10,000 Matching Annotations
  1. Jul 2018
    1. On 2014 May 01, Giovanni M. Dall’Olio commented:

      Interesting article that explores the role of pseudogene formation by retrotranscription of mRNAs in somatic cells in cancer.

      After reading the article, my impression is that pseudogene formation is not a major process driving cancer. In fact, the percentage of pseudogenes found in the cancer samples analyzed is relatively low, and only few of these cases seem to be relevant to the cancer process. However, it is very good to have an estimate of how much somatic retrotransposition is important in cancer, and to have a few examples of newly formed pseudogenes causing damage to the expression of cancer genes.

      One detail that didn't convince me of the article is the result presented in Figure 3. I don't think there are enough points for the Wilcoxon test, except maybe only for the lung samples. Maybe a permutation test would be more robust, although the real problem is that there are too few data points.

      A colleague of mine also noted, from Figure 1, that most of the pseudogenes observed seem to be patient-specific, rather than cancer-type specific. For example patient PD7354 (if we interpret the sample naming scheme right) seems to host half of pseudogene events observed. Can it be that this individual is somehow more prone to this process, e.g. that retrontransposon activity is higher in this individual?

      It would also be useful to have an estimate of the overall mutation rate in somatic cells in the samples analyzed, to know if this correlates with pseudogene formation.

      Congratulations again to the author for the work presented.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 09, David Keller commented:

      Are tadalafil and vardenafil also associated with increased risk of melanoma?

      Sildenafil (Viagra) was associated with increased risk of melanoma in this study, and a plausible biological mechanism was proposed to explain this association. Two other closely-related phosphodiesterase inhibitors are widely used: tadalafil (Cialis) and vardenafil (Levitra); are these drugs also associated with increased risk of melanoma? One would expect this association to be a class effect.

      The results of this study will be of particular concern to men who have used sildenafil and may be at elevated risk of melanoma due to history of extensive sun exposure, basal or squamous cell skin cancers, Parkinson disease (which causes autonomic erectile dysfunction and is associated with elevated risk of melanoma) or family history of melanoma.

      A recent editorial advocates adjuvant therapy with vitamin D and melatonin for every stage of melanoma, and as secondary prophylaxis versus melanoma recurrence (1). Given the low risk of harm from these supplements, might it be reasonable for persons at high risk for melanoma to consider taking them as primary prophylaxis?

      Reference

      1: Slominski AT, Carlson JA. Melanoma resistance: a bright future for academicians and a challenge for patient advocates. Mayo Clin Proc. 2014 Apr;89(4):429-33. doi: 10.1016/j.mayocp.2014.02.009. PubMed PMID: 24684870.

      If you find this comment unhelpful, please explain why, to facilitate discussion


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 May 02, Francisco Xavier Castellanos commented:

      This appears to be a well-intentioned open trial with unblinded reports provided by the parents as the evidence of improvement. There is no way in which these observations can be interpreted as a "demonstration" of a "beneficial effect." Was this "clinical trial" registered on clinicaltrials.gov?


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 24, Guillaume Filion commented:

      This article is one of the "CISCOM meta-analyses", which are very similar papers written by different authors. For more information about the CISCOM meta-analyses, check the blog post "A flurry of copycast on PubMed" at the following link http://blog.thegrandlocus.com/2014/10/a-flurry-of-copycats-on-pubmed


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 May 03, Stefanie Butland commented:

      All interaction data from this paper are freely available at IntAct http://www.ebi.ac.uk/intact/query/24705354 and are featured as Dataset of the Month for May 2014. These include 312 binary interactions from yeast two-hybrid, anti tag coimmunoprecipitation, fluorescence microscopy and luminescence based mammalian interactome mapping (LUMIER) experiments.

      We submitted our data directly to IntAct through the IMEx Consortium as part of the publication process. I encourage others to consider this route to making your data available for re-use and re-mixing as the expert biocuration service provided by IntAct was smooth, accurate, and required very little of our time. A very positive experience.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 May 23, Roy Wise commented:

      It is interesting that the decrease in dopamine release that Willhun et al. found to correlate with escalated cocaine intake (decreased within-session inter-response times) was a decrease in the core rather than the shell of nucleus accumbens. Like Willhun et al., we have found that attenuated dopamine actions in the core (but not the shell) of accumbens results in greater cocaine intake—shorter inter-response times—during periods of self-administration (Suto, Psychopharmacol 205, 431-439). Conversely, we find that enhanced dopamine actions in the core (but not the shell) result in decreased intake—longer inter-response times (Suto J Neurosci 31, 17917-17922). We expected the opposite: that dopamine antagonists and agonists would increase or increase intravenous cocaine intake when infused (by reverse dialysis) into the ventromedial shell but not the core. Our hypothesis was based on the findings that reinforcing actions of cocaine take place in the shell and not the core (Carlezon, Psychopharmacol, 122, 194-197; Ikemoto, J Neurosci 23, 9305-9311); we assumed that goal of intravenous self-administration was to elevate dopamine levels at the site where dopamine (Ikemoto, J Neurosci 17,8580-8587) is rewarding. On the contrary, our experiments and Willhun’s, suggest that dopaminergic activation in the core serves quite a different function than that served by dopaminergic activation in the shell. Dopamine in the shell serves the functions of establishing and maintaining cocaine self-administration habits, whereas dopamine in the core serves the function of limiting cocaine intake, producing periods of cocaine satiation between successive injections. The mechanisms for establishing, maintaining, and reinstatement of cocaine self-administration have been studied extensively, but have not yet led to a proven medication for cocaine addiction. Perhaps it is time to turn attention to the endogenous mechanisms for what appears to be a state of drug satiety.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 May 08, Serge Ahmed commented:

      This study provides important novel insights into the neurobiological mechanisms that drive escalation of cocaine intake in rats. It shows that escalation of i.v. cocaine self-administration is selectively associated with a loss of cue-induced phasic dopamine in the nucleus accumbens. Reversing this neurochemical deficit with l-Dopa (30 mg/kg) was sufficient to reverse escalated levels of cocaine intake, suggesting that cue-induced phasic dopamine plays a causal role in regulating cocaine intake.

      This conclusion is rather unexpected because cocaine cues are generally invoked as key triggers of drug seeking but not as key regulators of drug taking. Perhaps one way to test this hypothesis would be to measure the effects of cue omission (or reduced cue intensity) on maintenance of cocaine self-administration. This intervention should prevent (or reduce) cue-induced phasic dopamine and thus induce an increase in cocaine intake, at least initially.

      Finally, to better understand the action of l-Dopa on escalation of cocaine intake, it will be important to check whether and to what extent it also affects tonic dopamine levels during a session of cocaine self-administration.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Oct 31, Morten Oksvold commented:

      Please note that this article contains unreliable data, and should not be cited. The central ethical review board in Sweden found research misconduct in six articles by Macchiarini, including this one.

      Please see the report from the Central ethical review board in Sweden: http://www.epn.se/media/2516/pressmeddelande-o-12-2016eng.pdf https://drive.google.com/file/d/0By2HqPi4t2RbYzZweVRieVVMajhJQUM0cmFMekwyRVJTUFVr/view

      This information was provided by Leonid Schneider (forbetterscience.com).


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2016 Sep 10, Morten Oksvold commented:

      Before reading or citing this article, please read the reports regarding the Macchirini case:

      http://ki.se/sites/default/files/karolinska_institutet_and_the_macchiarini_case_summary_in_english_and_swedish.pdf

      http://ki.se/en/news/macchiarini


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2016 Feb 14, Md. Shahidul Islam commented:

      None


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Jun 23, Darko Lavrencic commented:

      (R)evolving concept of spontaneous intracranial hypotension: from Georg(es) Schaltenbrand coming back to Georg(es) Schaltenbrand, see http://www.med-lavrencic.si/research/correspondence/


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 05, GRAHAM COLDITZ commented:

      Arising from an ARRA funded award to build capacity for cancer prevention and control in Guatemala, this paper summarizes in a way the power of linking to service providers who are keen to improve outcomes in their population. The value added from these training opportunities and return on investment keeps compounding. See, for example, the Global Health Scholars program in internal medicine at Barnes and Washington University School of Medicine. http://ghs.wustl.edu


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:

      Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Jul 30, David Keller commented:

      This study can't tell whether avoiding sun exposure or the resulting low vitamin D levels were to blame

      The authors state that "all-cause mortality is related to low vitamin D levels", and that "exposure to sunlight remains the main source of vitamin D". They did not have access to data concerning vitamin D levels or supplementation in their study. Therefore, they cannot control for vitamin D levels, nor prove that sun exposure confers any benefits beyond those associated with higher vitamin D levels, regardless of whether the vitamin D is obtained from sun exposure or by taking a supplement pill. We know that sun exposure leads to increased risk for malignant melanoma. It is unreasonable to risk a deadly skin cancer to obtain vitamin D, rather than take vitamin D in supplement form. This will not change until a study is performed which controls for vitamin D levels, and thereby demonstrates mortality benefit for sun exposure which is independent of sunlight's effect on raising vitamin D levels.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2017 Jul 31, David Keller commented:

      Study subjects who exercise indoors, avoid solar radiation exposure & take vitamin D"

      PubMed Commons commenters have difficulty responding to anonymous "unhelpful" votes which do not include any explanation or critique. I will have to guess that the study of spelunkers would be impractical (too few spelunkers) or biased (spelunkers are too distinctly different than non-spelunkers). So, instead of spelunking, substitute any form of moderate indoor exercise which includes social contact. For example, subjects who work out at indoor gyms and take vitamin D supplements at adequate doses could be compared with persons who exercise outdoors, to control for vitamin D levels and other confounding sources of bias associated with sun exposure of the skin. This is an important question, because the inherently limited findings of the Melanoma In Southern Sweden (MISS) study are being touted widely in the popular media by sunbathing advocates, which could lead to a future increase in malignant melanoma and even all-cause mortality, depending on the degree to which the MISS trial was confounded by unrecognized biases. The public generally are not informed that the MISS results do not prove that sunbathing promotes health or longevity, independent of vitamin D levels, or some other unrecognized and uncontrolled difference between sun-seekers and sun-avoiders.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2017 Jul 28, David Keller commented:

      A proposed study to determine whether sun exposure promotes good health, or good health is a result of other factors associated with sun exposure, like sociable moderate exercise

      Spelunking, the recreational exploration of caves, is an "outdoor" activity that promotes healthful cardiovascular exertion and social interaction, but zero sun exposure. The null hypothesis is that solar irradiation of the skin is not beneficial per se, and that the mortality benefits observed by Lindqvist and colleagues were due to factors associated with sun exposure, such as higher vitamin D levels, social interactions, and moderate exertion during hikes, volleyball games, etc. If sun exposure was, therefore, at best a "bystander" factor overall with regard to the mortality benefits observed in Lindqvist's study, then we ought to observe similar benefits in mortality among active spelunkers, despite the fact that they derive zero sun exposure from cave exploration. Is spelunking associated with the same benefits as sun exposure (or, are spelunkers equally self-preselected for good health) after correcting for vitamin D status? Spelunking mortality must also be corrected for the high rate of rabies from bat bites, and for trauma associated with spelunking, such as impalement injuries on stalactites and stalagmites, sudden drops into deep holes and other unseen hazards, and spelunkers who get lost in the caves and are never heard from again, who should be presumed dead, by intention to treat. Importantly, the serum vitamin D levels of spelunkers should be raised by oral supplementation until both groups exhibit equivalent levels. After those corrections, if spelunkers are found to be as healthy as those who sun-expose, we would have evidence supporting the null hypothesis, posed above.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    4. On 2017 Jul 24, David Keller commented:

      Swedes, and other depigmented peoples, should not engage in "daily, midday....sun exposure" of any duration

      Depigmentation of northern peoples may seem like an evolutionary argument in favor of sun exposure to the skin. However, there is no safe threshold of solar radiation, especially for the fair-skinned, who are at the highest risk of melanoma. The risk of mutation accrues with each and every ionizing photon that hits the skin.

      If there is benefit to sunbathing, then we should seek to identify and isolate the factors that provide such benefits, and advise the healthy how to maximize the benefits associated with sun exposure while minimizing its risks. The increased mortality from sunbathing is fully expressed over decades, not months or years. Older generations may have been too busy or too embarrassed to lay around near-naked in the sun. Today's generation suffers from no such inhibitions or lack of leisure time, and melanoma is now one of the fastest-increasing causes of cancer death.

      My recommendation to patients, in "light" of the findings of Dr. Lindqvist and others, is as follows:

      1) Avoid sun exposure to the skin. Never sunbathe. The best sunblock is clothing or shade.

      2) Engage in outdoor activities that promote cardiovascular exertion and social interactions, which are healthy, while fully dressed and always seeking shade.

      3) Evidence suggests that indirect light exposure can prevent or treat seasonal depression. This means that visual perception of bright light may be beneficial, but avoid direct solar radiation to the retina, which again causes melanoma. Outdoor activities in the shade safely provide this benefit.

