanation of the relevance of this work could be improved. The conclusion that the work hints at coordination instead of feedforward sensory-motor control is explained over only a few lines. The authors could provide a more detailed explanation of how the two models compete (coordination vs feedforward sensory-motor control), and why choosing one option over the other could provide advantages in this context.

      Since the fact that the ANN neurons form a syncytium is an important finding of this study, it would be useful to have additional illustrations of it. For instance, pictures showing anastomosing membranes could typically be added in Figure 2.

      Also, to better establish the importance of the study, it could be useful to explain why the balancers' cilia spontaneously beat in the first place (instead of being static and just acting as stretch sensors).

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, the authors describe the production of a high-resolution connectome for the statocyst of a ctenophore nervous system. This study is of particular interest because of the apparent independent evolution of the ctenophore nervous system. The statocyst is a component of the aboral organ, which is used by ctenophores to sense gravity and regulate the activity of the organ's balancer cilia. The EM reconstruction of the aboral organ was carried out on a five-day-old larva of the model ctenophore Mnemiopsis leidyi. To place their connectome data in a functional context, the authors used high-speed imaging of ciliary beating in immobilized larvae. With these data, the authors were able to model the circuitry used for gravity sensing in a ctenophore larva.

      Strengths:

      Because of it apparently being the sister phylum to all other metazoans, Ctenophora is a particularly important group for studies of metazoan evolution. Thus, this work has much to tell us about how animals evolved. Added to that is the apparent independent evolution of the ctenophore nervous system. This study provides the first high-resolution connectomic analysis of a portion of a ctenophore nervous system, extending previous studies of the ctenophore nervous system carried out by Sid Tamm. As such, it establishes the methodology for high-resolution analysis of the ctenophore nervous system. While the generation of a connectome is in and of itself an important accomplishment, the coupling of the connectome data with analysis of the beating frequency of balancer cell cilia provides a functional context for understanding how the organization of the neural circuitry in the aboral organ carries out gravity sensing. In addition, the authors identified a new type of syncytial neuron in Mnemiopsis. Interestingly, the authors show that the neural circuitry controlling cilia beating in Mnemiopsis shares features with the circuitry that controls ciliary movement in the annelid Platynereis, suggesting convergent evolution of this circuitry in the two organisms. The data in this paper are of high quality, and the analyses have been thoroughly and carefully done.

      Weaknesses:

      The paper has no obvious weaknesses.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      It has been a long time since I enjoyed reviewing a paper as much as this one. In it, the authors generate an unprecedented view of the aboral organ of a 5-day-old ctenophore. They proceed to derive numerous insights by reconstructing the populations and connections of cell types, with up to 150 connections from the main Q1-4 neuron.

      Strengths:

      The strengths of the analysis are the sophisticated imaging methods used, the labor-intensive reconstruction of individual neurons and organelles, and especially the mapping of synapses. The synaptic connections to and from the main coordinating neurons allow the authors to create a polarized network diagram for these components of the aboral organ. These connections give insight into the potential functions of the major neurons. This also gives some unexpected results, particularly the lack of connections from the balancer system to the coordinating system.

      Weaknesses:

      There were no significant weaknesses in the paper - only a slate of interesting unanswered questions to motivate future studies.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript "Realistic coupling enables flexible macroscopic traveling waves in the mouse cortex" by Sun, Forger, and colleagues presents a novel computational framework for studying macroscopic traveling waves in the mouse cortex by integrating realistic brain connectivity data with large-scale neural simulations.

      The key contributions include:<br /> (1) developing an algorithm that combines spatial transcriptomic data (providing detailed neuron positions and molecular properties) with voxelized connectivity data from the Allen Brain Atlas to construct neuron-to-neuron connections across ~300,000 cortical neurons;<br /> (2) building a GPU-accelerated simulation platform capable of modeling this large-scale network with both excitatory and inhibitory Hodgkin-Huxley neurons;<br /> (3) extending phase-based analysis methods from 2D to 3D to quantify traveling wave activity in the realistic brain geometry; and<br /> (4) demonstrating that realistic Allen connectivity generates significantly higher levels of macroscopic traveling waves compared to simplified local or uniform connectivity patterns.

      The study reveals that wave activity depends non-monotonically on coupling strength and that slow oscillations (0.5-4 Hz) are particularly conducive to large-scale wave propagation, providing new insights into how anatomical connectivity enables flexible spatiotemporal dynamics across the cortex.

      Strengths:

      The authors leverage two existing dense datasets of spatial transcriptomic data and connection strength between pairwise voxels in the mouse cortex in a novel way, allowing for the computational model to capture molecular and functional properties of neurons as determined by their neurotransmitter profiles, rather than making arbitrary assignments of excitatory/inhibitory roles. Additionally, the author's expansion of 2D phase dynamics to 3D phase gradient analysis methods is important and can be widely applied to calcium imaging, LFP recordings, and likely other electrophysiological recordings.

      Weaknesses:

      Despite these important computational advancements, a few aspects of this model, particularly the inability to validate the model with experimental neural data, diminish my enthusiasm for this paper:

      (1) The model's Allen connectivity approach overlooks critical aspects of real cortical dynamics. Most importantly, it excludes subcortical structures, especially the thalamus, which drives cortical traveling waves through thalamocortical interactions. The authors' method of electrically stimulating all layer 4 neurons simultaneously to initiate waves is artificially crude and bears little resemblance to natural wave generation mechanisms.

      (2) The model handles voxel-to-voxel connections crudely when neurons have mixed excitatory/inhibitory properties and varying synaptic strengths. Real connectivity differs dramatically between neuron types (pyramidal cells vs. interneurons, across cortical layers), but the model only distinguishes excitatory and inhibitory neurons. Additionally, uniform synaptic weights ignore natural variations in connection strength based on neuron type, distance, and functional role. Integrating the updated thalamocortical dataset mentioned by the authors, even at regional resolution, would substantially improve the model.

      (3) While the authors bridge microscopic (single neuron) and mesoscopic (regional connectivity) data to study macroscopic (whole-cortex) waves, they don't integrate the distinct mechanisms operating at each scale. The framework demonstrates that realistic connectivity enables macroscopic waves but fails to connect how wave dynamics emerge and interact across spatial scales systematically.

      (4) Claims that Allen connectivity produces higher phase gradient directionality (PGD) than local connectivity appear limited to delta oscillations at very specific coupling strengths and applied currents. Few parameter combinations show significantly higher PGD for Allen connectivity, and these are generally low PGD values overall.

      (5) Broadly, it's unclear how this computational framework can study memory, learning, sleep, sensory processing, or disease states, given the disconnect between simulated intracellular voltages and the local field potentials or other electrophysiological measurements typically used to study cortical traveling waves. While computationally impressive, the practical research applications remain vague.

      (6) The paper needs a clearer explanation for why medium coupling (100%) eliminates waves in Allen connectivity (Figure 6) while stronger coupling (150%) restores them.

      (7) Does using a single connectivity parameter (ρ = 300) across all regions miss important regional differences in cortical connectivity density?

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This work presents a spiking network model of traveling waves at the whole-brain scale in the mouse neocortex. The authors use data from the Allen Institute to reconstruct connectivity between different neocortical sites. They then quantify macroscopic traveling waves following stimulation of all layer 4 neurons in the neocortex.

      Strengths:

      Overall, the results are interesting and shed new light on the dynamic organization of activity across the neocortex of the mouse. The paper uses realistic neuron models specifically fit to intracellular recordings, demonstrating that traveling waves occur in the mouse neocortex with both realistic connectivity and realistic single-neuron dynamics. The paper is also well-written in general. For these reasons, the authors have generally achieved their aims in this work.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Description of Algorithm 1:<br /> While the Methods section clearly explains the density parameter \rho, the statement on line 358 concerning the "ideal" average number of connections is a little unclear. The authors should explicitly clarify that \rho is a free parameter that can be adjusted to balance computational feasibility (for a given set of computational resources) and biological fidelity.

      (2) Lines 102-103:<br /> The \rho parameter used here results in approximately 300 connections per neuron on average. The authors should state clearly that the number of connections per cell is the key determinant of computational feasibility (cf. Morrison et al., Neural Computation, 2005). The authors should also review neuronal density and synaptic connectivity in the mouse neocortex and clearly reference density and connectivity in their model to the biological scales found in the mouse.

      (3) Line 131:<br /> From the plots in Figure 2, it is not clear that the stimulus response is necessarily a rhythmic oscillation, in the sense of a single narrowband frequency.

      (4) Line 217:<br /> The authors should clarify how these findings relate to the results from Mohajerani et al. (Nature Neuroscience, 2013) or differ from them.

      (5) Line 230:<br /> Because higher temporal frequency activity also tends to be more spatially localized, a correlation between PGD and temporal frequency could be an inherent consequence of this relationship, rather than a meaningful result.

      (6) Line 247-248:<br /> It is not clear that the algorithm for generating connections between neurons presented here really relates to those for community detection. For example, in the case of the Allen Institute data, the communities are essentially in the data already.

      (7) Line 284-285:<br /> The relationship between conduction delay is more direct than this sentence suggests. Conduction delay is fundamentally determined by the time required for action potentials to propagate along axons, making it intrinsically linked to anatomical distance.

      (8) Line 287-288:<br /> The authors suggest at this point that they do not have enough information to estimate time delays due to axonal conduction along white matter fibers. However, experimental data from white matter connections typically includes information about fiber length, which does enable estimating conduction delays. These estimations have been previously implemented for Allen Institute connectome data in the mouse (Choi and Mihalas, PLoS Comput Biology, 2019) and human connectome data (Budzinski et al., Physical Review Research, 2023).

      (9) Lines 294-295:<br /> Several methods do exist for detecting and characterizing wave dynamics in three-dimensional data (Budzinski et al., Physical Review Research, 2023).

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary: This study investigated how visuospatial attention influences the way people build simplified mental representations to support planning and decision-making. Using computational modeling and virtual maze navigation, the authors examined whether spatial proximity and the spatial arrangement of obstacles determine which elements are included in participants' internal models of a task. The study developed and tested an extension of the value-guided construal (VGC) model that incorporates features of spatial attention for selecting simpler task mental representation.

      Strengths:

      (1) Original Perspective: The study introduces an explicit attentional component to established models of planning, offering an approach that bridges perception, attention, and decision-making.

      (2) Methodological Approach: The combination of computational modeling, behavioral data, and eye-tracking provides converging measures to assess the relationship between attention and planning representations.

      (3) Cross-validated data: The study relies on the analysis of three separate datasets, two already published and an additional novel one. This allows for cross-validation of the findings and enhances the robustness of the evidence.

      (4) Focus on Individual Differences: Reports of how individual variability in attentional "spillover" correlates with the sparsity of task representations and spatial proximity add depth to the analysis.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Clarity of the VGC model and behavioral task: The exposition of the VGC model lacks sufficient detail for non-expert readers. It is not clear how this model infers which maze obstacles are relevant or irrelevant for planning, nor how the maze tasks specifically operationalize "planning" versus other cognitive processes.

      The method for classifying obstacles as relevant or irrelevant to the task and connecting metacognitive awareness (i.e., participants' reports of noticing obstacles) to attentional capture is not well justified. The rationale for why awareness serves as a valid attention proxy, as opposed to behavioral or neurophysiological markers, should be clearer.

      (2) Attention framework: The account of attention is largely limited to the "spotlight" model. When solving a maze, participants trace the correct trail, following it mentally with their overt or covert attention. In this perspective, relevant concepts are also rooted in attention literature pertaining to object-based attention using tasks like curve tracing (e.g., Pooresmaeili & Roelfsema, 2014) and to mental maze solving (e.g., Wong & Scholl, 2024), which may be highly relevant and add nuance to the current work. This view of attention may be more pertinent to the task than models of simultaneously tracking multiple objects cited here. Prior work (notably from the Roelfsema group) indicates that attentional engagement in curve-tracing tasks may be a continuous, bottom-up process that progressively spreads along a trajectory, in time and space, rather than a "spotlight" that simply travels along the path. The spread of attention depends on the spatial proximity to distractors - a point that could also be pertinent to the findings here.

      Moreover, the tracing of a "solution" trail in a maze may be spontaneous and not only a top-down voluntary operation (Wong & Scholl, 2024), a finding that requires a more careful framing of the link to conscious perception discussed in the manuscript.

      Conceptualizing attention as a spatial spotlight may therefore oversimplify its role in navigation and planning. Perhaps the observed attentional modulation reflects a perceptual stage of building the trail in the maze rather than a filter for a later representation for more efficient decision making and planning. A fuller discussion of whether the current model and data can distinguish between these frameworks would benefit readers.

      (3) Lateralization of attention: The analysis considers whether relevant information is distributed bilaterally or unilaterally across the visual display, but does not sufficiently address evidence for attentional asymmetries across the left and right visual fields due to hemispheric specialization (e.g., Bartolomeo & Seidel Malkinson, 2019). Whether effects differ for left versus right hemifield arrangements is not made explicit in the presented findings.

      (4) Individual differences: Individual differences in attentional modulation are a strength of the work, but similar analyses exploring individual variation in lateralization effects could provide further insight, and the lack of such analyses may mask important effects.

      (5) Distinction between overt and covert attention: The current report at times equates eye movement patterns with the locus of attention. However, attention can be covertly shifted without corresponding gaze changes (see, for example, Pooresmaeili & Roelfsema, 2014).

      The implications for interpreting the relationship between eye movement, memory, and attention in this setting are not fully addressed. The potential dynamics of attention along a maze trajectory and their impact on lateralization analysis would benefit from further clarification.

      Appraisal of Aims and Results:

      The study sets out to determine how spatial attention shapes the construction of task representations in planning contexts. The authors provide evidence that spatial proximity and arrangement influence which environmental features are incorporated into internal models used for navigation, and that accounting for these effects improves model predictions. There is clear documentation of individual variation, with some participants showing greater attentional spillover and more sparse awareness profiles.

      However, some conceptual and methodological aspects would be clearer with greater engagement with the broader literature on attention dynamics, a more explicit justification of operational choices, and more targeted lateralization analyses.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Castanheira et al. investigate the role of spatial attention for planning during three maze navigation experiments (one new experiment and two existing datasets). Effective planning in complex situations requires the construction of simplified representations of the task at hand. The authors find that these mental representations (as assessed by conscious awareness) of a given stimulus are influenced by (spatially) surrounding stimuli. Individual participants varied in the degree to which attention influenced their task representations, and this attentional effect correlated with the sparsity of representations (as measured by the range of awareness reports across all stimuli). Spatially grouping task-relevant information on either the left or right side of the maze led to mental representations more similar to optimal representations predicted by the value-guided construal (VGC) model - a normative model describing a theoretical approach to simplifying complex task information. Finally, the authors propose an update to this model, incorporating an attentional spotlight component; the revised descriptive model predicts empirical task representations better than the original (normative) VGC model.

      Strengths:

      The novelty of this study lies in the proposal and investigation of a cognitive mechanism through which a normative model like value-guided construal can enable human planning. After proposing attention as this mechanism, the authors make concrete hypotheses about mismatches between the VGC predictions and real human behavior, which are experimentally validated. Thus, not only does this study describe a possible mechanism for simplification of task information for planning, but the authors also propose a descriptive model, revising VGC to incorporate this attentional component.

      A strength of this paper is the variety of investigative approaches: analysis of existing data, novel experiment, and a computational approach to predict experimental findings from a theoretical model. Analyzing pre-existing datasets increases the size of the participant cohort and strengthens the authors' conclusions. Meanwhile, comparing the predictions of the existing normative model and the authors' own refined model is a clever approach to substantiate their claims. In addition, the authors describe several crucial controls, which are key to the interpretability of their results. In particular, the eye tracking results were critical.

      In summary, this paper constitutes an important step toward a more complete understanding of the human ability to plan.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) There is a critical conceptual gap in the study and its interpretation, mainly due to the reliance on a self-report metric of awareness (rather than an objective measure of behavioral performance).

      a. Awareness is tested by a 9-point self-report scale. It is currently unclear why awareness of task-irrelevant obstacles in this task would necessarily compromise optimal planning. There is no indication of whether self-reported awareness affects performance (e.g., navigation path distance, time to complete the maze, number of errors). Such behavioral evidence of planning would be more compelling.

      b. Relatedly, it would have been more convincing to have an objective measure of awareness, for instance, how the presence or absence of a "task-irrelevant" obstacle affects performance (e.g., change navigation path distance or time to complete the maze), or whether participants can accurately recall the location of obstacles.

      c. Consequently, I'm not sure that we can conclude that the spatial context does impact participants' ability to plan spatial navigation or to "incorporate task-relevant information into their construal". We know that the spatial context affects subjective (self-reported) awareness, but the authors do not present evidence that spatial context affects behavioral performance.

      d. Another concern that may complicate interpretation is the following: Figure 3c shows improved VGC model predictions (steeper slope) for mazes with greater lateralization. However, there are notable outliers in these plots, where a high lateralization index does not correspond to good model performance. There is currently no discussion/explanation of these cases.

      (2) I noticed an issue with clarity regarding task-relevance. It is currently not fully clear which obstacles are "task irrelevant". Also, the term is used inconsistently, sometimes conflating with "awareness". For example, in the "Attentional spotlight model of task representations" section, the authors state that "task-relevant information becomes less relevant when surrounded by task-irrelevant information". But they really mean that participants become less aware of those task-relevant obstacles. I assume task-relevance is an objective characteristic related to maze organization, not to a participant's construal. Indeed, the following paragraph provides evidence of model predictions of awareness.

      (3) The behavioral paradigm has some distinct disadvantages, and the validity of the task is not backed up by behavioral data.

      a. I understand the need for central fixation, but it also makes the task less naturalistic.

      b. The task with its top-down grid view does not seem to mimic real human navigation. Though this grid may be similar to mental maps we form for navigation, the sensory stimuli corresponding to possible paths and to spatial context during real-life navigation are very different.

      c. Behavioral performance is not reported, so it is unknown whether participants are able to properly complete the task. The task seems pretty difficult to navigate, especially when the obstacles disappear, and in combination with the central fixation.

      d. There is no discussion of whether/how this navigation task generalizes to other forms of planning.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors build on a recent computational model of planning, the "value-guided construal" framework by Ho et al. (2022), which proposes that people plan by constructing simple models of a task, such as by attending to a subset of obstacles in a maze. They analyze both published experimental data and new experimental data from a task in which participants report attention to objects in mazes. The authors find that attention to objects is affected by spatial proximity to other objects (i.e., attentional overspill) as well as whether relevant objects are lateralized to the same hemifield. To account for these results, the authors propose a "spotlight-VGC" model, in which, after calculating attention scores based on the original VGC model, attention to objects is enhanced based on distance. They find that this model better explains participant responses when objects are lateralized to different hemifields. These results demonstrate complex interactions between filtering of task-relevant information and more classical signatures of attentional selection.

      Strengths:

      (1) The paper builds on existing modeling work in a novel manner and integrates classic results on attention into the computational framework.

      (2) The authors report new and extensive analyses of existing data that shed light on additional sources of systematic variability in responses related to attentional spillover effects

      (3) They collect new data using new stimuli in the original paradigm that directly test predictions related to the lateralization of task-relevant information, including eye tracking data that allows them to control for possible confounds.

      (4) The extended model (spotlight-VGC) provides a formal account of these new results.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The spotlight-VGC model has a free parameter - the "width" of the attentional spotlight. This seems to have been fixed to be 3 squares. It would be good if the authors could describe a more principled procedure for selecting the width so that others can use the model in other contexts.

      (2) Have the authors considered other ways in which factors such as attentional spillover and lateralization could be incorporated into the model? The spotlight-VGC model, as presented, involves first computing VGC predictions and only afterwards computing spillover. This seems psychologically implausible, since it supposes that the "optimal" representation is first formed and then it gets corrupted. Is there a way to integrate these biases directly into the VGC framework, perhaps as a prior on construals? The authors gesture towards this when they talk about "inductive biases", but this is not formalized.

      (3) Can the authors rule out that the lateralization effects are the result of memory biases since the main measure used is a self-report of attention?

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      This work provides a valuable toolkit for endogenous isolation of projection neuron subtypes. With further validation, it could present a solid method for low-input ribosome affinity purification using a ribosomal RNA (rRNA) antibody. The experimental evidence for the distinct ribosomal complexes is limited to this method and indirect support from complementary analyses of pre-existing data. However, with additional experimental data to support the specificity of ribosomal complex pulldown and confirmation of the putative ribosomal complex proteins of interest, the study would provide compelling evidence for translation regulation of neuronal development through compositional ribosome heterogeneity. This work would be of interest to neuroscientists, developmental biologists, and those studying translational networks underlying gene regulation.

      Strengths

      (1) This in vivo labeling of specific projection neurons and ribosomal rRNA affinity purification method accommodates a low input of <100K somata per replicate, which is useful for the study of neuronal subtypes with limited input. In principle, this set of techniques could work across different cell types with limited input, depending on the molecule used for cell type labeling.

      (2) The authors are also able to isolate endogenous neurons with minimal perturbation up to the point of collection, preserving the native state for the neuron in vivo as long as possible prior to processing.

      (3) This study identified over a dozen potential non-ribosomal proteins associated with SCPN ribosomal complexes, as well as a ribosomal protein enriched in CPN.

      Limitations

      (1) In this study, the authors address the advantages of their ribosomal complex isolation method in SCPN and CPN against RPL22-HA affinity purification. While this does show more pull-down of the ribosomal RNA by the Y10B rRNA antibody, the authors claim this method identifies cell-type-specific ribosomal complex proteins without demonstrating a positive control for the method's specificity. There are very limited experiments to truly delineate how "specific" this method is working and whether there could be contamination from other complexes bound by the antibody. I see this as the major limitation that should be addressed. To boost their claims of capturing cell-type-specific ribosomal complexes, the authors could consider applying their rRNA affinity purification pipeline to compare cell types with well-characterized ribosome-associated proteins, like mouse embryonic stem cells and HELA cells. The reviewer can completely appreciate the elegance in the neural characterization here, but it seems there needs to be a solid foothold on the specificity of the method, perhaps facilitated by cell types that can be more readily scaled up and tested.

      (2) The authors followed up on their differentially enriched ribosomal complex proteins by analyzing the ribosome association of these proteins in external datasets. While this analysis supports the ribosome-association of these proteins, there is limited experimental validation of physical association with the ribosome, much less any functional characterization. The reciprocal pulldown of PRKCE is promising; however, I would recommend orthogonal validation of several putative ribosomal complex proteins to increase confidence. Specifically, the authors could use sucrose gradient fractionation of SCPN and CPN, followed by a western blot to identify the putative interaction with the 80S monosome or polysomes. This would also provide evidence towards the pulldown capturing association with mature ribosome species, which is currently unclear. This experiment would provide substantial evidence for the direct association of these non-ribosomal proteins with subtype-specific ribosomal complexes.

      (3) The authors state interest in learning more about the differences underlying translational regulation of projection neuron development. This method only captures neuronal somata, which will only capture ribosomes in the main cell body. There are also ribosomes regulating local translation in the axons, which may also play a critical role in axonal circuit establishment and activity. These ribosomal complex interactions may also be rather transient and difficult to capture at only one developmental stage. Therefore, this method is currently limited to a single developmental snapshot of ribosomal complexes at P3 within the main cell body. It would be exciting to see the extended utility of this method to sample neurites and additional developmental stages to gain further resolution on the developmental translation regulation of these projection neurons.

      Likely impact of the work on the field, and the utility of the methods and data to the community:

      The authors introduce a unique pipeline of techniques to identify cell-type-specific ribosomal complex compositions. With more validation, there is certainly potential for those studying neuronal translation to leverage this method in limited primary cells as an alternative to existing methods that do not rely on ribosomal protein tagging, such as ARC-MS (Bartsch et al., 2023), RAPIDASH (Susanto and Hung et al., 2024), and RAPPL (Nature Communications, 2025).

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study presents a sophisticated molecular dissection of ribosome-associated complexes (RCs) in two well-defined cortical projection neuron subtypes (ScPN and CPN) during early postnatal development. The authors develop and optimize an rRNA immunoprecipitation-mass spectrometry (rRNA IP-MS) workflow to recover RCs from FACS-purified, retrogradely labeled neurons, achieving remarkable subtype specificity and biochemical resolution. Through proteomic profiling, they reveal both shared and distinct ribosome-associated proteins between ScPN and CPN, with a focus on non-core RC components and their potential functional relevance. The work advances our understanding of cell-type-specific translation regulation, moving beyond the transcriptome to explore the proteome-level complexity in neuronal subtypes.

      Strengths:

      This work stands out for its technical sophistication and innovation. The authors combine retrograde labeling, FACS purification, and an optimized rRNA IP-MS approach (low input) to isolate ribosome-associated complexes from highly specific neuronal subtypes in vivo, a challenging issue that they execute with impressive rigor. The methodological pipeline is both elegant and well-controlled, yielding high-quality, reproducible data. The depth of proteomic coverage is remarkable, with nearly all known cytoplasmic ribosomal proteins identified, along with hundreds of ribosome-associated proteins (RAPs), including translation factors, chaperones, and RNA-binding proteins. The analysis not only reveals shared components between ScPN and CPN RCs but also uncovers subtype-specific differences in associated proteins.