      4) Strict avoidance of sun exposure can lead to sub-optimum blood levels of vitamin D, which I suggest measuring with blood tests, and treating with vitamin D supplements. I aim for a high-normal vitamin D level.

      If any other benefits of sun exposure are isolated, we should strive to find safer ways to attain those benefits. The increased risk of melanoma from sunbathing cannot be mitigated. Doctors who advise sunbathing violate the Hippocratic directive of "Primum non nocere" ("First, do no harm").


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    5. On 2017 Jul 18, P G Lindqvist commented:

      Are we forcing people to choose health or melanoma? Or having a balanced discussion regarding pros and cons of sun exposure? In our observational study 2014 Lindqvist PG, 2014, we showed a strong inverse relationship between sun exposure and risk of all-cause death. Dr Keller is right that from an observational study we might not show causality. Even if we adjust for age, education, marital status, income, exercise, and smoking there might still be residual bias. The authors draw the conclusion that we might titrate a proper vitamin D dose, without raising the melanoma risk. This might be right, however, we still do not know that vitamin D is “the” mediator. Over exposure of solar radiation is a risk factor for melanoma incidence. However, as we show in our 2016 publication Lindqvist PG, 2016 using the same material. Incidence of melanoma increase with increasing sun exposure, but avoiders of sun exposure have a higher death rate if caching melanoma. Therefore, in Sweden, which is a country with scarcity of UV radiation we suggest daily, midday, short time sun exposure.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    6. On 2017 Jul 07, David Keller commented:

      Forcing a choice between vitamin D deficiency and elevated melanoma risk is a fallacy of the inevitable alternative

      This observational study demonstrated that sun exposure was inversely associated with all-cause mortality. No epidemiological study, such as this, can distinguish whether lack of sun exposure caused increased mortality, or whether being destined to have increased mortality for other reasons caused reduced participation in outdoor activities that confer sun exposure.

      Even if reduced sun exposure does increase mortality due to relative vitamin D deficiency, as the authors theorize, nobody would be forced to risk melanoma by exposing their skin to solar radiation. Vitamin D supplements can be titrated to match the blood levels of vitamin D achieved by sun worshipers, without raising melanoma risk.

      The implication that people must choose to either raise their risk of melanoma by exposing their skin to solar radiation, or suffer morbidity and mortality from relative vitamin D deficiency is an example of the logical fallacy known as "the inevitable alternative".

      Exposure of the skin to solar radiation is the number one modifiable risk factor for malignant melanoma, a cancer with rapidly rising incidence in the U.S.

      The American Academy of Dermatology warns, "There is no scientifically validated, safe threshold level of UV exposure from the sun or indoor tanning devices that allows for maximal vitamin D synthesis without increasing skin cancer risk." [1]

      1: American Academy of Dermatology, Position Paper on Vitamin D. Accessed on 7/5/2017 at:<br> https://www.aad.org/Forms/Policies/Uploads/PS/PS-Vitamin D Postition Statement.pdf


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Mar 02, Atanas G. Atanasov commented:

      Consumer Health Digest Scientific Abstract of this article is available at: https://www.consumerhealthdigest.com/general-health/is-yogurt-beneficial-for-healthy-aging.html


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Apr 04, Kamel Hamzaoui commented:

      Yes the paper is very interesting and conclusion was true


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jul 26, Borja Ibanez commented:

      Fully agree with the comment. It is surprising that the infarct-limiting effect of b-blockers in the context of reperfused STEMI was not properly evaluated. The METOCARD-CNIC is the first trial and should be taken as a pilot endeavor. It shows that metoprolol (most probably not all b-blocker are equal in this regard) reduces infarct size. This was the primary endpoint and the trial was powered just for this outcome. All other benefits are hypothesis generating. Of note, the reduction in heart failure events could be of massive socioeconomic impact if proven in dedicated trials. There is a European multidisciplinary consortium working on the design of a large trial recruiting STEMI Pts in 8 countries randomizing them to pre-PCI metoprolol or placebo. primary outcome will be death or admission due to heart failure. This is the MOVE ON! trial. If positive, clinical practive will change. Until then, these results are the most promising in the field of cardioprotection in STEMI


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Jul 24, Ryan Radecki commented:

      Post-publication commentary:

      "The Return of Metoprolol – for Anterior STEMI"

      Beta-blockade early in the course of myocardial infarction was once fashionable – until COMMIT demonstrated an excess of early cardiogenic shock detracting from subsequent late, favorable effects. This led to beta-blockade initiation being deferred until after hemodynamic stability established....

      http://www.emlitofnote.com/2014/07/the-return-of-metoprolol-for-anterior.html


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jul 28, Amanda Capes-Davis commented:

      Please be aware that HBL-100 is known to be misidentified and it is unclear if these cells are actually from breast. Some authentic stocks may exist; stocks can be authenticated using a test method such as short tandem repeat (STR) profiling. For a list of known cross-contaminated or otherwise misidentified cell lines, see http://iclac.org/databases/cross-contaminations/.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 07, Anders von Heijne commented:

      This is an interesting and important discussion. However, the cited paper by Geurts et al from 2005, that compares 3D FLAIR and 3D DIR, reports on older versions of both sequences, with scan times around 15 minutes and with rather poor image quality in the published images. Modern versions using parallell imaging renders much better image quality with higher GM-WM contrast. 3D FLAIR is also much better in showing cortical lesions than comparable 2D sequences, if not quite as good as 3D DIR. The most pragmatic approach is then, in my opinion, to use modern 3D FLAIR, with reconstructed 3mm consecutive images in three orthogonal planes for monitoring MS patients, allowing for the best trade off between scan time and detection of WM and GM lesions. 3D DIR has its place in the diagnostic phase of neuroinflammation in order to optimize the process of differential diagnosis.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Mar 10, Pavel Nesmiyanov commented:

      Logically, simple sugars (e.g. glucose) become available for the lactic bacteria other bacteria, that are cariogenic, after the enzymatic action and the toothpaste with amylase activity only promotes caries.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Dec 19, F-C Sung commented:

      I am sorry that there was a typo error in the first paragraph of dicussion. The numbers in the abstract and results were correct.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2016 Dec 19, Cheng-Yang Hsieh commented:

      In the first paragraph of Discussion, the authors mentioned that "ESRD patients undergoing HD had an adjusted HR of 3.67 for SDH and an adjusted HR of 6.54 for SDH-associated mortality, compared with the control cohort." The numbers of "3.67" and "6.54" were different from those described in the Abstract and Results (i.e., "3.81" and "6.34", respectively). Please check.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 04, Arnaud Chiolero MD PhD commented:

      An interesting study trying to disentangle the relative contribution of birth weight and weight gain at differents times during the lifecourse on cardiometabolic risk factors


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Jun 02, Thomas Perls, MD, MPH commented:

      The authors of this meta-analysis of largely nonagenarians (Deelen J, 2014) indicate that they were unable to reproduce the associations of the 281 SNPs in Sebastiani P, 2012 and used their negative result to assert that the findings were false positive associations.

      In Sebastiani P, 2016 however, we show that the genetic basis of surviving to age 90 years (the 5th and 15th percentiles of survival for men and women belonging to the 1900 birth cohort) is weaker and different from the genetic basis of surviving to the top one percentile, which in turn is different for those surviving to the top 0.1 percentile of survival.

      Our analysis of sibling relative risk of extreme longevity (Sebastiani P, 2016) indicates is that one should not be surprised by the lack of consistent results between these studies of people surviving to markedly different percentiles of survival and therefore having substantially different degrees of statistical power to discover variants associated with extreme survival. Reinforcing this point, when studies are similar in terms of a rare percentile of survival, a large number of the associated SNPs are actually replicated (Sebastiani P, 2013).


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2015 Mar 31, Thomas Perls, MD, MPH commented:

      None


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 11, David Keller commented:

      Vitamin D and melatonin for primary prophylaxis of melanoma?

      Slominski and Carlson advocate adjuvant therapy with vitamin D and melatonin for every stage of melanoma, and as secondary prophylaxis versus melanoma recurrence. [1] Given the low risk of harm from these supplements, might it be reasonable for persons at high risk of melanoma to consider taking them as primary prophylaxis? An example might be a man with Parkinson disease, associated sildenafil use, a family history of melanoma, and personal history of extensive sun exposure and basal cell carcinomas.

      A recent observational study links the use of sildenafil to increased incidence of melanoma, and describes a plausible biological mechanism for this medication side-effect (1). This will be of particular concern to men who have used sildenafil or related phosphodiesterase inhibitors, and who may be at elevated risk of melanoma due to a history of extensive sun exposure, basal or squamous cell skin cancers, Parkinson disease (which causes autonomic erectile dysfunction and is associated with elevated risk of melanoma) or family history of melanoma.

      References

      1: Slominski AT, Carlson JA. Melanoma resistance: a bright future for academicians and a challenge for patient advocates. Mayo Clin Proc. 2014 Apr;89(4):429-33. doi: 10.1016/j.mayocp.2014.02.009. PubMed PMID: 24684870; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4050658.

      2: Li WQ, Qureshi AA, Robinson KC, Han J. Sildenafil Use and Increased Risk of Incident Melanoma in US Men: A Prospective Cohort Study. JAMA Intern Med. 2014 Apr 7. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2014.594. [Epub ahead of print] PubMed PMID: 24710960.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 26, Guillaume Filion commented:

      This article is one of the "CISCOM meta-analyses", which are very similar papers written by different authors. For more information about the CISCOM meta-analyses, check the blog post "A flurry of copycast on PubMed" at the following link http://blog.thegrandlocus.com/2014/10/a-flurry-of-copycats-on-pubmed


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Dec 15, Victoria MacBean commented:

      Plain English summary:

      Sickle Cell Disease (SCD) is amongst the most prevalent genetic diseases worldwide. Only being inherited if both of one’s parents carry a ‘faulty’ gene in their DNA, SCD affects the Haemoglobin molecules that carry oxygen in the blood, distorting the shape of the red blood cells into so-called crescent shaped ‘sickles’.

      It has been shown previously that the majority of adults with SCD have changes in their lungs that can be found on a CT scanner, a high powered X-ray scanner that can create a detailed 3D image of the lungs, including airways and blood vessels.

      This study showed that findings like particularly large blood vessels in the lung were linked to reduced lung function. This study aimed to show a link between these changes in the lung and the resulting changes in heart function that one can view on an ‘echocardiogram’ in the same group of patients. An ‘echocardiogram’ is a scanner used to observe the way in which the heart functions, from ultrasound waves ‘bouncing’ off the heart. It can view the structure of the heart and vessels, as well as blood flow. In SCD the heart has to pump more blood through the lungs in order to deliver enough oxygen to the tissues.

      Adults with SCD were assessed using CT, echocardiography, and other lung function tests such as lung capacity, between the years 2009-2013. This same group of adults had previously been shown to have lung changes on CT scans between 2003-2005.

      Whilst there was a large variety in the lung function of the 28 patients with altered lung features, it was demonstrated that lung structure changes seen on CT scans was related to the patients’ decline in lung function, and changes in the function of the heart displayed on echocardiogram tests. Importantly, the results of the study suggest that some of the changes found in the blood vessels between the heart and lungs may be able to explain the differences in the lungs found on CT scan and the decline in lung function. The results of this study help us to understand the complex relationships between heart, lung and blood vessel function in SCD.

      This summary was produced by David Launer, Year 12 student from JFS School, London, as part of the investigators' departmental outreach programme.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Oct 05, Christopher Southan commented:

      This overview would be much more useful if the authors surfaced the 1543 molecular entites via PubChem CIDs, including the previous seven therapeutic splits.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Oct 21, Emmanouil Giorgakis commented:

      Interesting findings; however, the conclusions should be treated with caution. Drain Fluid Amylase (DFA) levels are function of both serum fluid amylase ( SFA) levels and the presence of a leak.. It would arguably be useful to introduce SFA levels into the equation and measure DFA/ SFA ratios rather than the absolute DFA levels: not only because a DFA>=100 would be clinically irrelevant if SFA levels were of the same range, but also because a DFA/SFA ratio is a figure independent of measuring units, thus applicable in various laboratory settings. Secondly, various authors have challenged the clinical usefulness of the biochemical diagnosis of an International Study Group on Pancreatic Fistula (ISGPF) Grade A Pancreatic Fistula ( PF), since such fistulae are transient and devoid of clinical sequealae or deviations in the clinical management. Furthermore, Cochrane meta-analysis published by Koti et. al. in 2010 demonstrated that the risk of PF is lower with the use of somatostatin analogues (SA), without this affecting the mortality rates. It would thus be crucial to clarify whether SA had been used perioperatively -and before a diagnosis of an established pancreatic leak was made-, on which case the outcome of the multivariate analysis and DFA1 cutoff should had been controlled for this variable among the rest. Thank you


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jul 24, Ryan Radecki commented:

      "The Broken ED Sepsis Quality Measure"

      Are there yet sufficient mandates in the Emergency Department? Door-to-physician times, door-to-CT time in acute ischemic stroke, door-to-analgesia for long bone fractures – and, on the horizon, National Quality Forum proposed measures for delivery of sepsis bundle components within 3 and 6 hours....

      http://www.emlitofnote.com/2014/07/the-broken-ed-sepsis-quality-measure.html


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jun 12, Gaetano Santulli commented:

      Goldenberg et al. (1) report that cardiac resynchronization therapy with a defibrillator (CRT-D) is associated with a significant long-term survival benefit in patients with mild heart failure, left ventricular dysfunction, and left bundle-branch block (LBB), whereas no benefit was found in patients without LBB. The Authors, however, do not provide data elaborating whether CRT-D improved any measurable variable of mechanical dyssynchrony. Therefore it is unclear whether the long-term clinical failure of CRT-D observed in patients without LBB resulted from persistent mechanical dyssynchrony or other underlying mechanisms, including myocardial fibrosis and myocardial stress (2). This aspect is particularly relevant since CRT-D has been demonstrated to primarily improve electrical dyssynchrony but does not uniformly decrease morbidity and mortality, even in patients with a wide QRS complex (3). Moreover, given the issues recently raised regarding the importance of QRS measurements, the Authors should have provided more data on the methodology used to measure QRS duration (4).