      Particularly notable is the integration of this new proteomic dataset with previously published transcriptomic and ribosome footprinting data, which helps to validate the specificity and relevance of the findings. Overall, the clarity of the writing, the robustness of the data, and the transparency of the methods make this a strong and compelling contribution.

      Weaknesses:

      Despite the depth and high quality of the dataset, the study remains descriptive. While the identification of subtype-specific RC components is intriguing, the current version of the manuscript does not explore their functional roles or the biological consequences of their alterations. There is no perturbation, causal testing, in vitro or in vivo manipulation to demonstrate whether these proteins are necessary for ScPN or CPN identity, specific axonal targeting, metabolism, or synaptic function.

      One important point highlighted by the authors in the discussion - and critical for establishing the subtype specificity of the identified proteins - is that some ribosomal complexes may be specialized for specific developmental stages, rather than exclusively for the subtype-specific needs of projection neuron development. The work presented here provides a valuable starting point for further investigation into such RC specialization. However, it will be essential to determine to what extent these RCs exhibit true subtype specificity, independently of their temporal maturation context.

      As a result, key mechanistic insights remain a bit speculative. Although several of the identified proteins have known roles in processes like synaptogenesis or metabolism, their relevance to the specific neuronal subtypes under study is not experimentally addressed. That said, given its rich content and the comprehensive early postnatal dataset, the manuscript represents an extremely valuable resource for the community. While primarily exploratory, it lays a strong foundation for future functional studies aimed at uncovering the biological impact of the identified ribosomal complexes.

  3. Sep 2025
    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Mazer & Yovel 2025 dissect the inverse problem of how echolocators in groups manage to navigate their surroundings despite intense jamming using computational simulations.

      The authors show that despite the 'noisy' sensory environments that echolocating groups present, agents can still access some amount of echo-related information and use it to navigate their local environment. It is known that echolocating bats have strong small and large-scale spatial memory that plays an important role for individuals. The results from this paper also point to the potential importance of an even lower-level, short-term role of memory in the form of echo 'integration' across multiple calls, despite the unpredictability of echo detection in groups. The paper generates a useful basis to think about the mechanisms in echolocating groups for experimental investigations too.

      Strengths:

      * The paper builds on biologically well-motivated and parametrised 2D acoustics and sensory simulation setup to investigate the various key parameters of interest

      * The 'null-model' of echolocators not being able to tell apart objects & conspecifics while echolocating still shows agents succesfully emerge from groups - even though the probability of emergence drops severely in comparison to cognitively more 'capable' agents. This is nonetheless an important result showing the direction-of-arrival of a sound itself is the 'minimum' set of ingredients needed for echolocators navigating their environment.

      * The results generate an important basis in unraveling how agents may navigate in sensorially noisy environments with a lot of irrelevant and very few relevant cues.

      * The 2D simulation framework is simple and computationally tractable enough to perform multiple runs to investigate many variables - while also remaining true to the aim of the investigation.

      Weaknesses:

      * Authors have not yet provided convincing justification for the use of different echolocation phases during emergence and in cave behaviour. In the previous modelling paper cited for the details - here the bat-agents are performing a foraging task, and so the switch in echolocation phases is understandable. While flying with conspecifics, the lab's previous paper has shown what they call a 'clutter response' - but this is not necessarily the same as going into a 'buzz'-type call behaviour. As pointed out by another reviewer - the results of the simulations may hinge on the fact that bats are showing this echolocation phase-switching, and thus improving their echo-detection. This is not necessarily a major flaw - but something for readers to consider in light of the sparse experimental evidence at hand currently.

      * The decision to model direction-of-arrival with such high angular resolution (1-2 degrees) is not entirely justifiable - and the authors may wish to do simulation runs with lower angular resolution. Past experimental paradigms haven't really separated out target-strength as a confounding factor for angular resolution (e.g. see the cited Simmons et al. 1983 paper). Moreover, to this reviewer's reading of the cited paper - it is not entirely clear how this experiment provides source-data to support the DoA-SNR parametrisation in this manuscript. The cited paper has two array-configurations, both of which are measured to have similar received levels upon ensonification. A relationship between angular resolution and signal-to-noise ratio is understandable perhaps - and one can formulate such a relationship, but here the reviewer asks that the origin/justification be made clear. On an independent line, also see the recent contrasting results of Geberl, Kugler, Wiegrebe 2019 (Curr. Biol.) - who suggest even poorer angular resolution in echolocation.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      This manuscript describes a detailed model for bats flying together through a fixed geometry. The model considers elements which are faithful to both bat biosonar production and reception and the acoustics governing how sound moves in air and interacts with obstacles. The model also incorporates behavioral patterns observed in bats, like one-dimensional feature following and temporal integration of cognitive maps. From a simulation study of the model and comparison of the results with the literature, the authors gain insight into how often bats may experience destructive interference of their acoustic signals and those of their peers, and how much such interference may actually negatively effect the groups' ability to navigate effectively. The authors use generalized linear models to test the significance of the effects they observe.

      The work relies on a thoughtful and detailed model which faithfully incorporates salient features, such as acoustic elements like the filter for a biological receiver and temporal aggregation as a kind of memory in the system. At the same time, the authors abstract features that are complicating without being expected to give additional insights, as can be seen in the choice of a two-dimensional rather than three-dimensional system. I thought that the level of abstraction in the model was perfect, enough to demonstrate their results without needless details. The results are compelling and interesting, and the authors do a great job discussing them in the context of the biological literature.

      With respect to the first version of the manuscript, the authors have remedied all my outstanding questions or concerns in the current version. The new supplementary figure 5 is especially helpful in understanding the geometry.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors provide a detailed ultrastructural analysis of the larval pharyngeal sensory organs, including the dorsal pharyngeal sensilla, dorsal pharyngeal organ, ventral pharyngeal sensilla, and posterior pharyngeal sensilla. Using electron microscopy and 3D reconstruction, Richter et al., present a comprehensive mapping and classification of pharyngeal sensory structures, defining mthe orphological type of pharyngeal sensilla based on ultrastructure and generating a neuron-to-sensillum map. These findings significantly advance our understanding of internal larval sensory systems and establish a robust framework for future functional studies in coordination with external sensory systems.

      Strengths:

      The application of high-resolution electron microscopy and 3D imaging analysis successfully overcomes technical challenges associated with visualizing deep internal structures. This enables an unprecedented level of anatomical detail of the larval pharyngeal sensory system. Thus, the study complements and completes existing maps of larval sensory circuits, contributing a comprehensive neuroanatomical characterization of larval sensory input pathways. These insights will inform future studies on larval behavior, sensory processing, and may also have applied relevance for insect control strategies.

      Weaknesses:

      While the manuscript is concise, clearly written, and methodologically rigorous, it primarily addresses a specialized readership with expertise in insect neuroanatomy.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript documents the structure of the pharyngeal nervous system of the Drosophila larva. The authors wanted to achieve a detailed ultrastructural reconstruction of the gustatory sensory organs in the Drosophila pharynx. Using serial EM and the associated bioinformatics tools, they have achieved their goal. The paper is written clearly and illustrated beautifully with 3D models and annotated sections. The data will significantly enrich the field of Drosophila neurobiology.

      Strengths:

      Given the dataset, the findings presented are solid and will be an important work of reference for the future.

      Weaknesses:

      Previous work, including EM, on the pharyngeal sensory organ is not sufficiently referenced and used for comparison with the data presented in this study.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This work provides important new evidence of the cognitive and neural mechanisms that give rise to feelings of shame and guilt, as well as their transformation into compensatory behavior. The authors use a well-designed interpersonal task to manipulate responsibility and harm, eliciting varying levels of shame and guilt in participants. The study combines behavioral, computational, and neuroimaging approaches to offer a comprehensive account of how these emotions are experienced and acted upon. Notably, the findings reveal distinct patterns in how harm and responsibility contribute to guilt and shame and how these factors are integrated into compensatory decision-making.

      Strengths:

      (1) Investigating both guilt and shame in a single experimental framework allows for a direct comparison of their behavioral and neural effects while minimizing confounds.

      (2) The study provides a novel contribution to the literature by exploring the neural bases underlying the conversion of shame into behavior.

      (3) The task is creative and ecologically valid, simulating a realistic social situation while retaining experimental control.

      (4) Computational modeling and fMRI analysis yield converging evidence for a quotient-based integration of harm and responsibility in guiding compensatory behavior.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Post-experimental self-reports rely both on memory and on the understanding of the conceptual difference between the two emotions. Additionally, it is unclear whether the 16 scenarios were presented in random order; sequential presentation could have introduced contrast effects or demand characteristics.

      (2) In the neural analysis of emotion sensitivity, the authors identify brain regions correlated with responsibility-driven shame sensitivity and then use those brain regions as masks to test whether they were more involved in the responsibility-driven shame sensitivity than the other types of emotion sensitivity. I wonder if this is biasing the results. Would it be better to use a cross-validation approach? A similar issue might arise in "Activation analysis (neural basis of compensatory sensitivity)."

      Additional comments and questions:

      (1) Regarding the traits of guilt and shame, I appreciate using the scores from the subscales (evaluations and action tendencies) separately for the analyses (instead of a composite score). An issue with using the actions subscales when measuring guilt and shame proneness is that the behavioral tendencies for each emotion get conflated with their definitions, risking circularity. It is reassuring that the behavior evaluation subscale was significantly correlated with compensatory behavior (not only the action tendencies subscale). However, the absence of significant neural correlates for the behavior evaluation subscale raises questions: Do the authors have thoughts on why this might be the case, and any implications?

      (2) Regarding the computational model finding that participants seem to disregard self-interest, do the authors believe it may reflect the relatively small endowment at stake? Do the authors believe this behavior would persist if the stakes were higher? Additionally, might the type of harm inflicted (e.g., electric shock vs. less stigmatized/less ethically charged harm like placing a hand in ice-cold water) influence the weight of self-interest in decision-making?

      Taken together, the conclusions of the paper are well supported by the data. It would be valuable for future studies to validate these findings using alternative tasks or paradigms to ensure the robustness and generalizability of the observed behavioral and neural mechanisms.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors combined behavioral experiments, computational modeling, and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate the psychological and neural mechanisms underlying guilt, shame, and the altruistic behaviors driven by these emotions. The results revealed that guilt is more strongly associated with harm, whereas shame is more closely linked to responsibility. Compared to shame, guilt elicited a higher level of altruistic behavior. Computational modeling demonstrated how individuals integrate information about harm and responsibility. The fMRI findings identified a set of brain regions involved in representing harm and responsibility, transforming responsibility into feelings of shame, converting guilt and shame into altruistic actions, and mediating the effect of trait guilt on compensatory behavior.

      Strengths:

      This study offers a significant contribution to the literature on social emotions by moving beyond prior research that typically focused on isolated aspects of guilt and shame. The study presents a comprehensive examination of these emotions, encompassing their cognitive antecedents, affective experiences, behavioral consequences, trait-level characteristics, and neural correlates. The authors have introduced a novel experimental task that enables such a systematic investigation and holds strong potential for future research applications. The computational modeling procedures were implemented in accordance with current field standards. The findings are rich and offer meaningful theoretical insights. The manuscript is well written, and the results are clearly and logically presented.

      Weaknesses:

      In this study, participants' feelings of guilt and shame were assessed retrospectively, after they had completed all altruistic decision-making tasks. This reliance on memory-based self-reports may introduce recall bias, potentially compromising the accuracy of the emotion measurements.

      In many behavioral economic models, self-interest plays a central role in shaping individual decision-making, including moral decisions. However, the model comparison results in this study suggest that models without a self-interest component (such as Model 1.3) outperform those that incorporate it (such as Model 1.1 and Model 1.2). The authors have not provided a satisfactory explanation for this counterintuitive finding.

      The phrases "individuals integrate harm and responsibility in the form of a quotient" and "harm and responsibility are integrated in the form of a quotient" appear in the Abstract and Discussion sections. However, based on the results of the computational modeling, it is more accurate to state that "harm and the number of wrongdoers are integrated in the form of a quotient." The current phrasing misleadingly suggests that participants represent information as harm divided by responsibility, which does not align with the modeling results. This potentially confusing expression should be revised for clarity and accuracy.