      References

      1.Goldenberg I, Kutyifa V, Klein HU, et al. Survival with Cardiac-Resynchronization Therapy in Mild Heart Failure. The New England journal of medicine 2014;370:1694-701.
      
      2.Bilchick KC, Kuruvilla S, Hamirani Y, et al. Impact of Mechanical Activation, Scar, and Electrical Timing on Cardiac Resynchronization Therapy Response and Clinical Outcomes. Journal of the American College of Cardiology 2014;63:1657-66.
      
      3.Kass DA. Predicting cardiac resynchronization response by QRS duration: the long and short of it. Journal of the American College of Cardiology 2003;42:2125-7.
      
      4.De Guillebon M, Thambo JB, Ploux S, et al. Reliability and reproducibility of QRS duration in the selection of candidates for cardiac resynchronization therapy. Journal of cardiovascular electrophysiology 2010;21:890-2.
      

      Celestino Sardu, MD¹, Gaetano Santulli, MD, PhD²

      ¹Second University of Naples, Naples, Italy; ²Columbia University Medical Center, New York, NY, USA


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jun 05, Swapnil Hiremath commented:

      This article was discussed on May 27th 2014 on the open online nephrology journal club, #NephJC, on twitter. An introductory comment is available on the eAJKD blog here and also on the NephJC website. It was quite a spirited discussion, with participation from nephrologists, cardiologists, electrophysiogists and more. A transcript and a storify'd version of the tweetchat is available at the same NephJC link and also from the Nephrology-on-demand editor here.

      On June 3rd, there was a Google Hangout on air, between the NephJC editors, Dr John Mandrola and Dr George Bakris, one of the principal investigators of the trial,which can be viewed on Youtube.

      The highlights of the tweetchat were: 1. Symplicity HTN 3 was a great study and the investigators and sponsors were to be commended for carrying out this astutely designed trial, which overcame issues related to regression to the mean, placebo effect and Hawthorne effect. 2. There were some intriguing signals, such as the differential effect based on race, the role of aldosterone antagonists, and technical aspects which should be pursued in future studies. 3. Renal denervation remains an exciting innovation, though carefully designed studies, with emphasis on patient selection, mechanistic and technical aspects and long term safety and efficacy need to be conducted to help delineate the exact role of renal denervation will be in the therapy of hypertension.

      Interested individuals can track and join in the conversation by following @NephJC or #NephJC, or visit the webpage at www.NephJC.com.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Jan 03, M Mangan commented:

      The claims in this work have become the basis for a serious misinterpretation of the incidence of autism that is flying around social media at this time. A couple of medical doctors have weighed in on the claims, and their detailed comments can be found here: Glyphosate – The New Bogeyman and Oh, no! GMOs are going to make everyone autistic!.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Dec 29, Jeffrey Beall commented:

      None


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 26, Guillaume Filion commented:

      This article is one of the "CISCOM meta-analyses", which are very similar papers written by different authors. For more information about the CISCOM meta-analyses, check the blog post "A flurry of copycast on PubMed" at the following link http://blog.thegrandlocus.com/2014/10/a-flurry-of-copycats-on-pubmed


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 14, André R R Freitas commented:

      This paper is very good, clearly identifies the risk of introduction of Chikungunya in America. In my view the endemic transmission of Chikungunya in Brazil and the rest of the Americas is a matter of time, too little time. So the vaccine against dengue should not be considered as a relief to the programs used to control Aedes ssp.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 May 05, J. Andrew Pruszynski commented:

      There are two small typos in the paper. The abstract should state that arm muscles and neurons showed goal-dependent responses at ~70 and 35ms, respectively (rather than the other way around). The methods of the Spatial Target Perturbation should state that the monkey received water reward if it returned its hand to the displayed goal target within 500 ms of perturbation onset and remained within it for another 700 ms (rather than returning to the central target area within 750 ms and remaining within it for another 1 s).


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Sep 01, Wenyi Wei commented:

      We would like to make a correction for the author entry of our paper (PMID: 24670654) by adding the middle name "C." for Dr. Lewis C. Cantley.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 May 06, Madhusudana Girija Sanal commented:

      1) The authors argue that the effects of VSG(vertical sleeve gastrectomy) are mediated through FXR but do not provide this VITAL data in THIS paper on bile acid/salt plasma levels before and after the procedure in wild and FXR-KO animals. Bile salts/acids are FXR ligands. 2) The authors correlate the improvement in glucose tolerance and other beneficial effects to changes in gut microflora especially Roseburia (which is associated with butyrate levels). However, I doubt, the data in Extended Data Table -1 does not support this. The highest level of butyrate is in KO-vertical sleeve gastrectomy (KO-VSG) 29.79 microM/g. 3) Do authors have some hypothesis on butyrate levels and improvement in GTT? 4) Fig-5-G It is not clear how the relative abundance is plotted (X-axis) and if it is significant. Compare, with the data in Extended Data Figure 4<br> 5) Antibiotics were given to the animals during surgery? What effect it had on the gut microbiota? A control for anitbiotics is essential in this experiment. 6) Materials and Methods: It is not clear how the animals were monitored during the entire period and how the data was collected with respect to food preference. I was not able to find the actual data in the paper.<br> 7) What was the eating pattern? What if the KO eats much less food all time-a very basal level so a vertical sleeve surgery do not have much effect on eating? 8) If mediated through FXR, treatment with agonist/antagonist in wild animals would have shown some effect. Can administration of INT-747 –FXR agonist nullify/accentuate the effect of VSG in wild animals? What about DDAH Expression? 9) Explanation for associated with a 24% increase in fasting blood glucose in KO-mice? 10) The authors also ignored the endocrine role of resected gastric mucosa-gastric hormone measurement should have been done.

      By and large this paper is description of observations of VSG in FXR-KO mouse rather than elucidation of the mechanisms.

      According to the authors, they found the association of VSG and FXR through ‘unbiased’ pathway analysis. However, these is a paper by Mencarelli A1, Renga B, D'Amore C, et al. (2013) which correlates the beneficial effects of bariatric surgeries to the selective activation of intestinal nuclear receptors (FXR,LXR). This key paper (perhaps the first paper correlating bariatric surgery to nuclear receptors) is not cited by the authors in the current paper. There is no doubt that this is an area we are still in dark when it comes to the mechanisms. Research in this area will answer many important questions. For example: Some recent studies found the beneficial effects of bariatric surgery is more than medical management –however it is not clear if bariatric surgery can be recommended for non-obese diabetic. Will an FXR modulator will bestow at least some of the positive effects which are currently provided by painful and risky procedures like VSG?


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 May 06, Eric Johnson commented:

      The authors make a comparison between RAD and ddRAD and present the higher read coverage at each locus as a favorable feature of ddRAD, but the comparison is not useful. The ddRAD protocol they chose yielded 26,891 ddRAD loci and a per locus depth of 111 for the 3M reads. The RAD-Seq protocol they choose yielded 206,841 loci and a per locus depth of 14 for the same amount of reads.

      The protocols could have been easily changed to make the loci number even by merely changing the RAD protocol to use a less-frequently cutting enzyme such as SbfI, which is one of the more common choices in RAD projects because it creates fewer loci needing sequencing. Reducing the RAD loci number would have increased the read depth at each locus.

      The only conclusion a reader should draw from this comparison is that sequencing more loci will reduce the read depth at the loci, and that an appropriate enzyme should be chosen for the number of reads planned.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Mar 18, Arvind Babu Rajendra Santosh commented:

      Dear Author: Greetings, Could you please guide us in metastatic gene markers for oral squamous cell carcinoma tissues. Please respond us with literature support on the markers and methodology for the detection. Sincerely yours with respect, Dr A Babu Santosh.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Apr 09, Paola Capparelli Pettersen commented:

      It's stated that the mammography screening in Denmark and Norway is from 50 to 69 years old, but in the conclusion the authors say that the mortality has increased in women older than 59 years not invited to screening.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jun 02, Melanie Courtot commented:

      We are grateful to Drs. Botsis, Woo and Ball for their comment on our article, and the opportunity to address their questions. We entirely agree on the importance of improving automated analysis of safety data, and we would have welcomed the opportunity to reuse their data. However, our multiple FOIA requests, beginning in May 2012, to obtain the complete datasets from the multiple published works to allow us to directly compare our approach with theirs were unsuccessful. Our correspondence eventually culminated in May 2013 with a response that “After a thorough and diligent investigation, CBERs search did not locate a record that contains the MedDRA terms for the VAERS identification numbers that were the subject of this paper or any document responsive to your request for the list of those records that are the 100 confirmed anaphylaxis cases”. This may cause slight differences in the exact numerical values being compared, and where applicable we attempted to infer possible causes, as stated in [Courtot M, 2014]: “However details of the original analysis approach necessary for reproducing the original results were not made available and we could only hypothesize the cause of results we obtained that were not in concordance with the original publication.” We also added specific mentions of differences (for example, in the legend of Table 2) when we were able to form such a hypothesis. Due to size restriction on this response, we address below the points in the order raised. A version including text from the original comment is available online.

      We hope this clarifies the issues raised. We encourage Botsis et al., as well as other interested parties, to release their code and data upon publication as we did and in accordance with PLoS’s Data Policy. The availability of the dataset supporting published results will increase reproducibility or research and foster scientific advances. It will also prevent the need to form hypotheses when trying to interpret existing work, which may be detrimental to interpretation of the scientific content.

      Detailed comments:

      (1)The total number of potentially positive cases used in [Courtot M, 2014] is 237; this can be verified in the data we published alongside the paper. Table 2 contains information from 2 sources, as mentioned in its legend: (1) values taken from the existing published work from Botsis et al. [Botsis T, 2013], and identified by an asterisk, and (2) values obtained through our own analysis. The former, from [Botsis T, 2013], are the values from the testing set (as mentioned in the legend); it would be of little interest to compare results obtained from the training set. The latter, from our own analysis [Courtot M, 2014], encompass results from the Ontology Classification as well as from the Expanded SMQ. Regarding the Ontology Classification, the method does not use a training phase to compute the classifier results and so there is no basis on which to make the split. Similarly, there is no training done with the ABC tool. We hypothesized that there was a desire in [Botsis T, 2013] to keep the approach consistent across methods used and consequently acknowledge in the legend of the table that this may result in a small difference for the ABC classification row. With respect to the Expanded SMQ results, the table includes the results on the whole dataset. However the text does provide additional information: “Similar results were obtained using a 50/50 training/testing data split: 92% sensitivity (8696% at 95% CI) and 81% specificity (80-82% at 95% CI) in the testing set, AUC 0.93 (0.9-0.95 at 95% CI).”

      (2) The original paper [Botsis T, 2013] did not document any deviation of the MedDRA terms used to the standard. The dataset used in our analysis (including the MedDRA terms) is published online. While it would certainly have been of interest to be able to compare our results with the set of MedDRA terms used by Botsis et al., answers to our FOIA requests stated that “no such record were located”.

      (3) We provided details about our cosine similarity method and cited the original paper in which the method is described. We indicate that we obtained similar results on the whole dataset as with training/testing split, as we mentioned in point (1) above. In both cases, a cosine similarity score was computed for each pair of vectors (query and vector of PTs from the specific report considered) and the best cut-off point was determined. Regarding the use of additional PTs, we do not agree with the commentators’ assessment that that the additional PTs were commonly reported or unrelated to anaphylaxis. Indeed, the very first term in the list of additional PTs we suggest and provide as supplementary material for review is Hypersensitivity, which we do consider to be related to anaphylaxis. Specifically, the SMQ for anaphylaxis contains the term “type 1 hypersensitivity”. The next 4 PTs in Table S1 are already included in the SMQ; the following one is “pharyngeal oedema”. The SMQ already lists “oedema mouth”, “oropharyngeal swelling”, “laryngotracheal oedema” etc. and it seems appropriate to assume that “pharyngeal oedema” is related.

      (4)(a) Using a statistical correlation between MedDRA PTs and outcome is an original contribution made by Courtot et al. in [Courtot M, 2014] and clearly described as such when constructing the Expanded SMQ (and supported by Table S1 in appendix). It was never implied that this had been done in [Botsis T, 2013]. We believe the referred-to excerpt is “Rather than creating a bag of words de novo based on keyword extraction from a training set of reports, we rather chose to expand on a known, already widely implemented, screening method, i.e., the SMQs.” We did not intend to refer to Botsis et al.’s work - we use the word “we”, and expected readers would understand that this referred to the authors of the manuscript [Courtot M, 2014]. We apologize for any confusion.

      (4)(b) We are aware that Botsis et al. used the online version of the tool, and that the tool does not require the existence of a tentative diagnosis when data is submitted. However, The ABC tool is a diagnosis confirmation tool based on the Brighton guidelines, and the authors of that tool assume that instructions given with the guideline are being followed. Note that the tool itself is labeled “ANALYZE DATABASE Confirm a diagnosis for all cases in your database (Excel spreadsheet)” on the Brighton Collaboration website. We hypothesized that the diagnosis was automatically pre-selected for the batch entry done by Botsis et al. As a result, we emphasize a discrepancy between the information reported as ‘Insufficient evidence’ by Botsis et al, and what is expected based on the Brighton Case definition: ‘Reported anaphylaxis with insufficient evidence’ [Rüggeberg JU, 2007]. In [Botsis T, 2013], 488 cases are classified as ‘Insufficient evidence’. With the guideline in mind, we reviewed the records and showed that only 3 reports should have been classified as “Reported anaphylaxis with insufficient evidence”. Indeed, only 12 reports in the VAERS dataset were reported as anaphylaxis (and corresponding synonyms) in the VAERS report itself, of which 3 do not meet the Brighton case definition. Our results have been communicated to the Brighton Collaboration, with the aim of emphasizing the process for those users of the ABC tool who may not have a full understanding of the logic of the Brighton guideline.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 May 08, Taxiarchis Botsis commented:

      Commentary on article “The Logic of Surveillance Guidelines: An Analysis of Vaccine Adverse Event Reports from an Ontological Perspective” by Mélanie Courtot, Ryan R. Brinkman and Alan Ruttenberg.

      Taxiarchis Botsis<sup>1</sup> , Emily Jane Woo<sup>1</sup> , Robert Ball<sup>1</sup>

      <sup>1</sup> Office of Biostatistics and Epidemiology, Center for Biologics Evaluation and Research (CBER), Food and Drug Administration (FDA), Rockville, MD

      In their recent work, we were pleased to see Courtot et al. [Courtot M, 2014] pursue a line of research we initiated [Botsis T, 2011, Botsis T, 2012, Botsis T, 2013, Botsis T, 2013] to improve the efficiency of the application of case definitions to spontaneous reports of adverse events following immunization reported to the US Vaccine Adverse Event Reporting System (VAERS). VAERS is a spontaneous reporting system co-managed by the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) and Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). Courtot et al. described the development and evaluation of an adverse event ontology and proposed using it to represent the Brighton Collaboration (BC) case definitions. Their work is based on data sets consisting of “possible anaphylaxis” reports identified by FDA VAERS review staff and used in our research [Botsis T, 2011, Botsis T, 2013]. Using data obtained from the FDA through a series of Freedom of Information Act (FOIA) requests, the authors evaluated their system using the BC case definition for anaphylaxis. However, we have some concerns regarding the methods that Courtot et al. used to compare their results with ours [Botsis T, 2011, Botsis T, 2013], and we believe some clarification on the part of the authors would be helpful. First, in Table 2 of their results, Courtot et al. present a comparison of the sensitivity, specificity and AUC of their methods with those previously published by us [Botsis T, 2013]. The values taken for comparison from our report [Botsis T, 2013] were based on a randomly selected 25% testing subset. The authors’ results appear to be based on the entire set of 6034 reports for H1N1 vaccine previously classified by the FDA VAERS review staff as either “possible anaphylaxis” (N=237, not 236 as reported by Courtot et al.) or not “possible anaphylaxis” since they do not state otherwise. This issue is important to understand because the comparison presented in Table 2 of Courtot et al. might not be based on the same data. Second, we are concerned about the source of the Medical Dictionary for Regulatory Activities (MedDRA) Preferred Terms (PTs) used in all the analyses because these were not provided in the FOIA responses. While it is possible to obtain MedDRA PTs from public data, these PTs would differ from those used in our analysis because of constraints on the number of PTs available in the public files. It might also be possible to convert the narrative text to PTs, but doing so would certainly produce a different set from ours [Botsis T, 2013]. Both of these differences could explain and contribute to variability in the comparative results.<br> Third, Courtot et al. neither explained whether they used a training/testing split when developing and then testing the expanded Standardized MedDRA Query (SMQ), nor exactly how cosine similarity was calculated for this purpose. They suggest the use of an expanded SMQ by selecting the PTs that are significantly correlated with the outcome and adding 120 new PTs to the original anaphylaxis SMQ. Details about the cosine similarity calculations are not provided, but it appears that Courtot et al. are using the expanded SMQ to perform a cosine similarity based classification on the same set used for estimating the correlations; such an approach could explain the high sensitivity and might not be reproducible if applied prospectively to another data set. Additionally, regarding the established and validated SMQ for anaphylaxis, it seems paradoxical that the addition of PTs which are commonly reported and unrelated to anaphylaxis would increase its performance. Clarification would be helpful. Fourth, the subsection “Automated Case Screening” and the discussion for the appropriate use of Automatic Brighton Classification (ABC) tool contain a number of misinterpretations for the work performed in [Botsis T, 2013]. With regard to the “Automated Case Screening” subsection, we would like to clarify two main points. First, we did not use the statistical correlation between the MedDRA PTs and the outcome (i.e., the “potentially positive” label) as implied by Courtot et al. We used the cosine similarity scores of the report vectors in the training subset to define the best cutoff point for the classification of reports and further evaluated it with the scores in the testing subset. Second, we used only MedDRA PTs in that analysis; we did not follow a “bag of words” approach and the terms were not obtained “based on keyword extraction” as stated by Courtot et al. With regard to the ABC tool, we used the online version of the tool that allows the processing of an appropriately formatted database of reports. While certain fields of the database must be marked as present (‘yes’), absent (‘no’), or even remain blank, the existence of a tentative diagnosis is not necessary to perform the classification. The online ABC tool processes the database and assigns one of the following labels to a report: “Level 1”, “Level 2”, “Level 3”, “Not a case”, “Insufficient evidence”, and “No evidence”. As previously stated [Botsis T, 2013], we prepared the database of 6034 reports to allow their automated classification by the ABC tool without human intervention or further interpretation of either the classification process or the labels produced by the ABC tool; this analysis was performed in collaboration with the ABC tool developer at the Brighton Collaboration. The development of advanced methodologies for the automated processing of safety surveillance data is important considering the large number of reports submitted to the FDA and other public health authorities. We welcome the reuse of our data for serious and constructive research, as well as the exploration of other methodologies that may offer efficient, effective, and rigorous processing of our data. We encourage the authors of this study and any other researchers to follow up with queries about our work, and we welcome them to maintain ongoing communication to ensure the most appropriate use of the data and avoid misinterpretations that might reduce the usefulness of the work and its potential application.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 14, Wilma Waterlander commented:

      Thank you Andrew for your comment. My apologies that we didn't refer to this paper in our article. An important difference between this paper and ours however is that the tax in this study was added to various unhealthier items and not exclusively on soft drinks (as in our paper). The effects are therefore likely to be different, in particular when you look at substitution effects. Would be great to see more experimental studies on this topic!


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Apr 03, Andrew Brown commented:

      This statement is incorrect: "However, there have been no experimental studies of the effects of price increases on SSBs or the effects on close substitutes such as diet drinks, alcohol or sugary snacks." See: "From Coke to Coors: A Field Study of a Sugar-Sweetened Beverage Tax and its Unintended Consequences" (http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2079840)


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 May 21, David Keller commented:

      Your wish may have come true!

      The latest study concludes "Among smokers who have attempted to stop without professional support, those who use e-cigarettes are more likely to report continued abstinence than those who used a licensed NRT product bought over-the-counter or no aid to cessation. This difference persists after adjusting for a range of smoker characteristics such as nicotine dependence."(1)

      Would you please write an editorial entitled "If only there were a cure for Parkinson's disease"? I would personally appreciate that, and although it seems unscientific, your editorials seem to get results in a remarkably quick fashion ;-)

      Reference

      1: Brown J, et al. Real-world effectiveness of e-cigarettes when used to aid smoking cessation: a cross-sectional population study. Addiction. Pre-publication online at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/add.12623/abstract


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 May 09, David Keller commented:

      Let the bad be the enemy of the truly horrendous

      It has been estimated that “up to 98% of tobacco-related deaths are attributable to combustible products"(1). E-cigarettes deliver nicotine to the lungs as a cooler vapor, without tobacco combustion, or its byproducts such as carbon monoxide, carcinogenic tars and hot smoke. While nicotine is an addictive drug known to increase the risk of heart attacks, the argument for making e-cigarettes available to smokers is that e-cigarettes are predicted to cause fewer lung cancers, due to the absence of hot tobacco combustion products in their vapors. In addition, e-cigarette users are not inhaling the carbon monoxide found in cigarette smoke, which contributes to coronary ischemia among other ill effects. Even if the use of e-cigarettes does not cut down the number of cigarettes smoked, at least we know that when a chain-smoker is inhaling from an e-cigarette at a given moment, he is not also smoking a regular cigarette at that same time. Smoking causes lung cancer and heart attacks, while e-cigarettes cause "only" the latter. Using e-cigarettes is a bad choice, but this is a case of letting the bad be the enemy of the truly horrendous. I advise my patients neither to smoke nor to e-smoke, but if I had to pick which vice is more harmful, I would say the former. For nicotine addicts who prefer the inhaled route of delivery, the FDA has approved the use of Nicotrol, a nicotine inhaler which does not have the flavorings and other questionable ingredients included in e-cigarettes, and which is probably the least bad choice.

      Reference

      1: Fiore MC, Schroeder SA, Baker TB. Smoke, the chief killer--strategies for targeting combustible tobacco use. N Engl J Med. 2014 Jan 23;370(4):297-9. doi:10.1056/NEJMp1314942 PubMed PMID: 24450888


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 May 13, David Keller commented:

      Call for the patent holders of exenatide to help advance Parkinson's research & also profit

      Follow-up data gathered a year after the end of the original study strengthens the suggestion that exenatide may have slowed down the progression of Parkinson's disease (PD). Think how much more convincing these results would be if exenatide had been tested in a double-blinded and placebo-controlled fashion. The authors of the original study wrote that the reason they conducted an open label study with no placebo was that they were unable to procure a suitable placebo, which would have required the manufacturer of Byetta (exenatide) to supply the pen injector devices filled with saline solution.

      A recent meta-analysis found that Parkinson patients treated with active drug had significantly less objectively measured motor response to the same intervention if they knew there was a chance that they were receiving placebo, even with effective double blinding (1). This decreased response, dubbed "the lessebo effect", could account for up to 4.1 UPDRS units of motor improvement for patients in studies lacking double-blinded placebo controls.

      Astra-Zeneca and Bristol-Myers-Squibb should supply these and other scientists with placebo Byetta or Bydureon injector devices, filled with sterile normal saline, to facilitate further research. This would help to advance investigation of exenatide as possibly the first proven disease-modifying medication for PD, which would be of tremendous benefit not only for PD patients, but for the stock-holders of the companies which own the patents on exenatide. This is truly a chance for these companies to do well while doing good.

      Reference

      1) Mestre TA, Shah P, Marras C, Tomlinson G, Lang AE. Another face of placebo: The lessebo effect in Parkinson disease: Meta-analyses. Neurology. 2014 Apr 22;82(16):1402-9. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000340. Epub 2014 Mar 21. PubMed PMID: 24658930; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4001195.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 09, David Keller commented:

      Let the patient decide when to start medication for Parkinson disease

      Reluctance to start medication is not a critical problem for patients with Parkinson disease (PD), because no medication has been proven to affect the underlying progression of PD. Patient "non-compliance" with the use of PD medications does not have the kind of downstream harms associated with non-compliance with statins or aspirin in patients with atherosclerotic vascular disease. On the other hand, delaying the use of levodopa is associated with delayed motor complications, such as dyskinesias. Patients should not be encouraged to take Parkinson medications unless they are needed, and the patient decides when they are needed, based on the way they feel, not the physician.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 May 17, Mark Rubin commented:

      Check out also: Rubin, M., Badea, C., & Jetten, J. (2014). Low status groups show in-group favoritism to compensate for their low status and to compete for higher status. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations. doi: 10.1177/1368430213514122


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 26, Guillaume Filion commented:

      This article is one of the "CISCOM meta-analyses", which are very similar papers written by different authors. For more information about the CISCOM meta-analyses, check the blog post "A flurry of copycast on PubMed" at the following link http://blog.thegrandlocus.com/2014/10/a-flurry-of-copycats-on-pubmed


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 May 19, David Keller commented:

      Was this "Lessebo" effect durable for one year after the end of a clinical trial?