      In the Discussion, the authors state: "Since no brain region associated with social cognition showed significant responses to harm or responsibility, it appears that the human brain encodes a unified measure integrating harm and responsibility (i.e., the quotient) rather than processing them as separate entities when both are relevant to subsequent emotional experience and decision-making." However, this interpretation overstates the implications of the null fMRI findings. The absence of significant activation in response to harm or responsibility does not necessarily imply that the brain does not represent these dimensions separately. Null results can arise from various factors, including limitations in the sensitivity of fMRI. It is possible that more fine-grained techniques, such as intracranial electrophysiological recordings, could reveal distinct neural representations of harm and responsibility. The interpretation of these null findings should be made with greater caution.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Zhu et al. set out to elucidate how the moral emotions of guilt and shame emerge from specific cognitive antecedents - harm and responsibility - and how these emotions subsequently drive compensatory behavior. Consistent with their prediction derived from functionalist theories of emotion, their behavioral findings indicate that guilt is more influenced by harm, whereas shame is more influenced by responsibility. In line with previous research, their results also demonstrate that guilt has a stronger facilitating effect on compensatory behavior than shame. Furthermore, computational modeling and neuroimaging results suggest that individuals integrate harm and responsibility information into a composite representation of the individual's share of the harm caused. Brain areas such as the striatum, insula, temporoparietal junction, lateral prefrontal cortex, and cingulate cortex were implicated in distinct stages of the processing of guilt and/or shame. In general, this work makes an important contribution to the field of moral emotions. Its impact could be further enhanced by clarifying methodological details, offering a more nuanced interpretation of the findings, and discussing their potential practical implications in greater depth.

      Strengths:

      First, this work conceptualizes guilt and shame as processes unfolding across distinct stages (cognitive appraisal, emotional experience, and behavioral response) and investigates the psychological and neural characteristics associated with their transitions from one stage to the next.

      Second, the well-designed experiment effectively manipulates harm and responsibility - two critical antecedents of guilt and shame.

      Third, the findings deepen our understanding of the mechanisms underlying guilt and shame beyond what has been established in previous research.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Over the course of the task, participants may gradually become aware of their high error rate in the dot estimation task. This could lead them to discount their own judgments and become inclined to rely on the choices of other deciders. It is unclear whether participants in the experiment had the opportunity to observe or inquire about others' choices. This point is important, as the compensatory decision-making process may differ depending on whether choices are made independently or influenced by external input.

      (2) Given the inherent complexity of human decision-making, it is crucial to acknowledge that, although the authors compared eight candidate models, other plausible alternatives may exist. As such, caution is warranted when interpreting the computational modeling results.

      (3) I do not agree with the authors' claim that "computational modeling results indicated that individuals integrate harm and responsibility in the form of a quotient" (i.e., harm/responsibility). Rather, the findings appear to suggest that individuals may form a composite representation of the harm attributable to each individual (i.e., harm/the number of people involved). The explanation of the modeling results ought to be precise.

      (4) Many studies have reported positive associations between trait gratitude, social value orientation, and altruistic behavior. It would be helpful if the authors could provide an explanation about why this study failed to replicate these associations.

      (5) As the authors noted, guilt and shame are closely linked to various psychiatric disorders. It would be valuable to discuss whether this study has any implications for understanding or even informing the treatment of these disorders.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary

      The authors previously published a study of RGC boutons in the dLGN in developing wild-type mice and developing mutant mice with disrupted spontaneous activity. In the current manuscript, they have broken down their analysis of RGC boutons according to the number of Homer/Bassoon puncta associated with each vGlut3 cluster.

      The authors find that, in the first post-natal week, RGC boutons with multiple active zones (mAZs) are about a third as common as boutons with a single active zone (sAZ). The size of the vGluT2 cluster associated with each bouton was proportional to the number of active zones present in each bouton. Within the author's ability to estimate these values (n=3 per group, 95% of results expected to be within ~2.5 standard deviations), these results are consistent across groups: 1) dominant eye vs. non-dominant eye, 2) wild-type mice vs. mice with activity blocked, and at 3) ages P2, P4, and P8. The authors also found that mAZs and sAZs also have roughly the same number (about 1.5) of sAZs clustered around them (within 1.5 um).

      However, the authors do not interpret this consistency between groups as evidence that active zone clustering is not a specific marker or driver of activity dependent synaptic segregation. Rather, the authors perform a large number of tests for statistical significance and cite the presence or absence of statistical significance as evidence that "Eye-specific active zone clustering underlies synaptic competition in the developing visual system (title)". I don't believe this conclusion is supported by the evidence.

      Strengths

      The source dataset is high resolution data showing the colocalization of multiple synaptic proteins across development. Added to this data is labeling that distinguishes axons from the right eye from axons from the left eye. The first order analysis of this data showing changes in synapse density and in the occurrence of multi-active zone synapses is useful information about the development of an important model for activity dependent synaptic remodeling.

      Weaknesses

      In my previous review I argued that it was not possible to determine, from their analysis, whether the differences they were reporting between groups was important to the biology of the system. The authors have made some changes to their statistics (paired t-tests) and use some less derived measures of clustering. However, they still fail to present a meaningfully quantitative argument that the observed group differences are important. The authors base most of their claims on small differences between groups. There are two big problems with this practice. First, the differences between groups appear too small to be biologically important. Second, the differences between groups that are used as evidence for how the biology works are generally smaller than the precision of the author's sampling. That is, the differences are as likely to be false positives as true positives.

      (1) Effect size. The title claims: "Eye-specific active zone clustering underlies synaptic competition in the developing visual system". Such a claim might be supported if the authors found that mAZs are only found in dominant-eye RGCs and that eye-specific segregation doesn't begin until some threshold of mAZ frequency is reached. Instead, the behavior of mAZs is roughly the same across all conditions. For example, the clear trend in Figure 4C and D is that measures of clustering between mAZ and sAZ are as similar as could reasonably be expected by the experimental design. However, some of the comparisons of very similar values produced p-values < 0.05. The authors use this fact to argue that the negligible differences between mAZ and sAZs explain the development of the dramatic differences in the distribution of ipsilateral and contralateral RGCs.

      (2) Sample size. Performing a large number of significance tests and comparing p-values is not hypothesis testing and is not descriptive science. At best, with large sample sizes and controls for multiple tests, this approach could be considered exploratory. With n=3 for each group, many comparisons of many derived measures, among many groups, and no control for multiple testing, this approach constitutes a random result generator.

      The authors argue that n=3 is a large sample size for the type of high resolution / large volume data being used. It is true that many electron microscopy studies with n=1 are used to reveal the patterns of organization that are possible within an individual. However, such studies cannot control individual variation and are, therefore, not appropriate for identifying subtle differences between groups.<br /> In response to previous critiques along these lines, the authors argue they have dealt with this issue by limiting their analysis to within-individual paired comparisons. There are several problems with their thinking in this approach. The main problem is that they did not change the logic of their arguments, only which direction they pointed the t-tests. Instead of claiming that two groups are different because p < 0.05, they say that two groups are different because one produced p < 0.05 and the other produced p > 0.05. These arguments are not statistically valid or biologically meaningful.

      To the best of my understanding, the results are consistent with the following model:

      • RGCs form mAZs at large boutons (known)

      • About a quarter of week-one RGC boutons are mAZs (new observation)

      • Vesicle clustering is proportional to active zone number (~new observation)

      • RGC synapse density increases during the first post-week (known)

      • Blocking activity reduces synapse density (known)

      • Contralateral eye RGCs for more and larger synapses in the lateral dLGN (known)

      • With n=3 and effect sizes smaller than 1 standard deviation, a statistically significant result is about as likely to be a false positive as a true positive.

      • A true-positive statistically significant result does is not evidence of a meaningful deviation from a biological model.

      Providing plots that show the number of active zones present in boutons across these various conditions is useful. However, I could find no compelling deviation from the above default predictions that would influence how I see the role of mAZs in activity dependent eye-specific segregation.

      Below are critiques of most of the claims of the manuscript.

      Claim (abstract): individual retinogeniculate boutons begin forming multiple nearby presynaptic active zones during the first postnatal week.

      Confirmed by data.

      Claim (abstract): the dominant-eye forms more numerous mAZ contacts,

      Misleading: The dominant-eye (by definition) forms more contacts than the non-dominant eye. That includes mAZ.

      Claim (abstract): At the height of competition, the non-dominant-eye projection adds many single active zone (sAZ) synapses

      Weak: While the individual observation is strong, it is a surprising deviation based on a single n=3 experiment in a study that performed twelve such experiments (six ages, mutant/wildtype, sAZ/mAZ)

      Claim (abstract): Together, these findings reveal eye-specific differences in release site addition during synaptic competition in circuits essential for visual perception and behavior.

      False: This claim is unambiguously false. The above findings, even if true, do not argue for any functional significance to active zone clustering.

      Claim (line 84): "At the peak of synaptic competition midway through the first postnatal week, the non-dominant-eye formed numerous sAZ inputs, equalizing the global synapse density between the two eyes"

      Weak: At one of twelve measures (age, bouton type, genotype) performed with 3 mice each, one density measure was about twice as high as expected.

      Claim (line 172): "In WT mice, both mAZ (Fig. 3A, left) and sAZ (Fig. 3B, left) inputs showed significant eye-specific volume differences at each age."

      Questionable: There appears to be a trend, but the size and consistency is unclear.

      Claim (line 175): "the median VGluT2 cluster volume in dominant-eye mAZ inputs was 3.72 fold larger than that of non-dominant-eye inputs (Fig. 3A, left)."

      Cherry picking. Twelve differences were measured with an n of 3, 3 each time. The biggest difference of the group was cited. No analysis is provided for the range of uncertainty about this measure (2.5 standard deviations) as an individual sample or as one of twelve comparisons.

      Claim (line 174): "In the middle of eye-specific competition at P4 in WT mice, the median VGluT2 cluster volume in dominant-eye mAZ inputs was 3.72 fold larger than that of non-dominant-eye inputs (Fig. 3A, left). In contrast, β2KO mice showed a smaller 1.1 fold difference at the same age (Fig. 3A, right panel). For sAZ synapses at P4, the magnitudes of eye-specific differences in VGluT2 volume were smaller: 1.35-fold in WT (Fig. 3B, left) and 0.41-fold in β2KO mice (Fig. 3B, right). Thus, both mAZ and sAZ input size favors the dominant eye, with larger eye-specific differences seen in WT mice (see Table S3)."

      No way to judge the reliability of the analysis and trivial conclusion: To analyze effect size the authors choose the median value of three measures (whatever the middle value is). They then make four comparisons at the time point where they observed the biggest difference in favor of their hypothesis. There is no way to determine how much we should trust these numbers besides spending time with the mislabeled scatter plots. The authors then claim that this analysis provides evidence that there is a difference in vGluT2 cluster volume between dominant and non-dominant RGCs and that that difference is activity dependent. The conclusion that dominant axons have bigger boutons and that mutants that lack the property that would drive segregation would show less of a difference is very consistent with the literature. Moreover, there is no context provided about what 1.35 or 1.1 fold difference means for the biology of the system.

      Claim (189): "This shows that vesicle docking at release sites favors the dominant-eye as we previously reported but is similar for like eye type inputs regardless of AZ number."

      Contradicts core claim of manuscript: Consistent with previous literature, there is an activity dependent relative increase in vGlut2 clustering of dominant eye RGCs. The new information is that that activity dependence is more or less the same in sAZ and mAZ. The only plausible alternative is that vGlut2 scaling only increases in mAZ which would be consistent with the claims of their paper. That is not what they found. To the extent that the analysis presented in this manuscript tests a hypothesis, this is it. The claim of the title has been refuted by figure 3.

      Claim (line 235): "For the non-dominant eye projection, however, clustered mAZ inputs outnumbered clustered sAZ inputs at P4 (Fig. 4C, bottom left panel), the age when this eye adds sAZ synapses (Fig. 2C)."

      Misleading: The overwhelming trend across 24 comparisons is that the sAZ clustering looks like mAZ clustering. That is the objective and unambiguous result. Among these 24 underpowered tests (n=3), there were a few p-values < 0.05. The authors base their interpretation of cell behavior on these crossings.

      Claim (line 328): "The failure to add synapses reduced synaptic clustering and more inputs formed in isolation in the mutants compared to controls."

      Trivially true: Density was lower in mutant.

      Claim (line 332): "While our findings support a role for spontaneous retinal activity in presynaptic release site addition and clustering..."

      Not meaningfully supported by evidence: I could not find meaningful differences between WT and mutant beside the already known dramatic difference in synapse density.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Zhang and Speer examine changes in the spatial organization of synaptic proteins during eye specific segregation, a developmental period when axons from the two eyes initially mingle and gradually segregate into eye-specific regions of the dorsal lateral geniculate. The authors use STORM microscopy and immunostain presynaptic (VGluT2, Bassoon) and postsynaptic (Homer) proteins to identify synaptic release sites. Activity-dependent changes of this spatial organization are identified by comparing the β2KO mice to WT mice. They describe two types of synapses based on Bassoon clustering: the multiple active zone (mAZ) synapse and single active zone (sAZ) synapse. In this revision, the authors have added EM data to support the idea that mAZ synapses represent boutons with multiple release sites. They have also reanalyzed their data set with different statistical approaches.