      A recent clinical trial demonstrated possible beneficial effects of exenatide on slowing the progression of Parkinson disease (1); however, this study was hampered by lack of a double-blinded placebo control arm. Instead, a usual-treatment group was followed passively over time to serve as a comparator. Clearly, some of the benefits seen in the patients who self-injected with exenatide daily could have been due to the "Lessebo" effect during that study, or shortly afterward. But durable benefits were measured in the exenatide intervention group a full year after their last self-injection. How durable is the "Lessebo" effect? Could it account for some or all of the benefit seen with exenatide a full year after the end of that study?

      Reference

      1: Aviles-Olmos I, Dickson J, Kefalopoulou Z, Djamshidian A, Kahan J, Fmedsci PE, Whitton P, Wyse R, Isaacs T, Lees A, Limousin P, Foltynie T. Motor and Cognitive Advantages Persist 12 Months After Exenatide Exposure in Parkinson's Disease. J Parkinsons Dis. 2014 Mar 24. [Epub ahead of print] PubMed PMID: 24662192.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Jan 20, E DAVID KLONSKY commented:

      MMA Fighters Experience Less Head Trauma Compared to Hockey and Football: A Re-Analysis of Data in Hutchison et al. (2014)

      E. David Klonsky PhD, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia

      Hutchison et al. (2014) present the most detailed and authoritative analysis to date of head trauma in mixed martial arts (MMA) competitions. The study is a valuable contribution to the literature on the frequency and predictors of head trauma in combat sports. This comment was written to suggest a reinterpretation of a particular aspect of the paper.

      Hutchison et al. estimate the number of concussions that occur per 100 ‘athlete exposures’ in MMA, and compare their finding to data from other sports, including hockey and football. The figures reported are: 15.9 per 100 exposures for MMA, 8.8 for football, and 2.2 for hockey. Reported this way, the numbers imply that participating in MMA results in significantly more head trauma than hockey or football.

      However, the authors’ choice to standardize concussion figures per 100 athlete exposures overlooks a key consideration: it is not only the rate per exposure that matters, but the sheer number of exposures. It is significant, then, that the number of exposures per year and per career varies systematically across sports.

      Consider that a National Hockey League (NHL) regular season consists of 82 games, and that a National Football League (NFL) regular season consists of 16 games. In contrast, MMA fighters average approximately 3 fights per year. If we adjust the Hutchison et al. figures for number of exposures per year, concussion rates become highest for hockey and lowest for MMA: 1.80 for hockey, 1.41 for football, and 0.48 for MMA. In other words, a typical year of competition in professional football or hockey results in 3 to 4 times more concussions than a typical year of competition in MMA.

      It is also important to consider head trauma experienced over the course of a career. To do so we must first consider the number of career exposures typical for each sport. As a rough barometer of career exposures, let us consider players recently inducted into each sport’s Hall of Fame. The four 2014 NHL player inductees competed in an average of 1201 career games. The seven 2014 NFL player inductees competed in an average of 206 career games. In contrast, the last four fighters to be inducted into the UFC Hall of Fame competed in an average of 33 career matches.

      Notice the massive discrepancy in average career exposures across sports: 1201 (hockey) vs. 206 (football) vs. 33 (MMA).

      If we use these differences in career exposures to adjust the concussion figures reported by Hutchison et al., we get the following: 26.4 for hockey, 18.1 for football, and 5.25 for MMA. In short, the concussion rate for an MMA career is 3 to 5 times lower than that of a hockey or football career.

      This analysis, like the Hutchison et al. analysis, has limitations. First, the Hutchison et al. figures for MMA concussions were based on official bout results and video analysis rather than medical diagnosis. Second, the figures for hockey and football cited by Hutchison et al. were taken from previous studies using different methodologies and sampling procedures. Third, my analysis focuses on professional sports, which have longer seasons (i.e., more exposures) than sports at collegiate, high-school, or recreational levels. Fourth, I roughly rather than precisely estimated the average numbers of career exposures. Finally, a full understanding of head trauma risk in sports requires data on concussions that occur during practice and training, not just competition.

      In sum, understanding the risks of head trauma in sports is critical. Some have been quick to associate MMA with disproportionately high risk of head trauma compared to other sports. However, when the stakes are high, it is important to get the science right. And the available data suggest that professional MMA fighters will experience less, not more, head trauma compared to professionals competing in hockey and football.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jun 27, Serge Ahmed commented:

      The authors propose to reclassify addictive drugs, including cocaine, according to their common property of directly evoking a high frequency of de novo dopamine transients that would surpass that normally evoked by “natural” rewards. This property is particularly obvious in awake and behaving rats. This property would be necessary and sufficient to explain why “a higher reward magnitude [would be] conferred to abused drugs compared to natural rewards” and why “drug-associated cues [would acquire] the powerful ability to reinstate drug seeking and taking”.

      This is an interesting hypothesis but it totally ignores recent research showing that when rats have a choice between i.v. cocaine and sweet water (a nondrug reward which is “processed by sensory systems”), most rats prefer the latter over the former regardless of the dose of cocaine available and even after a long history of cocaine self-administration (e.g., http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24260227; http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22871910; http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23551949; http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24886157; http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24602027; http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20676364; http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17668074; but see: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23319458). Thus, it seems that the ability of a drug, like cocaine, to directly evoke a high frequency of de novo dopamine transients is not sufficient to explain its addictive potential (see also: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23428657).


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 11, Marcus Peter commented:

      None


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Apr 07, Iffat Sumia commented:

      Could CD95 then be a dependence receptor? (a kind of receptor that, when deprived of its ligand, stops its normal function and triggers cell death - Goldschneider D, 2010)


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 26, Guillaume Filion commented:

      This article is one of the "CISCOM meta-analyses", which are very similar papers written by different authors. For more information about the CISCOM meta-analyses, check the blog post "A flurry of copycast on PubMed" at the following link http://blog.thegrandlocus.com/2014/10/a-flurry-of-copycats-on-pubmed


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Mar 28, Sergio Stagnaro commented:

      None


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Mar 28, Daniel Schwartz commented:

      This is a useful review that focuses on the actionable interventions that, based on current knowledge, could reduce the incidence of breast cancer. http://qxmd.com/r/24647877


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2014 Mar 25, GRAHAM COLDITZ commented:

      More resource materials in support of the prevention messages in this manuscript are available at http://tinyurl.com/BCpriorityExtra


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 02, Daniel J Simons commented:

      I wrote an extensive post-publication "HI-BAR" review of this paper on my blog (stands for Had I Been A Reviewer). You can access it at http://blog.dansimons.com/2014/04/hi-bar-benefits-of-lumosity-training.html

      I posted a list of my concerns about the paper as a comment on the article at PLoS, and I've duplicated that list below. The blog post gives a more detailed discussion and explanation of each point. If the authors respond on PLoS, I'll update my comment and add a link to their response here as well.

      In short, I do not think the paper permits the conclusion that game training produced any reliable benefits on the reported outcome measure.

      List of questions and concerns:

      1) The sample size of 15 in the training group and 12 in the control group is problematically small, especially for correlational analysis, but also for the primary analyses.

      2) The "limited-contact" control group does not permit an inference that anything specific to the training led to the transfer effects. See http://pps.sagepub.com/content/8/4/445.full

      3) The paper includes no corrections for multiple tests, and the core findings likely would not be significant with correction.

      4) The paper does not report the means and variability for the accuracy data, leaving open the possibility of a speed-accuracy tradeoff.

      5) The choice of response time cutoffs and exclusions were somewhat arbitrary, so it's not clear how robust these effects would be to other cutoffs.

      6) The contrasts used to measure alertness and distraction were not defined. Which conditions were compared?

      7) The alertness and distraction tests do not include a test of the difference between the training and control group. The fact that the training group difference was significant (but see below) and the control group difference was not does not mean that the difference between the groups was significant.

      8) The training improvements for the alertness and distraction outcome measures were reported to be p=.05 and p=.04. But, they were truncated from p=.0565 and p=.0451. The first was not significant, and truncating the p-values is inappropriate. (Note that neither would be significant after correcting for multiple tests.)

      9) The paper reports 20 correlations (each outcome measure with each of the 10 games in the training condition), but does not correct for multiple tests. And, correlations based on N=15 are of questionable reliability anyway. Moreover, correlations between training improvements and improvements on an outcome measure do not provide evidence for the efficacy of training.

      10) The conclusion claims support for the idea that training improved "attention filtering," but the study does not test the mechanism that improved (and, the evidence that anything improved is uncertain).

      11) The clinicaltrials.gov registration linked from the paper was posted after the paper was first submitted for publication. It is not a pre-registration.

      12) The clinicaltrials.gov registration mentions a number of outcome measures that were not reported in the paper and were not mentioned on the PLoS Protocol and Consort Checklist (in the supplementary materials). If these measures were collected, they should be reported in the paper and in the supplemental materials. It is unclear whether these outcome measures just were not significant or were withheld for other reasons. In either case, the presence of unreported outcome measures makes it impossible to interpret the p-values for the one outcome measure reported in the paper.

      13) The clinicaltrials.gov registration also lists a 24-week testing session that wasn't mentioned in the paper. Was the reported testing session an interim one?


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Jan 21, Alberto Carbonell commented:

      Please visit http://p-sams.carringtonlab.org/ for using P-SAMS, the Plant Small RNA Maker Suite, to design artificial microRNAs and synthetic trans-acting siRNAs. P-SAMS also outputs the sequence of the oligonucleotides needed to clone the corresponding small RNA(s) in BsaI/ccdB-based "B/c" vectors.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Oct 28, Salomone Di Saverio commented:

      Thanks for your reply. You have written: "They still believe fibrin sealants may be beneficial in the setting of liver surgery and they criticize our nonsignificant findings." If you read carefully our letter it was wirtten: "Although from our experience we might agree with the authors' conclusions that fibrin sealant use does not significantly influence the incidence and severity of surface-related complications after liver resection, after looking carefully at the data of the present randomized controlled trial, we have several concerns about a good balance between the 2 groups and percentages and statistical significance given in the article. The generalizability of the results may therefore be affected and the conclusions may not be strongly supported by the data." It does mean that we DO agree that fibrin sealant does NOT influence complications and therefore is NOT beneficial, but we had concerns on the good balance between the 2 groups and percentages and statistical significance in this study. Furthermore many differences between groups, although not reaching merely statistical significance, did show a clinically meaningful significance; e.g. the percentage of patients who underwent preoperative chemotherapy less than 3 months before surgery, even counting the original 20% vs 12%, is still really very close to the boundaries of statistical significance (0.059) and maybe almost double of patients between the two groups (30/154 vs 17/148) can be clinically meaningful? Having said that, we confirm once again our agreement that application of fibrin glue sealant after hepatectomy does not seem justified


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 16, Joerg Striessnig commented:

      This is a very valuable review article providing the reader with a recent update of how histone acetylation state can affect specific brain functions in rodents that bear clinically relevance for human therapies.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 04, David Keller commented:

      Move Leftward on MultiTarget's Receiver Operating Curve

      The MultiTarget test has higher sensitivity but lower specificity than FIT, the standard colon cancer fecal screen (1). MultiTarget detects more colon malignancies but triggers more unnecessary colonoscopies. Colonoscopy is expensive and invasive, limiting the acceptability of false-positive fecal screens. MultiTarget’s Composite Score threshold was set at 183. Increasing this threshold score will increase specificity and lower sensitivity (2). It should be raised until MultiTarget’s specificity equals FIT’s; if, at that point, MultiTarget’s sensitivity remains higher than FIT’s, then replacing FIT screening with MultiTarget will detect more malignancies without forcing patients to undergo more unnecessary colonoscopies.

      MultiTarget incorporates fecal hemoglobin testing. Reducing the cofactor X6 in the Logistic Score has the effect of increasing specificity and reducing sensitivity of the fecal hemoglobin component of MultiTarget without affecting the performance of the DNA components of the Composite Score.

      Fecal hemoglobin testing should be eliminated from colon cancer stool screening protocols as DNA tests improve, because bleeding commonly originates from benign sources, such as hemorrhoids, degrading specificity.

      References

      1) Imperiale TF, Ransohoff DF, Itzkowitz SH, Levin TR, Lavin P, Lidgard GP, Ahlquist DA, Berger BM. Multitarget stool DNA testing for colorectal-cancer screening. N Engl J Med. 2014 Apr 3;370(14):1287-97. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1311194. Epub 2014 Mar 19. PubMed PMID: 24645800.

      2) Florkowski CM. Sensitivity, specificity, receiver-operating characteristic (ROC) curves and likelihood ratios: communicating the performance of diagnostic tests. Clin Biochem Rev. 2008 Aug;29 Suppl 1:S83-7. PubMed PMID: 18852864; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2556590.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 04, David Keller commented:

      Move Leftward on MultiTarget's Receiver Operating Curve

      The MultiTarget test has higher sensitivity but lower specificity than FIT, the standard colon cancer fecal screen (1). MultiTarget detects more colon malignancies but triggers more unnecessary colonoscopies. Colonoscopy is expensive and invasive, limiting the acceptability of false-positive fecal screens. MultiTarget’s Composite Score threshold was set at 183. Increasing this threshold score will increase specificity and lower sensitivity (2). It should be raised until MultiTarget’s specificity equals FIT’s; if, at that point, MultiTarget’s sensitivity remains higher than FIT’s, then replacing FIT screening with MultiTarget will detect more malignancies without forcing patients to undergo more unnecessary colonoscopies.