      Strengths:

      The data presented is of good quality and provides an unprecedented view at high resolution of the presynaptic components of the retinogeniculate synapse during active developmental remodeling. This approach offers an advance to the previous mouse EM studies of this synapse because of the CTB label allows identification of the eye from which the presynaptic terminal arises.

      Weaknesses:

      While the interpretation of this data set is much more grounded in this second revised submission, some of the authors' conclusions/statements still lack convincing supporting evidence. In particular, the data does not support the title: "Eye-specific active zone clustering underlies synaptic competition in the developing visual system". The data show that there are fewer synapses made for both contra- and ipsi- inputs in the β2KO mice-- this fact alone can account for the differences in clustering. There is no evidence linking clustering to synaptic competition. Moreover, the findings of differences in AZ# or distance between AZs that the authors report are quite small and it is not clear whether they are functionally meaningful.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      This study is a follow-up to a recent study of synaptic development based on a powerful data set that combines anterograde labeling, immunofluorescence labeling of synaptic proteins, and STORM imaging (Cell Reports, 2023). Specifically, they use anti-Vglut2 label to determine the size of the presynaptic structure (which they describe as the vesicle pool size), anti-Bassoon to label active zones with the resolution to count them, and anti-Homer to identify postsynaptic densities. Their previous study compared the detailed synaptic structure across the development of synapses made with contra-projecting vs. ipsi-projecting RGCs and compared this developmental profile with a mouse model with reduced retinal waves. In this study, they produce a new detailed analysis on the same data set in which they classify synapses into "multi-active zone" vs. "single-active zone" synapses and assess the number and spacing of these synapses. The authors use measurements to make conclusions about the role of retinal waves in the generation of same-eye synaptic clusters. The authors interpret these results as providing insight into how neural activity drives synapse maturation, the strength of their conclusions is not directly tested by their analysis.

      Strengths:

      This is a fantastic data set for describing the structural details of synapse development in a part of the brain undergoing activity-dependent synaptic rearrangements. The fact that they can differentiate the eye of origin is what makes this data set unique over previous structural work. The addition of example images from the EM dataset provides confidence in their categorization scheme.

      Weaknesses:

      Though the descriptions of single vs multi-active zone synapses are important and represent a significant advance, the authors continue to make unsupported conclusions regarding the biological processes driving these changes. Although this revision includes additional information about the populations tested and the tests conducted, the authors do not address the issue raised by previous reviews. Specifically, they provide no assessment of what effect size represents a biologically meaningful result. For example, a more appropriate title is "The distribution of eye-specific single vs multi-active zone is altered in mice with reduced spontaneous activity" rather than concluding that this difference in clustering is somehow related to synaptic competition. Of course, the authors are free to speculate, but many of the conclusions of the paper are not supported by their results.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This computational study investigates the physical mechanisms underlying enhancer-promoter (E-P) interactions across genomic distances in Drosophila chromosomes, motivated by a previously published study that revealed unexpectedly frequent long-range contacts challenging classical polymer models. The authors performed coarse-grained polymer simulations testing three chromatin organization models: ideal polymers, loop extrusion, and compartmental segregation, comparing their predictions to experimental Hi-C contact maps, mean E-P distances, and two-locus mean-squared displacement dynamics. They found that compartmental segregation best captured both the structural and dynamic features observed experimentally, while neither ideal chains nor loop extrusion alone could reproduce all experimental observables. The combination of compartmental segregation with loop extrusion further improved agreement with experimental data, suggesting these mechanisms might be involved in Drosophila chromatin organization.

      Strengths:

      The paper has two primary strengths:

      (1) The simulations are based on biologically interpretable mechanisms (compartmentalization and loop extrusion), which may facilitate making specific experimentally testable predictions.

      (2) The work uses a systematic approach to increase model complexity by directly fitting to data, first establishing that simple models fail to capture the data until arriving at a more complex model that does capture the data.

      Weaknesses:

      I have two major concerns (detailed below) and multiple minor concerns.

      Major concerns:

      (1) While the upside of the mechanistic simulations is that they are interpretable, the downside is that specific choices for the considered mechanism were made, and conclusions drawn from it are necessarily biased by the initial choices. In this paper, only two mechanisms were considered: loop extrusion and compartmentalization. Yet, it is not clear why these are the most likely underlying mechanisms that might determine the chromosome dynamics. Indeed, previous work (not cited in this paper) showed that Drosophila chromosome structure is not determined by loop extrusion: https://elifesciences.org/articles/94070.

      This should be acknowledged, and the main reasons for choosing these particular mechanisms should be laid out. The conclusions of the paper must then necessarily always be seen under the caveat that only these two mechanisms were considered.

      (2) Even within the framework of the approach, insufficient evidence is given to support the title of the paper "Criticality-driven enhancer-promoter dynamics in Drosophila chromosomes" for two reasons:

      (a) The fact that the best-fit parameters are near a coil-globule transition does not mean that the resulting dynamics are criticality-driven. To claim criticality, one would usually expect much more direct evidence, such as diverging correlation lengths. Furthermore, it would need to be shown that the key features of the dynamics (which should be defined, presumably the static and dynamic exponents) indeed depend on the parameters being at this transition. i.e., when tuning the simulations away from this parameter point, does the behaviour disappear? Only in this case can it be claimed that the behaviour is driven by this phenomenon.

      (b) The results section actually contains no mention of the coil-globule transition, and it is not clear in what way the parameters are close to this transition.

      Thus, three things are necessary:

      (i) How the parameters are close to the transition needs to be explained in detail.

      (ii) The divergence of observed dynamics whenever the parameters are tuned away from the transition needs to be demonstrated.

      (iii) Even if 1 and 2 are fulfilled, a more careful title should be chosen, such as "Polymer simulations near the coil-globule transition are consistent with enhancer-promoter dynamics in Drosophila chromosomes."

      Many of the results in the figures and results section are rather repetitive and could be compressed. The main result of Figure 1 - that the data are not described by an ideal chain - was already fully shown and established in the original paper from which the data are taken. Figure 2 is a negative result with near-identical panels to Figure 3. Figure 4B is hard to interpret.

      The paper makes no concrete suggestions for new experiments to test the hypotheses formulated. Since the paper can only claim that the simulations are consistent with the data, it would significantly strengthen the paper if testable predictions could be made.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this work, Ganesh and colleagues use experimental data from Hi-C and from live-cell imaging to evaluate different polymer models of 3D genome organization in Drosophila based on both structural and dynamic properties. The authors consider several leading hypotheses, which are examined sequentially in increasing level of complexity - from the minimal Rouse polymer, to a model combining sequence-specific compartmentalization and loop-extrusion without extrusion blockers. They conclude that the combination of both compartmentalization and loop-extrusion gives the best agreement with the data. Their analysis also leads to concrete predictions about the processivity of cohesin loop extrusion in Drosophila, and a conclusion that the compartmental interaction strength is poised near criticality in the coil-globule phase space.

      Strengths:

      There is considerable interest in the field in understanding the mechanisms responsible for the 3D spatial organization genome and the dynamic movement of the genome, which has major implications for our understanding of long-range transcriptional regulation and other genome behaviors. The live-cell experimental work on which this study draws highlights the limitations of existing models to explain even the dynamic behaviors observed in the data, further exciting interest in further exploration. Therefore, this paper seeks to address an important gap in the field. The work is written in a well-organized, well-illustrated fashion. The text and figures are nicely integrated, easy to read, and explain challenging concepts with elegance and brevity in a manner that will be accessible to a broad audience.

      Weaknesses:

      The validity and utility of these conclusions are, in my view, substantially undermined by what appears to be unappreciated peculiarities of the live-cell data set that was used to constrain the model. The live-cell data comes from embryos were edited in a way that intentionally substantively changed both the 3D genome structure and dynamics specifically at the loci which are imaged, a case which is not at all explained by any of the models suggested nor acknowledged in the current work, nor compatible with the Hi-C data that simultaneously used to explain these models. As these ignored synthetic alterations have been previously shown to be determinative of transcriptional activity, the relevance of the author's work to transcriptional control (a prime motivation in the introduction) is unclear.

      The agreement in 3D organization, as represented in chromosome-scale contact frequency heatmaps, is substantially less impressive than the agreement seen in prior work with similar models. This discrepancy appears to be due in part to the unappreciated effects of the mentioned in the previous limitation, as well as inappropriate choices in metrics used to evaluate agreement. It is also not particularly surprising that combining more models, with more free parameters, results in an improvement in the quality of fit.

      Some major results, including both theoretical works and experimental ones, are ignored, despite their relevance to the stated objective of the work. The current manuscript and analysis could be improved substantially by a consideration of these works.

      I describe these issues in more detail below.

      Major issues:

      (1) The genetic element "homie" is present in a subset of the data: The experimental data used in this analysis come from different fly lines, half of which have been edited explicitly to alter genome structure and consequent transcriptional behavior, yet the authors are trying to fit with a common model - a problem which substantially undermines the utility of the analysis.

      Specifically, the authors evaluate the various models/simulations by comparing them to Hi-C from wildtype Drosophila embryos on the chromosome scale and 3D distances and dynamics from live cell imaging in genetically edited embryos, to a series of models in turn. The exercise fatally overlooks a critical fact, (admittedly not easily noticed in the work from Bruckner et al), that the fly embryos used for nearly all their analyses contain not only fluorescent labels, but also contain two copies of a powerful genetic sequence, "homie", known for its ability to dramatically change the 3D organization and dynamics of the genome. Whether or not the fluorescent labels themselves used in the study further alter structure and dynamics is not entirely clear (and will require further work beyond the scope of either study), but at least these fluorescent labels aren't known to dramatically affect 3D structure and dynamics the way homie is. The critical problem is that adding or removing the "homie", as shown in a collection of prior works I describe below in more detail, dramatically affects structure, dynamics, and gene expression. Whether or not the genome contains two distal cis-linked copies of homie fundamentally changes genome structure and dynamics, so to use one dataset which has this edit (the live-cell data) and one dataset which lacks it (the Hi-C data) is, in some sense, to guarantee failure of any model to match all the data.

      If the authors had chosen instead to focus exclusively on the 'no homie' genetic lines in the Brukner data, they would have a much smaller dataset (just 2 distances), which would not cover all the length scales of interest, but it would at least be a dataset not known to be contradictory to the Hi-C. The two 'no homie' lines make much more plausible candidates for the sort of generalizable polymer dynamics these authors seek to explain, as will hopefully be made more clear by a brief review of what is known about homie. I next describe the published data that support these conclusions about how homie affects 3D genome spatial organization and dynamics:

      What is "homie" and how does it affect 3D genome distances, dynamics, and gene expression?

      The genetic element "homie" was named by James Jaynes' lab ( Fujioka...Jaynes 2009) in reference to its remarkable "homing" ability - a fascinating and still poorly understood biological observation that some genetic sequences from Drosophila, when cloned on plasmids and reintegrated into the genome with p-elements, had a remarkable propensity to re-integrate near their endogenous sequence, (Hama et al., 1990; Kassis, 2002; Taillebourg and Dura, 1999; Bender and Hudson, 2000; Fujioka...Jaynes 2009). By contrast, most genetic elements tend to incorporate at random across the genome in such assays (with some bias for active chromatin).

      The Jaynes lab subsequently showed that flies carrying two copies of homie, one integrated in cis, ~140 kb distal from the endogenous element, formed preferential cis contacts with one another. Indeed, if a promoter and reporter gene were included at this distal integration site, the reporter gene would activate gene expression in the pattern normally seen by the gene, even-skipped. The endogenous copy of homie marks one border of ~16 kb mini-TAD which contains the even-skipped gene, (eve), and its developmental enhancers, so this functional interaction provides further evidence of physical proximity (as was also shown by 3C by Jaynes (Fujioka..., Schedl, Jaynes 2016), and later with elegant live imaging, by Jaynes and Gregor (Chen 2018)).

      Critically, if either copy of homie is deleted or substantially mutated, the 3D proximity is lost (Fujioka 2016, Chen 2018, Bruckner 2023), and the expression of the transgene is dramatically reduced (at 58 kb) or lost. Given the author's motivation of understanding "E-P" interactions, the fact that the increased 3D proximity provided by homie is as essential for transcription as the promoter itself at the ~150 kb distance, underscores that these are not negligible changes.