      MultiTarget incorporates fecal hemoglobin testing. Reducing the cofactor X6 in the Logistic Score has the effect of increasing specificity and reducing sensitivity of the fecal hemoglobin component of MultiTarget without affecting the performance of the DNA components of the Composite Score.

      Fecal hemoglobin testing should be eliminated from colon cancer stool screening protocols as DNA tests improve, because bleeding commonly originates from benign sources, such as hemorrhoids, degrading specificity.

      References

      1) Imperiale TF, Ransohoff DF, Itzkowitz SH, Levin TR, Lavin P, Lidgard GP, Ahlquist DA, Berger BM. Multitarget stool DNA testing for colorectal-cancer screening. N Engl J Med. 2014 Apr 3;370(14):1287-97. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1311194. Epub 2014 Mar 19. PubMed PMID: 24645800.

      2) Florkowski CM. Sensitivity, specificity, receiver-operating characteristic (ROC) curves and likelihood ratios: communicating the performance of diagnostic tests. Clin Biochem Rev. 2008 Aug;29 Suppl 1:S83-7. PubMed PMID: 18852864; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2556590.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Apr 03, David Keller commented:

      <div class="comm_header2">PermalinkShare</div>


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2014 Apr 02, David Keller commented:

      We need a colon cancer stool screen which does not equate fecal blood with malignancy

      The new "multitarget stool DNA test" studied in this clinical trial actually includes a hemoglobin immunoassay, so it retains a degree of the inherent inaccuracy caused by equating the presence of fecal blood with the presence of cancer. This new screening test for colon cancer uses a composite score computed from the presence of direct molecular markers of cancer (such as cancer-associated DNA strands), but the score also includes a component from the presence of fecal blood. The fecal blood test component cannot distinguish between blood from a benign source, such as a hemorrhoid, versus bleeding from a malignancy; this tends to increase the false-positive rate of the multitarget test, which was actually worse than that of the standard FIT test used as for comparison.

      Hemorrhoids are common, and their trace bleeding increases the number of benign (unnecessary) colonoscopies which are ordered as a result of screening for fecal hemoglobin. The presence of stool DNA specific for colon cancer will be a convincing argument in favor of colonoscopy for patients who attribute the presence of blood in their stool to their hemorrhoids. The ideal stool screening test would rely purely on detecting cancer-specific molecules, without the need to enhance sensitivity by including a test for fecal blood.

      A Modest Proposal - As a clinician, I would want to see the results of all the individual components of the multitarget stool screening test reported separately, along with the composite score calculated using the complex formula employed in this study. The investigators evidently fine-tuned this formula by altering the constant coefficients applied to each test component, in order to achieve what they hoped would be an optimized combination of sensitivity and specificity. However, whenever several directly-measured quantities are combined into a single number for the purpose of arriving at a binary result, much information is lost. The binary result will be a useful "yes or no" answer to the question of whether a patient requires colonoscopy. But clinicians should also be informed of the individual results of each of the component tests, along with the characteristics and implications of each, to allow a more individualized approach to the care of certain patients.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 07, Gary Collins commented:

      Thanks for the comments.

      The systematic review only included articles published in 2010 in the Core Clinical Journals [Abridged Index Medicus], so articles published in 2007 will not have been eligible, nor will articles published in 2011 onwards. The search string identified a large 11826 potential articles, mainly because of the inclusive search string. Prediction models are not easily identifiable due to inconsistent terminology for such models (e.g. Prognostic models, prediction models/rules, risk scores etc...and many don't even provide this in the title or abstract), no MeSH terms that are consistently used and tagged to such articles etc...

      Also, whilst there are clearly some good examples of external validation studies, the majority are not so well done and are poorly reported, yet validation is all what we are interested in - i.e. does a model actually work.

      It is very unlikely that including more up-to-date articles will have changed our conclusions. However, a reporting guideline is currently under peer-review to improve the reporting of studies developing or validating multivariable prediction models...

      http://blogs.bmj.com/bmj/2011/08/03/gary-collins-opening-up-multivariable-prediction-models/

      They will hopefully been out by the summer.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Apr 06, Shervin Assari commented:

      I wish a file was available to show the 11,826 articles. I am sure I am author of 1 or 2. I wish I could look at the evaluation results related to my work.

      Khedmat H, Karami GR, Pourfarziani V, Assari S, Rezailashkajani M, Naghizadeh MM. A logistic regression model for predicting health-related quality of life in kidney transplant recipients.Transplant Proc. 2007 May;39(4):917-22.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2014 Apr 06, GRAHAM COLDITZ commented:

      A pity this thorough review searched the literature back in February 2011 before our external validation of the Rosner-Colditz log-incidence breast cancer model, using an independent cohort (California Teachers Study) and comparable follow-up time period (calendar years) and endpoints (invasive breast cancer) was reported. see http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10549-013-2719-3

      Overall, this rigorous review reminds us to move beyond just publishing risk models but rather to carefully plan and implement validation studies. This paper should move the field forward and exemplifies the value of systematic review of methods to advance the conduct and reporting of studies.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Mar 16, Cicely Saunders Institute Journal Club commented:

      The Cicely Saunders Institute journal club reviewed this paper on Wednesday 3rd February 2016. We enjoyed discussing it and felt that the authors had addressed an important and understudied area of medical training. On the one hand, it is recognised that junior doctors increasingly need to be able to interact with terminally ill and other vulnerable groups of patients who need specialist palliative care and rehabilitation. On the other hand, there can be a common perception that it is unethical or unnecessary to involve the terminally ill in medical student teaching.

      To investigate this issue in greater depth, the authors’ objective was to review available literature on how terminally ill patients feel about being involved in undergraduate medical teaching. Results were presented on a PRISMA flow chart, and studies appraised using a quality scoring system.

      We felt that the paper would have been improved by the inclusion of clearer aims. The focus on patients’ views was interesting, and we wondered if the authors had considered extending this to include family perspectives.

      Future studies could take the question beyond ‘should we involve terminally ill patients’ to ‘how and when’ it might be more or less acceptable to involve terminally ill patients in undergraduate medical education. Our discussions also considered whether this question could be extended to other life-limiting populations, such as people with complex neurological conditions, and to other cultures, where health systems and beliefs differ from the UK NHS system.

      Commentary by Sophie Pask and Diana Jackson


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Aug 30, Michelle Lin commented:

      A great study showing the potential of age-adjusted d-dimer cutoff values for patients >50 years old. ALiEM-Annals of EM Global EM Journal club held August 2014, which includes a webcast with experts.

      http://www.aliem.com/adjust-pe-study-aliem-annals-em-journal-club/


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Jul 14, Marco Weiergräber commented:

      A comment has been posted on Pubpeer on this publication in Epilepsy Research refused to publish a letter to the editor from my site. The authors use a transmitter out of its technical specifications, i.e. the transmitter bandwidth is 1-50Hz, the nominal sampling rate 250 Hz. Theoretically, frequency reconstruction is possible up to 125 Hz (see Nyquist-Shannon sampling limit). However, the authors analyze up to 500Hz. This is simply not possible and not correct.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Nov 22, Torsten Seemann commented:

      The latest version of Prokka is now available from https://github.com/tseemann/prokka


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Mar 25, Stephen Turner commented:

      The current state of the art pipelines for bacterial genome annotation rely on web-servers with annotation times measuring in hours or days. Furthermore, sensitive data cannot be uploaded to a publicly accessible web-server. In this paper, the author introduces the Prokka software for rapid bacterial genome annotation. Prokka runs on local hardware, and can completely annotate a typical bacterial genome in under 10 minutes on a laptop. Prokka uses several feature prediction tools combined with similarity searching against annotated protein databases (either user-provided or publicly available). Prokka produces standards-compliant output files suitable for downstream analysis or submission to Genbank. Prokka is open-source and freely available.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 26, Salvatore Chirumbolo commented:

      This interesting paper by Marzotto M et al., evaluated gene expression after 24 hrs incubation with hydroalcoholic/water extracts of Gelsemium sempervirens plant. The authors used human neuroblastoma cell lines SH-SY5Y and also IMR32 cell line, to focus on previously published data on mouse behavioural tests. After an oligonucleotide microarray, the authors selected less than 10 genes for RT-PCR. However, using human neuronal cell line to elucidate results obtained with mouse models appears quite disputable. The Authors referred this study to the previously reported behavioural evidence on animals, when stated (Discussion pag 15) “In previous recent trials, Gelsemium s. showed anxiolytic-like effects in mouse emotional response models and appeared to work even at the high dilutions 9c and 30c [26,27]” ....and moreover “In general, the prevalence of down-regulation seems to indicate a tendency to reduce cell excitability, especially because several of the genes in question belong to surface receptors involved in GPCR signaling and calcium homeostasis. Moreover, this first microarray screening of the effects of Gelsemium s. on neurocytes revealed a significant down-regulation of genes for inflammatory response, olfactory transduction and neuron differentiation” (Discussion pag. 16). This may raise criticism, as the authors studied SH-SY5Y and IMR32 human neuroblastoma cell line, not mouse derived cell lines. Yet, several genes the Authors highlighted do not have homologues in mice. Some examples of this are reported as follows. The gene baculoviral IAP repeat containing 8 (BIRC 8) has no homologues in mice (only orthologue genes in Pan troglodytes), olfactory gene OR4X1 is not expressed (absent) in mouse, gene C1ORF167 has a non characterized orthologue gene LOC102634746 in mouse, certainly not matching the research purpose to relate olfactory gene to the behavioural test. The search for an involvement of neural genes related to anxiety/depression or mood disorders is biased by the expression of human genes having no orthologues/homologues in mice, where the authors reported evidence about Gelsemium action on behavioural tests in animal anxiety models. Genes such as EN2 (engrailed homeobox 2), GALR2 (galanin receptor 2), GPR25 (G-protein coupled receptor 25), KLKBL14, erroneously indicated by the authors as Klkbl14, namely PRSS54 (protease serine 54), tachykinin 4 or TAC4 have orthologues both in human and mouse, while OR5C1 (olfactory receptor 5C1) is an exclusive Homo sapiens gene (mouse has an olfactory receptor 368 gene on chromosome 2). The authors tested Gelsemium hydroalcoholic preparations without a Limulus test to prevent bias from contamination by LPS or bacteria: this may generate bias. Curiously, genes affected by Gelsemium 2c, such as DDl1, are bacterial genes, notoriously absent/non expressed in Homo sapiens: probably is a misprint (delta-like 1 (DLL1) instead of D-alanine-D-alanine ligase (DDl1)). “The genes investigated by quantitative PCR generally confirmed the changes obtained by microarray assay. DDl1, EN2, GALR2, GPR25, OR5C1, Klkbl4 and TAC4 genes were downregulated in Gelsemium s. 2c samples compared to Control 2c in the three replicated experiments” (Results pag. 7). DDl1 is a bacterial gene: maybe the authors would indicate DLL1 (delta-like 1 gene)?


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 29, Peter Wilson commented:

      In the manuscript the authors incorrectly state that the function of the enzyme thymidylate synthase (TS) is the following:

      "TS is a key enzyme that catalyzes the methylation of fluorod UMP, the precursor of DNA synthesis, into dTMP"

      This is incorrect. Thymidylate synthase catalyzes the reductive methylation of dUMP (deoxyuridine) to dTMP (thymidine monophosphate; thymidylate). Fluoro-dUMP (or FdUMP as it is more commonly abbreviated to) is a potent fluoropyrimidine-derived irreversible inhibitor of TS and has nothing to do with the enzymatic mechanism of TS or the subject content of this manuscript.

      The reference provided for the TS enzymatic mechanism is also not specific to TS whatsoever and the proper terms should be given as well as the abbreviations e.g. thymidine monophosphate (dTMP) and deoxyuridine monophosphate (dUMP).

      Since the subject of this manuscript is centered around TS, the enzymatic function should be described accurately and precisely and this should be rectified.

      The authors also state: "However, this is the first and initial meta-analysis of assessment whether TS expression is associated with objective response in patients with NSCLC treated with pemetrexed-containing chemotherapy."

      This is also incorrect. This study is now the third to address this issue with two previous manuscripts published in 2013. According to the BMC Cancer pre-publication record, both previous manuscripts addressing this issue were published for a significant period of time before the final version of this current paper was submitted by March 5th, 2014 and therefore should have been referenced and discussed appropriately.

      Liu Y, Yin TJ, Zhou R, Zhou S, Fan L, Zhang RG. Cancer Chemother Pharmacol. 2013 Nov;72(5):1125-32.