      These effects can be seen by plotting the data from Bruckner 2023, which includes data from labels with separations of 58 kb and ~150 kb "no homie" as well as homie. Unfortunately, the authors don't plot this data in the manuscript in the comparison of 3D distances, though the two-point MSD can be seen in Figure S13C, and laudably, the data is made public in a well-annotated repository on Zenodo, noted in the study. Note that the distance data in Figure S13 were filtered to exclude the transcriptionally off state, and are thus not the quantity the current authors are interested in. If they plot the published data for no homie, they will see the clear effect on the average 3D distance, R(s), and a somewhat stronger effect on the contact frequency P(s), which causes significant deviation from the trend-line followed by the homie-containing data.

      (2) The agreement between the "best performing" simulations for all models and the Hi-C data is not on par with prior studies using similar approaches, apparently due to some erroneous choices in how the optimization is carried out:

      Hi-C-comparison

      The 'best fit' simulation Hi-C looks strikingly different from the biological data in all comparisons, with clearly lower agreement than other authors have shown using highly similar methods (e.g., Shi and Thirumalai 2023; Di Pierro et al. 2017; Nuebler et al. 2018; Esposito et al. 2022; Conte et al. 2022), among many others. I believe this results from a few issues with how the current authors select and evaluate the data in their work:

      (a) Most works have used Pearson's correlation rather than Spearman's correlation when comparing simulation and Hi-C contact frequencies. Pearson's correlation is more appropriate when we expect the values to be linearly related, which they should be in this case, as they are constructed indeed to be measuring the same thing (contact frequency), just derived from two different methods. Spearman's correlation would have been justifiable for comparing how transcription output correlates with contact frequency. This may fix the bafflingly low correlations reported at lower adhesion values in Figure S2C.

      (b) Choice of adhesion strengths - The Hi-C map comparison in Figure 3 strongly suggests that a much more striking visual agreement would have been achieved if much weaker (but still non-zero) homotypic monomer affinity had been selected. In the authors' simulation, the monomer state (A/B identity) strongly dominates polymer position, resulting in the visual appearance of an almost black-and-white checkerboard. The data, meanwhile, look like a weak checkerboard superimposed on the polymer.

      (c) A further confounding problem is the aforementioned issue that the Hi-C data don't come from the edited cell lines, and that the interaction of the two Homie sites is vastly stronger than the compartment interactions of this region of the genome.

      (3) Some important concepts from the field are ignored:

      The crumpled/fractal globule model is widely discussed in the literature (including the work containing the data used in this study) - its exclusion from this analysis thus appears as a substantial gap/oversight:

      A natural alternative to the much-discussed Rouse polymer model is the "crumpled polymer" (Grosberg et al. 1988; Grosberg 2016; Halverson et al. 2011; Halverson et al. 2011), also known as the "fractal globule" (Lieberman-Aiden et al. 2009; Mirny 2011; Dekker and Mirny 2016; Boettiger et al. 2016), much discussed for the way it captures the ⅓ scaling of R(s), found for much of the genome (or, equivalently, the -1 exponent of the probability of contact as a function of genome separation, P(s)). Given the 1/3rd scaling in the data, and the fact that the original authors highlighted the crumpled model in addition to the Rouse model, it seems that this comparison would be instructive and the lack of discussion an oversight. Moreover, while prior works (e.g., Buckner, Gregor, 2023) used some traditional simplifying assumptions to estimate the MSD and relaxation time scaling of this model, I believe a more rigorous analysis with explicit simulations (as in Figure 1 for the Rouse model) would be instructive for the crumpled polymer simulations. Note the crumpled globule is not necessarily the same as the globule in the coil-globule transition discussed here - it requires some assumptions about non-entanglement to stay trapped in the meta-stable state which has the 1/3rd R(s) scaling that is indicative of this model, and not the 1/2 exhibited by equilibrium globules (for s<< length of the polymer) and dilute polymers alike.

      While the fit in Figure 2 appears to get closer to the 1/3rd exponent (B= 0.32), this appears to be a largely coincidental allusion of agreement - the simulation data in truth shows a systematic deviation, returning to the 1/2 scaling for distances from 500 kb to whole chromosomes. This feature is not very evident as the authors restrict the analysis to only the few points available in the experimental data, though had they tested intervening distances I expect they would show log-log P(s) is nonlinear (non-powerlaw) for distances less than the typical loop length up to a few fold larger than the loop length, and thereafter returns to the scaling provided by the 'base' polymer behavior. This appears to be Rouse-like in these authors' model, with R(s) going like 1/2, even though the data are closer to 1/3rd, as indeed most published simulated P(s) curves based on loop extrusion - e.g., (Fudenberg et al. 2016; Nuebler et al. 2018). In this vein, it would be instructive to the readers if the authors would include additional predictions from the simulation on the plot that lie at genomic separation distances not tested in the data, to better appreciate the predictions.

      Minor issues

      (1) I think it is too misleading to only describe the experimental data from Brukner as "E-P" interactions from Drosophila. It is important to note somewhere that this is not an endogenous interaction with a functional role in Drosophila - it is a synthetic interaction between enhancers in the vicinity of the eve gene and a synthetic promoter placed at a variable distance away. The uniformity is elegant - (it is the same pair of elements being studied at all distances), but also provides limited scope for generalization as suggested by the current text. Moreover, the enhancers were not directly labeled; rather, the 3D position of nascent RNA transcribed from eve was tracked with an RNA-binding protein and used as a proxy for the 3D position of the enhancers. There is not an individual enhancer at the eve locus that interacts with the transgene, but rather a collection of enhancers is distributed at different positions throughout the entire TAD, which contains eve, and must form separate loops to reach eve. Indeed, it was previously reported that differences in the local position of these enhancers, relative to eve, affect their ability to interact with the distal reporter gene and the endogenous eve gene (Chen 2018). There is also reported competition between these enhancers and the distal gene, which further complicates the analysis (especially since the state of eve and of its enhancers varies among the different cells as a function of stripe position) - see Chen 2018. All of this is ignored in the current work, despite the assertion of the application to understanding E-P interaction. A detailed discussion of these issues is not necessary, but I fear that ignoring them entirely is to invite further confusion and error.

      (2) I believe this sentence is overstated, given available data: " TAD borders are characterized by transitions between epigenetic states rather than by preferentially-bound CTCF [4, 23, 24]." Indeed, this claim has been repeatedly made in the literature as cited here. However, other data clearly demonstrate a strong enrichment of CTCF at TAD borders (and at epigenetic borders, which in Drosophila have a high correspondence with TAD borders, as the authors have already appropriately noted). See, for example, Figure 4 of Sexton Cell 2012, and compare to Figure 2 of Dixon 2012. Of minor note, CTCF peaks co-occupied by the Zinc Finger TF CP190 are more likely to be TAD borders than CTCF alone. How big a species-specific difference this is remains unclear, as it appears some mammalian CTCF-marked TAD boundaries may be co-occupied by additional ZNFs. While plenty of Drosophila TAD boundaries indeed lack CTCF, many are marked by CTCF, this is enriched relative to what would be expected by chance (or relative to the alignment of other TFs, like Twist or Eve with TAD boundaries), and it has been shown that CTCF loss is sufficient to remove a subset of these, see for example Figure 5 of (Kaushal et al. 2021) (though it is possible, most will require mutation of the all the border-associated factors that collectively bind many of the borders, dCTCF, CP190, mod(mdg4) and others).

      (3) This assertion is overstated given available data: "Although TAD boundaries in Drosophila are often associated with insulator proteins [20], there is no direct evidence that these elements block LEFs in vivo. Therefore, we did not impose boundary constraints in our simulations; LEFs were allowed to move freely unless stalled by collisions with other LEFs, with the possibility of crossover.". Deletion of insulator in Drosophila that lie within a common epigenetic state leads to fusion of TADs (e.g., Mateo et al., 2019 - deletion of the CTCF-marked Fub insulator, in posterior tissues where both flanks of Fub are active; Kaushal, 2021, has examples as well). Loss of CTCF causes a small number of TADs to fuse as measured by Hi-C. This is far from 'direct evidence that insulators block LEFs' - as the authors have already noted, even the idea that cohesin extrudes loops in Drosophila in the first place is indeed controversial. However, LEF activity and stalling at insulators would provide a very natural explanation of why chromatin in a shared epigenetic state should form distinct TADs, and why these TADs should fuse upon insulator deletion. Justifying the lack of stalling sites based on empirical data is thus not very convincing to this reviewer. I believe it would be more apt to simply describe this as a simplifying assumption, rather than the above phrase, which may be misleading.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      In this revised submission from Kapustin et al., the authors have made significant changes to the manuscript. Namely, the authors have addressed several of the major issues with the original submission, providing a more concrete link between fibronectin and the secretion of extracellular vesicles. Additionally, the authors have moderated some of the conclusions to better suit the rigor of the experimental results and limitations of their approach. Generally, the findings convey an interesting cell autonomous pathway in which smooth muscle cells sense fibronectin, which canonically is a proinflammatory substrate with activating properties in many tissues. Fibronectin-mediated integrin signaling stimulates secretion of small extracellular vesicles containing collagen VI which is deposited into the surrounding extracellular matrix. Collagen VI itself gleaned from extracellular vesicle secretion seems to further alter smooth muscle cell morphodynamics. For this later finding, much of the mechanism behind collagen VI vesicle loading and secretion has yet to be worked out. The authors provide evidence of extracellular vesicles containing collagen VI trapped in fibronectin in atherosclerotic plaques providing a nice validation of their in vitro findings in a diseased human cohort. Some limitations do still exist in the manuscript in its current form such as the assessment of the vesicle origins, contents and their association with the actin cytoskeleton; however, the rigor and execution are much improved from the preceding version. Overall, the pathobiology underlying vascular smooth muscle remodeling in disease states is a critical area of research that warrants further exploration.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      The findings in the current manuscript are interesting and valuable contributions to the fields of vascular biology and extracellular vesicle-related mechanisms. They suggest a potential role for smooth muscle cell-derived extracellular vesicles in presenting Type VI collagen to cells to orchestrate their migration, with proposed relevance to aberrant smooth muscle cell movements in the progression of atherosclerotic lesions. A wide range of assays are utilized to test various aspects of this working model, with the resulting data being largely solid and supporting several of the interpretations articulated by the authors. The revised manuscript has adequately addressed key weaknesses.

      The authors present data suggesting a working model in which vascular smooth muscle cells (vSMCs) are stimulated by fibronectin (FN) to generate small extracellular vesicles (sEVs) that harbor Type VI Collagen (collagen VI). These collagen VI-associated sEVs are suggested to accumulate in the extracellular matrix (ECM) and influence cell migration and adhesion dynamics, potentially contributing to disease progression in atherosclerosis. Majors strengths of this manuscript include robust imaging data and the inclusion of human-derived samples in their analysis. The authors also make a reasonable attempt to provide data to support the potential existence of these mechanistic connections, though some minor questions remain regarding data interpretation. The authors largely achieved their aims of finding evidence consistent with their interpretations, and they have presented logical support for their conclusions while acknowledging important limitations and caveats to their current study. This work will likely have a sustained impact on the field of sEV biology and potential intersections with vascular biology, including their methodology e.g., imaging approaches. As biologists continue to explore the role of sEVs in physiological and pathological processes, this work raises an interesting aspect that must be considered more broadly, and that is, what is the role of sEVs that are ECM-associated and not necessarily internalized by recipient cells? Are there discrete mechanisms that govern their role in maintaining and/or disrupting normal physiological processes? This manuscript makes an attempt to address these unresolved yet critical questions.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study examines how two common psychiatric treatments, antidepressant medication and cognitive distancing, influence baseline levels and moment-to-moment changes in happiness, confidence, and engagement during a reinforcement learning task. Combining a probabilistic selection task, trial-by-trial affect ratings, psychiatric questionnaires, and computational modeling, the authors demonstrate that each treatment has distinct effects on affective dynamics. Notably, the results highlight the key role of affective biases in how people with mental health conditions experience and update their feelings over time, and suggest that interventions like cognitive distancing and antidepressant medication may work, at least in part, by shifting these biases.

      Strengths:

      (1) Addresses an important question: how common psychiatric treatments impact affective biases, with potential translational relevance for understanding and improving mental health interventions.

      (2) The introduction is strong, clear, and accessible, making the study approachable for readers less familiar with the underlying literature.

      (3) Utilizes a large sample that is broadly representative of the UK population in terms of age and psychiatric symptom history, enhancing generalizability.

      (4) Employs a theory-driven computational modeling framework that links learning processes with subjective emotional experiences.

      (5) Uses cross-validation to support the robustness and generalizability of model comparisons and findings.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors acknowledge the limitations in the discussion section.

      Additional questions:

      (1) Group Balance & Screening for Medication Use: How many participants in the cognitive distancing and control groups were taking antidepressant medication? Why wasn't medication use included as part of the screening to ensure both groups had a similar number of participants taking medication?