      Wang T, Chuan Pan C, Rui Yu J, Long Y, Hong Cai X, De Yin X, Qiong Hao L, Li Luo L. PLoS One. 2013 Sep 10;8(9):e74284. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074284.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jun 09, carl voyles commented:

      In addition to the age of onset (61), profound weakness with forced retirement from battle (like modern day Parkee's)and numerous references to motor disorder in scripture, King David also suffered from autonomic dysfunction with sexual dysfunction and hypothermia terminally.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Jan 03, Lydia Maniatis commented:

      In comments on Yang and Purves (2004) in PP and PMC, I noted that the main claim - that perception of lightness is linked to the frequencies with which "visual patterns" have been encountered by organisms - is not only inconsistent with principles of biological evolution, but also lacks any supporting rationale. Interestingly, Purves, Monson, Sundararajan and Wojtach (2014) give the impression that they are going to provide such a rationale, in a short section of their article titled “Why Stimulus Frequency Predicts Perception and Behavior,” (which I quote in full below). (The article itself is one of several quite similar pitches for the “wholly empirical” account of visual perception that use Yang and Purves (2004) as key empirical reference).

      It should be noted, first, that the title of the section is misleading; it represents as fact claims that have not been corroborated. As I have discussed in detail, the methods of Yang and Purves (2004) are too vague, arbitrary, opaque, conceptually problematic and incomplete to allow the investigators to make such claims even for the narrow set of cases that was the focus of that study. Thus, what is missing in the account is both a rationale and evidence. This being the case, the extract below does not explain a fact, but offers a rationale for a (quite odd) hypothesis.

      This hypothesis, as summarized by Purves et al (2014), is that “perceptual values assigned by the frequency of occurrence of visual stimuli accord with the reproductive success of the species and individual.” Below I quote their rationalizations, interspersed with comments of my own.

      “Missing from this account, however, is why the frequencies of occurrence of visual stimuli sampled in this way predict perception. The reason, we maintain, is that the relative number of times biologically generated patterns are transduced and processed in accumulated experience tracks reproductive success.”

      [This is not a reason, it is a repetition of the unjustified claim that responding to stimuli (whether “biologically generated” or not) on the basis of their frequency is adaptive.]

      In Fig. 3, for example, the frequencies of occurrence of the patterns at the stage of photoreception have caused the central luminance value to occur more often when in the lower luminance surround than in the higher one, resulting in a steeper slope at that point on the cumulative distribution function.

      [There is no credible evidence for this claim (see my criticism of Yang and Purves (2004). In addition, Purves, Morgenstern and Wojtach (2015) have conceded that the sampling assumptions were flawed).]

      “If the relative ranking along this function corresponds to the perception of lightness”

      [the operative word is “if;” Purves and Yang (2004) did not test samples for their perceptual effects on independent observers]

      “then the higher the rank of a target luminance (T) in a given surround relative to another target luminance with the same surround, the lighter the target should appear. Therefore, because the target luminance in a darker surround (Fig. 3, Left) has a higher rank than the same target luminance in a lighter surround (Fig. 3, Right), the former should be seen as lighter than the latter, as it is. Because the frequency of occurrence of patterns is an evolved property”

      [this reference to frequency of occurrence of patterns being an evolved property makes no sense at all – the data in Yang and Purves (2004) are described and sampled as luminance values occurring in the environment, as an objective property of the environment outside of the evolving organism]

      “—and because these relative rankings along the function correspond to perception”

      [again, this “fact” lacks empirical evidence, as samples perceptual effects were not tested]

      “—the visually guided behaviors that result will in varying degrees have contributed to reproductive success.”

      [This is just a truism – a post hoc assumption that typical behaviors of existing organisms are behaviors that have contributed to survival and reproduction].

      “Thus, by aligning the frequencies of occurrence of light patterns over evolutionary time with perceptions of light and dark and the behaviors they elicit, this strategy can explain vision without solving the inverse optics problem.”

      [Nothing in the preceding paragraph serves to justify this statement. It doesn't explain, for example, why such a strategy would be adaptive.]

      The “explanation” thus rests on 1. Our taking on faith the unrationalized, never-really-tested and, in fact, impossible-to-test hypothesis that frequency of occurrence of loosely defined, arbitrarily sampled “visual patterns” over the evolutionary lifetime of the species tracks both perception and reproductive success, and 2. Overlooking the absurdity and contradiction of calling frequency of occurrence of luminances in the environment, as measured, using instruments, by Yang and Purves (2004) an “evolved property.” Even if we could make sense of the statement that “frequency of occurrence of patterns is an evolved [or “biologically generated” or occurring “at the stage of photoreception”] property, we would still need a. evidence and b. a rationale for such a unique assertion.

      In sum, this “explanation” contains neither evidence nor a rationale; it is wholly unempirical.

      p.s. The introduction of the idea of "evolved frequencies" seems unique to this article, in which the authors were (unusually) called on to rationalise their frequency hypothesis. In two more recent articles, reference is made to the "frequency of occurrence of image patterns" (Purves, Morgenstern & Wojtach, 2015a) that appear to be defined in the normal way, i.e. as the product of the focussing of "incoming light" which the structure and lens of the eye organise to form a "high-resolution image" (Purves, Morgenstern & Wojtach, 2015b). If we are talking about frequencies of images organised on basis of the straightforward focussing of incoming light rays, in the same way a camera's autofocus would, what aspect of these frequencies is "evolved" or "biologically generated"? And, again, redundantly, what's the theoretical rationale of the supposed frequency-perception link?


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 May 03, Brooke Morriswood commented:

      Please note that the manuscript contains an error concerning the gene ID of TbPIPKA orthologues. The correct orthologue of Tb427.04.1620 (TbPIPKA) is Tb927.4.1620, and not Tb927.10.1620 as stated in the text. The gene ID Tb927.10.1620 is a typo deriving from the location of the other two PI(4)P 5-kinases (Tb927.10.3890, Tb927.10.4770) on chromosome 10. We apologise for any misunderstanding. Graham Warren, Katy Schmidt, Lars Demmel, Brooke Morriswood.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 26, Guillaume Filion commented:

      This article is one of the "CISCOM meta-analyses", which are very similar papers written by different authors. For more information about the CISCOM meta-analyses, check the blog post "A flurry of copycast on PubMed" at the following link http://blog.thegrandlocus.com/2014/10/a-flurry-of-copycats-on-pubmed


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jul 07, Ryan Radecki commented:

      Post-publication commentary:

      "Enoximone Miracle for Asthma: Science? Advertising? Both?"

      Potential leaps forward in medical science are noteworthy. They should be peer-reviewed, disseminated, and developed – and those responsible, rewarded, to be sure. But, the medical literature is full of grey areas between science and advertising – including routine publication of sponsored trials, to a “Clinical Evidence Synopsis” composed solely by those with COI, to this latest egregious sample....

      http://www.emlitofnote.com/2014/06/enoximone-miracle-for-asthma-science.html


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Aug 24, Ben Goldacre commented:

      One of the trials in this article has the wrong trial registry ID associated with it on PubMed: both in the XML on PubMed, and in the originating journal article. The ID given is NCT00149913. We believe the correct ID, which we have found by hand searching, is NCT00159913.

      This comment is being posted as part of the OpenTrials.net project<sup>[1]</sup> , an open database threading together all publicly accessible documents and data on each trial, globally. In the course of creating the database, and matching documents and data sources about trials from different locations, we have identified various anomalies in datasets such as PubMed, and in published papers. Alongside documenting the prevalence of problems, we are also attempting to correct these errors and anomalies wherever possible, by feeding back to the originators. We have corrected this data in the OpenTrials.net database; we hope that this trial’s text and metadata can also be corrected at source, in PubMed and in the accompanying paper.

      Many thanks,

      Jessica Fleminger, Ben Goldacre*

      [1] Goldacre, B., Gray, J., 2016. OpenTrials: towards a collaborative open database of all available information on all clinical trials. Trials 17. doi:10.1186/s13063-016-1290-8 PMID: 27056367

      * Dr Ben Goldacre BA MA MSc MBBS MRCPsych<br> Senior Clinical Research Fellow<br> ben.goldacre@phc.ox.ac.uk<br> www.ebmDataLab.net<br> Centre for Evidence Based Medicine<br> Department of Primary Care Health Sciences<br> University of Oxford<br> Radcliffe Observatory Quarter<br> Woodstock Road<br> Oxford OX2 6GG


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Mar 19, Paolo Declich commented:

      I found this abstract very interesting: patients with sporadic FGPs are in fact patients without concurrent Helicobacter pylori infection. Their gastric antrum are gastritis free, so they can be considered low risk patients for gastric cancer developpment.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Mar 19, Jack Dowie commented:

      "...compensatory MCDA methods such as weighted sum models are not suitable for oncology decisionmaking problems. In fact, an MDT would be interested in treatments that are both effective (efficacy criterion) with and acceptable safety profile (safety criterion). A compensatory effect would arise if an alternative that exhibits a very good performance on efficacy and a poor performance on safety is preferred to another that has a good performance on efficacy criterion and a fair performance on safety, on the basis of a weighted sum score." This is simply a value judgement and truly person-centred care must allow patients to trade-off 'safety' with other criteria in MCDA-based decision aids or decisions.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 06, Shervin Assari commented:

      Great work. Any other publication on the same topic while gender is also considered?


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Mar 22, Marcos Malumbres commented:

      This is a problem here with the nomenclature. Cadherin-1 (mammalian symbol: Cdh1) and the Anaphase-promoting Complex (APC)-cofactor Cdh1 (mammalian symbol Fzr1) are two different genes with separate functions.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Aug 19, Nikolai Slavov commented:

      The classical work of Goldbeter and Koshland, (1981) described how a cycle of competing enzymes, such as a kinase and a phosphatase, can significantly amplify a signal. This work of Dasgupta et al. makes two very significant contributions to the Goldbeter-Koshland mechanism: (i) it generalizes these classical ideas beyond Michaelis-Menten kinetics to arbitrarily complicated enzyme mechanisms; (ii) it demonstrates how the noise of low copy-number fluctuations can be amplified by Goldbeter-Koshland kinetics and points to an elegantly simple solution, enzyme bifunctionality. It is a must read.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 04, Dale D O Martin commented:

      It should be noted that N-myristoylation of proteins can only occur on N-terminal Glycines, not internal glycines as they have predicted here and in other papers. That is why it's called N-myristoytlation. The only time that myristoylation can occur internally is following proteolysis of the protein that leads to an N-terminal Glycine on the C-terminal cleavage product. So far, this has only been shown to occur following caspase cleavage at Aspartate residues.

      The authors also don't include what prediction program they used nor do they reference any papers on myristoylation.

      I have commented on papers that include some of the same authors and this journal previously and I have brought it to their attention.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Oct 08, Salah Amasheh commented:

      A typographical error occurred in the title. The correct title would start with "Laurate Permeabilizes..."


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 26, Guillaume Filion commented:

      This article is one of the "CISCOM meta-analyses", which are very similar papers written by different authors. For more information about the CISCOM meta-analyses, check the blog post "A flurry of copycast on PubMed" at the following link http://blog.thegrandlocus.com/2014/10/a-flurry-of-copycats-on-pubmed


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 24, Markus Meissner commented:

      We characterised in this paper several conditional knockout mutants for core components of the known invasion machinery (Act1, MyoA, GAP45, MLC1 and MyoA,B/C). Surprisingly parasites remained invasive, albeit at reduced levels and for some factors, such as myosins we speculated that some functional overlap between MyoA and MyoC exists, since simultaneous depletion of these myosins resulted in a significantly stronger egress and invasion phenotype. A new study by Frenal et al., 2014 (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25393004) reproduces the phenotypic consequences of the simultaneous depletion of MyoA and MyoC and provides a mechanistic explanations for these findings.

      However, the central question regarding mechanistic role of the myosin/actin system during motility and invasion requires further analysis to answer the most intriguing question: How can the parasite move and invade in absence of a functional IMC (as seen in case of gap45KO parasites) or in absence of both the MyoA and MyoC motor (as seen for the MyoA,B,C mutants or in case of mlc1KO)?


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 18, Jacob Puliyel commented:

      None


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Apr 16, Arun Gupta commented:

      What is the hurry ?

      I would think it to be fundamentally flawed to introduce a vaccine based on a research that is not completed and its results have not been fully studied. Whatever the results are available show that the vaccine is not even cost effective. If you ask me that every child of India should get this I fail to understand the logic except that market must prevail because we live in the world which gives way to globalsation.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2014 Apr 15, Jacob Puliyel commented:

      50% More Intussusception Among Vaccinated: Full Trial Data Is Awaited

      According to the Clinical Trials Registry this study, started in March 2011 was estimated to be complete in April 2014.

      The data published in Lancet on March 12, 2014 refers only to the first half of the study (efficacy and safety of ORV 116E assessed in the first year of life). Data on efficacy and safety up to 2 years (from 14 days following the 3rd dose till the age of 2 years (24 months) + up to 14 days.) is still awaited.

      Even in this small sample studied for 1 year, the incidence of intussusception among the vaccinated was 50% more than controls. The rush to recommend licensing this drug before presenting the full trial data is surprising.