      (2) Assessment of the Practice of Cognitive Distancing: Is there a direct or more objective method to evaluate whether participants actively engaged in cognitive distancing during the task, and to what extent? Currently, the study infers engagement indirectly through the outcomes, but does not include explicit measures of participants' use of the technique. Would including self-report check-ins throughout the task, asking participants whether they were actively engaging in cognitive distancing, have been useful? However, including frequent self-report check-ins would increase procedural differences between groups, making perhaps the tasks less comparable beyond the intended treatment manipulation. Maybe incorporating a question at the end of the task, asking how much they engaged in cognitive distancing, could offer a useful measure of subjective engagement without overly disrupting the task flow.

      Conclusion:

      This study advances our understanding of the mechanisms underlying mental health interventions. The combination of computational modeling with behavioral and affective data offers a powerful framework for understanding how treatments influence affective biases and dynamics. These findings are of broad interest across clinical and mental health sciences, cognitive and affective research, and applied translational fields focused on improving psychological well-being.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      In this paper, Dercon and colleagues report on affective changes related to components of reinforcement learning and on the effects of brief training in psychological distancing and participants' self-reported antidepressant use. About 1,000 participants were assessed online, with half randomized to a brief training in psychological distancing with reminders to distance during the subsequent reinforcement learning (RL) task. Participants completed a battery of psychiatric questionnaires and answered questions about medication use, with about 14% of participants reporting current antidepressant use. All participants completed the RL task and rated their happiness, confidence, engagement, and (at the end of each block of trials) fatigue throughout the task. Computational models were used to estimate trial-by-trial values of expected value and prediction error and to assess the effects of these values on self-reported affect. Participants' affect ratings decreased over time, and participants with higher psychiatric symptoms (particularly anxiety/depressive symptoms) showed lower baseline affect and greater decreases in affect. Participants randomized to the distancing intervention and who reported antidepressant use differed in their affective ratings: distancing reduced the reductions in happiness over time, while antidepressant use was related to higher baseline happiness. Distancing also reduced the effects of trial-level expected value on happiness, while antidepressant use was related to a more enduring effect of trial-level values on happiness.

      Overall, this is an interesting paper with strong methods and an interesting approach. That psychiatric symptoms and cognitive distancing are related to affective ratings is not terribly novel; the relationship with antidepressant use is a bit more novel. The extension of the mood model to an RL task is a new contribution, as is the relationship of these effects with psychologically related manipulations.

      One major concern is the inference that can be drawn from the two "treatments": one is a brief instruction in a component of psychotherapy, and one is ongoing use of medication. The former is not a treatment in and of itself, but a (presumably) active ingredient of one. How to interpret antidepressant use as measured is unclear, e.g., are the residual symptoms in these participants an early indicator of treatment resistance? Are these participants with better access to health care? Are they receiving antidepressants for a mental health issue?

      There are some clarifications needed in the affect model as well.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The present manuscript investigates and proposes different mechanisms for the effects of two therapeutic approaches - cognitive distancing technique and use of antidepressants - on subjective ratings of happiness, confidence, and task engagement, and on the influence of such subjective experiences on choice behavior. Both approaches were found to link to changes in affective state dynamics in a choice task, specifically reduced drift (cognitive distancing) and increased baseline (antidepressant use). Results also suggest that cognitive distancing may reduce the weighing of recent expected values in the happiness model, while antidepressant use may reduce forgetting of choices and outcomes.

      Strengths:

      This is a timely topic and a significant contribution to ongoing efforts to improve our mechanistic understanding of psychopathology and devise effective novel interventions. The relevance of the manuscript's central question is clear, and the links to previous literature and the broader field of computational psychiatry are well established. The modelling approaches are thoughtful and rigorously tested, with appropriate model checks and persuasive evidence that modelling complements the theoretical argument and empirical findings.

      Weaknesses:

      Some vagueness and lack of clarity in theoretical mechanisms and interpretation of results leave outstanding questions regarding (a) the specific links drawn between affective biases, therapies aimed at mitigating them, and mental health function, and (b) the structure and assumptions of the modelling, and how they support the manuscript's central claims. Broadly, I do not fully understand the distinction between how choice behavior vs. affect are impacted separately or together by cognitive distancing. Clarification on this point is needed, possibly through a more explicit proposal of a mechanism (or several alternative mechanisms?) in the introduction and more explicit interpretation of the modelling results in the context of the cyclical choice-affect mechanism.

      (1) Theoretical framework and proposed mechanisms

      The link between affective biases and negative thinking patterns is a bit unclear. The authors seem to make a causal claim that "affective biases are precipitated and maintained by negative thinking patterns", but it is unclear what precisely these negative patterns are; earlier in the same paragraph, they state that affective biases "cause low mood" and possibly shift choices toward those that maintain low mood. So the directionality of the mechanism here is unclear - possibly explaining a bit more of the cyclic nature of this mechanism, and maybe clarifying what "negative thinking patterns" refer to will be helpful.

      More generally, this link between affect and choices, especially given the modelling results later on, should be clarified further. What is the mechanism by which these two impact each other? How do the models of choice and affect ratings in the RL task test this mechanism? I'm not quite sure the paper answers these questions clearly right now.

      The authors also seem to implicitly make the claim that symptoms of mental ill-health are at least in part related to choice behavior. I find this a persuasive claim generally; however, it is understated and undersupported in the introduction, to the point where a reader may need to rely on significant prior knowledge to understand why mitigating the impact of affective biases on choice behavior would make sense as the target of therapeutic interventions. This is a core tenet of the paper, and it would be beneficial to clarify this earlier on.

      It would be helpful to interpret a bit more clearly the findings from 3.4. on decreased drift in all three subjective assessments in the cognitive distancing group. What is the proposed mechanism for this? The discussion mentions that "attenuated declines [...] over time, [add] to our previously reported findings that this psychotherapeutic technique alters aspects of reward learning" - but this is vague and I do not understand, if an explanation for how this happens is offered, what that explanation is. Given the strong correlation of the drift with fatigue, is the explanation that cognitive distancing mitigates affect drift under fatigue? Or is this merely reporting the result without an interpretation around potential mechanisms?

      (Relatedly, aside from possibly explaining the drift parameter, do the fatigue ratings link with choice behavior in any way? Is it possible that the cognitive distancing was helping participants improve choices under fatigue?)

      (2) Task Structure and Modelling

      It is unclear what counted as a "rewarding" vs. "unrewarding" trial in the model. From my understanding of the task description, participants obtained positive or no reward (no losses), and verbal feedback, Correct/Incorrect. But given the probabilistic nature of the task, it follows that even some correct choices likely had unrewarding results. Was the verbal feedback still "Correct" in those cases, but with no points shown? I did not see any discussion on whether it is the #points earned or the verbal feedback that is considered a reward in the model. I am assuming the former, but based on previous literature, likely both play a role; so it would be interesting - and possibly necessary to strengthen the paper's argument - to see a model that assigns value to positive/negative feedback and earned points separately.

      From a theory perspective, it's interesting that the authors chose to assume separate learning rates for rewarding and non-rewarding trials. Why not, for example, separate reward sensitivity parameters? E.g., rather than a scaling parameter on the PE, a parameter modifying the r term inside the PE equation to, perhaps, assign different values to positive and zero points? (While I think overall the math works out similarly at the fitting time, this type of model should be less flexible on scaling the expected value and more flexible on scaling the actual #points / the subjective experience of the obtained verbal feedback, which seems more in line with the theoretical argument made in the introduction). The introduction explicitly states that negative biases "may cause low mood by making outcomes appear less rewarding" - which in modelling equations seems more likely to translate to different reward-perception biases, and not different learning rates. Alternatively, one might incorporate a perseveration parameter (e.g., similar to Collins et al. 2014) that would also accomplish a negative bias. Either of these two mechanisms seems perhaps worth testing out in a model - especially in a model that defines more clearly what rewarding vs. unrewarding may mean to the participant.

      If I understand correctly, the affect ratings models assume that the Q-value and the PE independently impact rating (so they have different weights, w2 and w3), but there is no parameter allowing for different impact for perceived rewarding and unrewarding outcomes? (I may be misreading equations 4-5, but if not, Q-value and PE impact the model via static rather than dynamic parameters.) Given the joint RL-affect fit, this seems to carry the assumption that any perceptual processing differences leading to different subjective perceptions of reward associated with each outcome only impact choice behavior, but not affect? (whereas affect is more broadly impacted, if I'm understanding this correctly, just by the magnitude of the values and PEs?) This is an interesting assumption, and the authors seem to have tested it a bit more in the Supplementary material, as shown in Figure S4. I'm wondering why this was excluded from the main text - it seems like the more flexible model found some potentially interesting differences which may be worth including, especially as they might shed additional insight into the influence of cognitive distancing on the cyclical choice-affect mechanisms proposed.

      Minor comments:

      If fatigue ratings were strongly associated with drift in the best-fitting model (as per page 13), I wonder if it would make sense to use those fatigue ratings as a proxy rather than allow the parameter to vary freely? (This does not in any way detract from the winning model's explanatory power, but if a parameter seems to be strongly explained by a variable we have empirical data for, it's not clear what extra benefit is earned by having that parameter in the model).

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Allodynia is commonly measured in the pain field using von Frey filaments, which are applied to a body region (usually hindpaw if studying rodents) by a human. While humans perceive themselves as being objective, as the authors noted, humans are far from consistent when applying these filaments. Not to mention, odors from humans, including of different sexes, can influence animal behavior. There is thus a major unmet need for a way to automate this tedious von Frey testing process, and to remove humans from the experiment. I have no major scientific concerns with the study, as the authors did an outstanding job of comparing this automated system to human experimenters in a rigorous and quantitative manner. They even demonstrated that their automated system can be used in conjunction with in vivo imaging techniques.

      While it is somewhat unclear how easy and inexpensive this device will be, I anticipate everyone in the pain field will be clamoring to get their hands on a system like this. And given the mechanical nature of the device, and propensity for mice to urinate on things, I also wonder how frequently the device breaks/needs to be repaired. Perhaps some details regarding cost and reliability of the device would be helpful to include, as these are the two things that could make researchers hesitant to adopt immediately.

      The only major technical concern, which is easy to address, is whether the device generates ultrasounic sounds that rodents can hear when idle or operational, across the ultrasonic frequencies that are of biological relevance (20-110 kHz). These sounds are generally alarm vocalizations and can create stress in animals, and/or serve as cues of an impending stimulus (if indeed they are produced by the device).

      Comments on revisions:

      Was Fig. 1 updated with the new apparatus design? i.e. to address issue of animal waste affecting function over time?

      I have no further comments.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Burdge, Juhmka et al describe the development and validation of a new automated system for applying plantar stimuli in rodent somatosensory behavior tasks. This platform allows the users to run behavior experiments remotely, removing experimenter effects on animals and reducing variability in manual application of stimuli. The system integrates well with other automated analysis programs that the lab has developed, providing a complete package for standardizing behavior data collection and analysis. The authors present extensive validations of the system against manual stimulus application. Proof of concept studies also show how the system can be used to better understand the effect of experimenters on behavior and the effects of how stimuli are presented on the micro features of the animal withdrawal response.

      Strengths:

      If widely adopted, ARM has the potential to reduce variability in plantar behavior studies across and within labs and provide a means to standardize results. It provides a way to circumvent the confounds that humans bring into performing sensitive plantar behavior tests (e.g. experimenter odors, experince, physical abilities, variation in stimulus application, sex). Furthermore, it can be integrated with other automated platforms, allowing for quicker analysis and potentially automated stimulus delivery. The manuscript also presents some compelling evidence on the effects of stimulus application time and height on withdrawals, which can potentially help labs that are manually applying stimuli standardize applications. The system is well validated and the results are clear and convincingly presented. Claims are well supported by experimental evidence.

      Weaknesses:

      ARM seems like a fantastic system that could be widely adopted, a primary weakness is that it is not currently available to other labs. This will eventually be remedied as it is commercialised.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This report describes the development and initial applications of the ARM (Automated Reproducible Mechano-stimulator), a programmable tool that delivers various mechanical stimuli to a select target (most frequently, a rodent hindpaw). Comparisons to traditional testing methods (e.g., experimenter application of stimuli) reveal that the ARM reduces variability in the anatomical targeting, height, velocity, and total time of stimulus application. Given that the ARM can be controlled remotely, this device was also used to assess effects of experimenter presence on reflexive responses to mechanical stimulation. Although not every experimenter had notable sex-dependent effects on animal behavior, use of the ARM never had this effect (for obvious reasons!). Lastly, the ARM was used to stimulate rodent hindpaws while measuring neuronal activity in the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (BLA), a brain region that is associated with the negative affect of pain. This device, and similar automated devices, will undoubtedly reduce experimenter-related variability in reflexive mechanical behavior tests; this may increase experimental reproducibility between laboratories who are able to invest in this type of technology.

      Strengths:

      Clear examples of variability in experimenter stimulus application are provided and then contrasted with uniform stimulus application that is inherent to the ARM.

      The ARM is able to quickly oscillate between delivery of various mechanical stimuli; this is advantageous for experimental efficiency.

      New additions to the ARM and PAWS platforms have been methodically tested to ensure reproducibility and reliability.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Ferreiro et al. present a method to simulate protein sequence evolution under a birth-death model where sequence evolution is guided by structural constraints on protein stability. The authors then use this model to explore the predictability of sequence evolution in several viral proteins. In principle, this work is of great interest to molecular evolution and phylodynamics, which has struggled to couple non-neutral models of sequence evolution to phylodynamic models like birth-death processes. Unfortunately, though, the model shows little improvement over neutral models in predicting protein sequence evolution, although it can predict protein stability better than models assuming neutral evolution. It appears that more work is needed to determine exactly what aspects of protein sequence evolution are predictable under such non-neutral phylogenetic models.

      Major concerns:

      (1) The authors have clarified the mapping between birth-death model parameters and fitness, but how fitness is modeled still appears somewhat problematic. The authors assume the death rate = 1 - birth rate. So a variant with a birth rate b = 1 would have a death rate d = 0 and so would be immortal and never die, which does not seem plausible. Also I'm not sure that this would "allow a constant global (birth-death) rate" as stated in line 172, as selection would still act to increase the population mean growth rate r = b - d. It seems more reasonable to assume that protein stability affects only either the birth or death rate and assume the other rate is constant, as in the Neher 2014 model.

      (2) It is difficult to evaluate the predictive performance of protein sequence evolution. This is in part due to the fact that performance is compared in terms of percent divergence, which is difficult to compare across viral proteins and datasets. Some protein sequences would be expected to diverge more because they are evolving over longer time scales, under higher substitution rates or under weaker purifying selection. It might therefore help to normalize the divergence between predicted and observed sequences by the expected or empirically observed amount of divergence seen over the timescale of prediction.

      (3) Predictability may also vary significantly across different sites in a protein. For example, mutations at many sites may have little impact on structural stability (in which case we would expect poor predictive performance) while even conservative changes at other sites may disrupt folding. I therefore feel that there remains much work to be done here in terms of figuring out where and when sequence evolution might be predictable under these types of models, and when sequence evolution might just be fundamentally unpredictable due to the high entropy of sequence space.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      In this study, the authors aim to forecast the evolution of viral proteins by simulating sequence changes under a constraint of folding stability. The central idea is that proteins must retain a certain level of structural stability (quantified by folding free energy, ΔG) to remain functional, and that this constraint can shape and restrict the space of viable evolutionary trajectories. The authors integrate a birth-death population model with a structurally constrained substitution (SCS) model and apply this simulation framework to several viral proteins from HIV-1, SARS-CoV-2, and influenza.

      The motivation to incorporate biophysical constraints into evolutionary models is scientifically sound, and the general approach aligns with a growing interest in bridging molecular evolution and structural biology. The authors focus on proteins where immune pressure is limited and stability is likely to be a dominant constraint, which is conceptually appropriate. The method generates sequence variants that preserve folding stability, suggesting that stability-based filtering may capture certain evolutionary patterns.

      However, the study does not substantiate its central claim of forecasting. The model does not predict future sequences with measurable accuracy, nor does it reproduce observed evolutionary paths. Validation is limited to endpoint comparisons in a few datasets. While KL divergence is used to compare amino acid distributions, this analysis is only applied to a single protein (HIV-1 MA), and there is no assessment of mutation-level predictive accuracy or quantification of how well simulated sequences recapitulate real evolutionary paths. No comparison is made to real intermediate variants available from extensive viral sequencing datasets which gather thousands of sequences with detailed collection date annotation (SARS-CoV-2, Influenza, RSV).

      The selection of proteins is narrow and the rationale for including or excluding specific proteins is not clearly justified.

      The analyzed datasets are also under-characterized: we are not given insight into how variable the sequences are or how surprising the simulated sequences might be relative to natural diversity. Furthermore, the use of consensus sequences to represent timepoints is problematic, particularly in the context of viral evolution, where divergent subclades often coexist - a consensus sequence may not accurately reflect the underlying population structure.

      The fitness function used in the main simulations is based on absolute ΔG and rewards increased stability without testing whether real evolutionary trajectories tend to maintain, increase, or reduce folding stability over time for the particular systems (proteins) that are studied. While a variant of the model does attempt to center selection around empirical ΔG values, this more biologically plausible version is underutilized and not well validated.

      Ultimately, the model constrains sequence evolution to stability-compatible trajectories but does not forecast which of these trajectories are likely to occur. It is better understood as a filter of biophysically plausible outcomes than as a predictive tool. The distinction between constraint-based plausibility and sequence-level forecasting should be made clearer. Despite these limitations, the work may be of interest to researchers developing simulation frameworks or exploring the role of protein stability in viral evolution, and it raises interesting questions about how biophysical constraints shape sequence space over time.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study is an evaluation of patient variants in the kidney isoform of AE1 linked to distal renal tubular acidosis. Drawing on observations in the mouse kidney, this study extends findings to autophagy pathways in a kidney epithelial cell line.

      Strengths:

      Experimental data are convincing and nicely done.

      Weaknesses:

      Some data are lacking or not explained clearly. Mutations are not consistently evaluated throughout the study, which makes it difficult to draw meaningful conclusions.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Context and significance:

      Distal renal tubular acidosis (dRTA) can be caused by mutations in a Cl-/HCO3- exchanger (kAE1) encoded by the SLC4A1 gene. The precise mechanisms underlying the pathogenesis of the disease due to these mutations are unclear, but it is thought that loss of the renal intercalated cells (ICs) that express kAE1 and/or aberrant autophagy pathway function in the remaining ICs may contribute to the disease. Understanding how mutations in SLC4A1 affect cell physiology and cells within the kidney, a major goal of this study, is an important first step to unraveling the pathophysiology of this complex heritable kidney disease.

      Summary:

      The authors identify a number of new mutations in the SLC4A1 gene in patients with diagnosed dRTA that they use for heterologous experiments in vitro. They also use a dRTA mouse model with a different SLC4A1 mutation for experiments in mouse kidneys. Contrary to previous work that speculated dRTA was caused mainly by trafficking defects of kAE1, the authors observe that their new mutants (with the exception of Y413H, which they only use in Figure 1) traffic and localize at least partly to the basolateral membrane of polarized heterologous mIMCD3 cells, an immortalized murine collecting duct cell line. They go on to show that the remaining mutants induce abnormalities in the expression of autophagy markers and increased numbers of autophagosomes, along with an alkalinized intracellular pH. They also reported that cells expressing the mutated kAE1 had increased mitochondrial content coupled with lower rates of ATP synthesis. The authors also observed a partial rescue of the effects of kAE1 variants through artificially acidifying the intracellular pH. Taken together, this suggests a mechanism for dRTA independent of impaired kAE1 trafficking and dependent on intracellular pH changes that future studies should explore.

      Strengths:

      The authors corroborate their findings in cell culture with a well-characterized dRTA KI mouse and provide convincing quantification of their images from the in vitro and mouse experiments.

      Weaknesses:

      The data largely support the claims as stated, with some minor suggestions for improving the clarity of the work. Some of the mutants induce different strengths of effects on autophagy and the various assays than others, and it is not clear why this is from the present manuscript, given that they propose pHi and the unifying mechanism.

    3. Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors have identified novel dRTA causing SLC4A1 mutations and studied the resulting kAE1 proteins to determine how they cause dRTA. Based on a previous study on mice expressing the dRTA kAE1 R607H variant, the authors hypothesize that kAE1 variants cause an increase in intracellular pH, which disrupts autophagic and degradative flux pathways. The authors clone these new kAE1 variants and study their transport function and subcellular localization in mIMCD cells. The authors show increased abundance of LC3B II in mIMCD cells expressing some of the kAE1 variants, as well as reduced autophagic flux using eGFP-RFP-LC3. These data, as well as the abundance of autophagosomes, serve as the key evidence that these kAE1 mutants disrupt autophagy. Furthermore, the authors demonstrate that decreasing the intracellular pH abrogates the expression of LC3B II in mIMCD cells expressing mutant SLC4A1. Lastly, the authors argue that mitochondrial function, and specifically ATP synthesis, is suppressed in mIMCD cells expressing dRTA variants and that mitochondria are less abundant in AICs from the kidney of R607H kAE1 mice. While the manuscript does reveal some interesting new results about novel dRTA causing kAE1 mutations, the quality of the data to support the hypothesis that these mutations cause a reduction in autophagic flux can be improved. In particular, the precise method of how the western blots and the immunofluorescence data were quantified, with included controls, would enhance the quality of the data and offer more supportive evidence of the authors' conclusions.

      Strengths:

      The authors cloned novel dRTA causing kAE1 mutants into expression vectors to study the subcellular localization and transport properties of the variants. The immunofluorescence images are generally of high quality, and the authors do well to include multiple samples for all of their western blots.

      Weaknesses:

      Inconsistent results are reported for some of the variants. For example, R295H causes intracellular alkalinization but also has no effect on intracellular pH when measured by BCECF. The authors also appear to have performed these in vitro studies on mIMCD cells that were not polarized, and therefore, the localization of kAE1 to the basolateral membrane seems unlikely, based upon images included in the manuscript. Additionally, there is no in vivo work to demonstrate that these kAE1 variants alter intracellular pH, including the R607H mouse, which is available to the authors. The western blots are of varying quality, and it is often unclear which of the bands are being quantified. For example, LAMP1 is reported at 100kDa, the authors show three bands, and it is unclear which one(s) are used to quantify protein abundance. Strikingly, the authors report a nonsensical value for their quantification of LCRB II in Figure 2, where the ratio of LCRB II to total LCRB (I + II) is greater than one. The control experiments with starvation and bafilomyocin are not supportive and significantly reduce enthusiasm for the authors' findings regarding autophagy. There are labeling errors between the manuscript and the figures, which suggest a lack of vigilance in the drafting process.

    1. Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Lysosomal damage is commonly found in many diseases including normal aging and age-related disease. However, the transcriptional programs activated by lysosomal damage has not been thoroughly characterized. This study aims to investigate lysosome damage-induced major transcriptional responses and the underlying signaling basis. The authors have convincingly shown that lysosomal damage activates a ubiquitination-dependent signaling axis involving TAB, TAK1, and IKK, which culminate in the activation of NF-kB and subsequent transcriptional upregulation of pro-inflammatory genes and pro-survival genes. Overall, the major aims of this study are successfully achieved.

      Strengths:

      This study is well-conceived and strictly executed, leading to clear and well-supported conclusions. Through unbiased transcriptomics and proteomics screens, the authors identifies NF-kB as a major transcriptional program activated upon lysosome damage. TAK1 activation by lysosome damage-induced ubiquitination is found to be essential for NF-kB activation and MAP kinase signaling. The transcriptional and proteomic changes are shown to be largely driven by TAK1 signaling. Finally, the TAK1-IKK signaling is shown to provide resistance to apoptosis during lysosomal damage response. The main signaling axis of this pathway has been convincingly demonstrated.

      Overall, this study identifies major transcriptional responses following lysosomal damage through unbiased approaches. It is important to consider the impact of these pathways in disease settings where lysosomal integrity is compromised.

      Comments on revisions:

      The authors have adequately addressed all previous comments. I have no further recommendations.

    2. Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Endo et al. investigate the novel role of ubiquitin response upon lysosomal damage in activating cellular signaling for cell survival. The authors provide a comprehensive transcriptome and proteome analysis of aging-related cells experiencing lysosomal damage, identifying transcription factors involved in transcriptome and proteome remodeling with a focus on the NF-κB signaling pathway. They further characterized the K63-ubiquitin-TAB-TAK1-NF-κB signaling axis in controlling gene expression, inflammatory responses, and apoptotic processes.

      Strengths:

      In the aging-related model, the authors provide a comprehensive transcriptome and characterize the K63-ubiquitin-TAB-TAK1-NF-κB signaling axis. Through compelling experiments and advanced tools, they elucidate its critical role in controlling gene expression, inflammatory responses, and apoptotic processes.

      Weaknesses:

      The study lacks deeper connections with previous research, particularly:

      • The established role of TAB-TAK1 in AMPK activation during lysosomal damage

      • The potential significance of TBK1 in NF-κB signaling pathways

      Comments on revisions:

      The authors have successfully addressed all the raised questions and the manuscript is now significantly improved.