      Furthermore, contrary to expectations, it is now known that transplacental rotavirus IgG interferes with immune response to the live oral rotavirus vaccine (ORV-116E) in Indian infants Appaiahgari MB, 2014. The very low incidence of severe rotavirus gastroenteritis (SRVGE)seen in the study of Bhandari N, 2014 may be due to the protective effect of the transplacental antibodies. The vaccine is probably not needed in these circumstances.

      However Appaiahgari MB, 2014 found that higher doses of the vaccine was able to overcome the inhibitory effect of this RV IgG. The safety of the high-dose-vaccine has not been studied and so cannot be recommended as yet.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    4. On 2014 Apr 09, B M Hegde commented:

      While I am in total agreement with the views of Dr. Puliyal on this vaccine, I have some more questions!

      While to cost effectiveness is ridiculously low, our incidence of this kind of diarrhea is also very low, why do we need to push this vaccine down our poor children’s throats. In a village if we have good primary care centers to look after children with diarrhea, with enough IV fluids and oral re-hydration solutions, the rotavirus is not a problem at all. Diarrhea eliminates the virus and the child recovers not because of the virus being killed but because of the good re-hydration.

      I remember my student days we used to have cholera epidemics very, very frequently. What mattered even then was just re-hydration and not the cholera vibrio. I am sure cholera was much more dangerous than simple viral diarrhea. In the former, vomiting was so troublesome it would not easily allow oral re-hydration. Despite that, studies showed that a good oral re-hydration was better than IV fluids even in that situation.

      Our administrators should do something for the ubiquitous calorie protein malnutrition in children that depletes the immune system attracting many infectious diseases. This kills thousands of children in India.I call that 'Nutritional Immune Deficiency Syndrome' (NIDS).

      We are wasting our time and money on vaccines and antibiotics instead. That saving must be used in feeding all our children and pregnant mothers in the first trimester of pregnancy. What we are doing now is dangerous and not science. Exporting American “science” to the third world worries some of the thinkers among the best brains in the world. www.edge.org presents some thoughts from the best scientific brains in this area.

      B. M. Hegde MD, FRCP(Lond.), FRCP(Edin.), FRCP(Glas.), FRCP(Dub.), FACC(U.S.A.). Formerly Vice Chancellor, Manipal Academy of Higher Education, Manipal, India


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    5. On 2014 Apr 07, Amitav Banerjee commented:

      DEVELOPING COUNTRIES ARE BEING PUSHED INTO A "CATCH-22" SITUATION. Developing countries provide a happy hunting ground for clinical trials. Medical researchers in developing countries are lured into conducting drug and vaccine trials. This is an absurdity. We have more pressing problems in developing countries such as malnutrition, infant diarrhea, malaria,and other infections besides emerging noninfectious diseases and all the resources are diverted to teach clinicians how to do clinical trials. Clinical trials are useful since it is about time clinicians became a little more scientific about what they do. However, it would help if these clinicians who are trained in doing drug trials realize that epidemiology is more important than drug tests and become genuine epidemiologists and public health workers. Who decides which technologies are important to those countries that do not produce them directly? Technology manufacturers and health ministries govern these decisions. For launching any vaccine or even for starting any trial of vaccine, the first consideration should be to find out the prevalent disease burden of the concerned disease. In case these data are not gathered before launching a vaccine, the window of opportunity is lost giving rise to a Catch-22 situation. We will never know whether the vaccine is preventing the disease or the disease was having low incidence in the targeted population before launch of the vaccine. Therefore resources should first be used to gather these basic data before starting any vaccine trial. That this is not done for most vaccines which are being hastily introduced in developing countries gives rise to doubts of serious "conflicts of interest" of the decision makers.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    6. On 2014 Apr 04, Jacob Puliyel commented:

      MINISCULE RISK REDUCTION MAKES $1 ROTAVIRUS VACCINE (116E) UNECONOMICAL IN INDIA

      The authors must be congratulated for this study and the candid reporting of the absolute risk reduction (ARR) and numbers needed to treat (NNT).

      LOW DISEASE BURDEN

      Although rotavirus vaccine efficacy is lower in developing countries, it is advocated for poor countries because of the higher disease burden. Severe rotavirus gastroenteritis (SRVGE) was more common in Malawi than South Africa (13.1 vs. 5.4) and even though efficacy was lower in Malawi (49.4% vs. 76.9%) more cases of SRVGE were prevented by vaccination (6.7 vs. 4.2) Madhi SA, 2010. This is often given as the justification for using the vaccine with such low efficacy in poor countries.

      The incidence SRVGE was low very low in the unvaccinated in India (3.4%) compared to 13.1 in Malawi and 5.4 in South Africa. This raises questions about the need for the vaccine in India using the ‘disease burden’ argument.

      The absolute risk reduction (ARR) by vaccination was small (1.7). This is much lower than the benefit in Malawi (6.7) and even South Africa (4.2) Madhi SA, 2010.

      The NNT was 55. At $3/child, vaccination will cost $ 165 per SRVGE avoided. This is four times the societal cost of hospitalized diarrhea in India ($40.60) Mendelsohn AS, 2008

      RISK OF INTUSSUSCEPTIONS

      Intussusceptions are more dangerous in developing countries where facilities for its diagnosis and treatment are not easily available in remote areas. The earlier rotavirus vaccine RotaShield had been approved after clinical trials involving 10,054 children. It was then withdrawn from the market for causing intussusceptions (1 in 12,000 children).

      After the RotaSheild fiasco, FDA approval of RotaTeq based on results of three phase III trials of the drug which treated a combined 72,324 infants in 11 countries.

      The 116E has been studied in only 4532 with 2187 controls (total 6719). This is grossly inadequate for studying safety of this drug. The authors seem to suggest that this small study is sufficient for licensing the drug and safety can be examined during post marketing surveillance! It will indeed be a very brave licensing authority who, based on this study of 6719 children, will license the drug in countries like India where active post marketing surveillance is non-existent and where there is no proper ‘VAERS-like’ system available. In this context Paul King has proposed an effective system for AEFI surveillance with meaningful penalties for any healthcare provider's failure to report any possible AEFI to those maintaining this AEFI database.

      Perhaps those keen on the roll out of the vaccine may put such a system in place and some good may come from this vaccine.

      Saurabh Kumar, Jacob Puliyel


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Aug 23, Jacob Puliyel commented:

      None


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Aug 15, Jacob Puliyel commented:

      Team science must not be allowed to influence objectivity

      Projections of deaths exaggerated 5 times

      The authors must be congratulated for describing the team effort involved in the development of the new 116E rotavirus vaccine. In this process the Government of India through its funding of the Department of Science and Technology, the Bill and Melinda Gates foundation and PATH; all became partners who had a stake in getting this vaccine licensed and commercialized. Objectivity can become a casualty in the process.

      The authors for example argue that the need for this vaccine is evident from the fact that rotavirus causes 75000-122000 deaths each year. The magnitude of the problem is not in disputed but the efficacy of the remedy needs to be evaluated. The vaccine is said to have efficacy of 50%. However it reduces only 10% of the projected mortality.

      Empirical data from the trial with the 116E vaccine however suggests that at best, (assuming 100% coverage with the vaccine,) deaths prevented will only be one tenth of the 100,000 deaths said to be caused by rotavirus.

      Data from the trial of the 116E vaccine showed that during the first two years after vaccination, the number of infants that needed to be immunized to prevent one episode of rotavirus diarrhea of any severity was 21 (NNT = 21) Bhandari N, 2014. Assuming case fatality from rotavirus diarrhea (all cases regardless of severity) is 1% with community management Lal S, 1994 Kosek M, 2003, 2100 babies will have to be vaccinated to prevent one death from diarrhea in the first 2 years of life. If the birth cohort of 25 million are vaccinated only 11,904 lives may be saved. Either the efficacy figures are wrong or we must assume the projections of death from rotavirus infection are exaggerated - multiplied 5 times.

      Empirical evidence on intussusception from the same trial, have been described elsewhere in PubMed Commons http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25091662#cm25091662_5770.

      Thus we salute the effort that went in to make the vaccine, but that enthusiasm must not stand in the way of dispassionate evaluation of risks and benefits of the drug.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 26, Guillaume Filion commented:

      This article is one of the "CISCOM meta-analyses", which are very similar papers written by different authors. For more information about the CISCOM meta-analyses, check the blog post "A flurry of copycast on PubMed" at the following link http://blog.thegrandlocus.com/2014/10/a-flurry-of-copycats-on-pubmed


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 03, Nicholas Henschke commented:

      On behalf of the authors, thank you for your comment and interest in our study. The search strategy used for the systematic review did identify the trial cited in the above comment [1], however, this trial was originally excluded due to insufficient data to allow meta-analysis. It should have been added to the Table of Excluded Studies in the review, citing this reason.

      By using a factorial design the trial provides an estimate of the effect of ultrasound therapy compared to sham ultrasound in the presence of other co-interventions (osteopathic manual therapy and sham osteopathic manual therapy). This comparison was deemed sufficiently different to those from other trials in the meta-analysis to warrant inclusion. If data for each cell in the factorial design had been reported [2] some comparisons may have been suitable for inclusion in the meta-analysis.

      The trial authors will be contacted and given the opportunity to provide this data for the future updates of this review. This study has now been added to an amended version of the review under "Studies awaiting classification".

      References

      [1] Licciardone, JC, et al. Osteopathic manual treatment and ultrasound therapy for chronic low back pain: a randomized controlled trial. Ann Fam Med 2013;11(2):122-129.

      [2] McAlister, FA, et al. Analysis and reporting of factorial trials: a systematic review. JAMA 2003;289:2545-2553.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Mar 17, JOHN LICCIARDONE commented:

      It was interesting to read this Cochrane review on therapeutic ultrasound for chronic low back pain. The authors used a search strategy that was designed to be highly sensitive in retrieving the relevant randomized controlled trials that were reported through October 2013. The search identified seven small randomized controlled trials on therapeutic ultrasound for non-specific chronic low back pain involving a total of 362 patients. The authors concluded that there was moderate quality evidence that therapeutic ultrasound improves back-specific function compared with placebo in the short term. There was low quality evidence that therapeutic ultrasound is no better than placebo for short-term pain improvement.

      It is unclear why their search strategy failed to identify the results of the OSTEOPATHIC Trial, which were reported in March 2013 1. Therein, 455 patients were randomized to ultrasound therapy or sham ultrasound therapy as part of a randomized controlled trial that used a 2x2 factorial design to assess outcomes at 12 weeks in patients with non-specific chronic low back pain. Osteopathic manual treatment was also compared with sham osteopathic manual treatment in this trial.

      As measured by the Roland-Morris Disability Questionnaire, there was no evidence of any benefit with ultrasound therapy in improving back-specific function [1]. Correspondingly, using a visual analogue scale for low back pain and guidelines recommended by the Initiative on Methods, Measurement, and Pain Assessment in Clinical Trials [2] and by the Cochrane Back Review Group [3], the OSTEOPATHIC Trial failed to identify even a small treatment effect with ultrasound therapy in any of its analyses for low back pain reduction [1]. By contrast, osteopathic manual treatment was associated with medium to large treatment effects in this domain [1].

      The authors of this Cochrane review concluded that there are few high quality randomized controlled trials assessing the use of ultrasound for improving pain or quality of life in patients with non-specific chronic low back pain. They further commented that future large trials with valid methodology are likely to have an important impact on their confidence in the estimated effect and may change their estimate. Consequently, it would be worthwhile to assess the impact of these OSTEOPATHIC Trial results upon the Cochrane review and its conclusions given that a greater number of patients were enrolled in the OSTEOPATHIC Trial than in all other seven trials combined in the Cochrane review.

      References

      1. Licciardone, JC, et al. Osteopathic manual treatment and ultrasound therapy for chronic low back pain: a randomized controlled trial. Ann Fam Med 2013;11(2):122-129.
      2. Dworkin, RH, et al. Interpreting the clinical importance of treatment outcomes in chronic pain clinical trials: IMMPACT recommendations. J Pain 2008;9(2):105-121.
      3. Furlan, AD, et al. 2009 updated method guidelines for systematic reviews in the Cochrane Back Review Group. Spine (Phila Pa 1976) 2009;34(18):1929-1941.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On date unavailable, commented:

      None


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Aug 28, Niall Duncan commented:

      Dr Bishop raises some interesting points. I can't add to those regarding reading ability per se but can perhaps add some information relevant to the MRS-related ones.

      Glutamate concentrations appear to be stable over the relevant age range, as do choline concentrations (Dezortova M, 2008 ; Blüml S, 2013). [A longitudinal study in rats has found a continuing increase in Glx over the lifespan which with the higher field strength may be worth taking into consideration (Morgan JJ, 2013).] Creatine concentrations appear to change over the relevant period, which could be an issue as this is taken as the reference substance in the paper. The authors look at this possibility though and say that there is no relationship between reading ability and creatine. I still find it odd that they don't report the age and tissue proportion corrected partial correlations as their main findings as taking these into account is pretty standard in the MRS literature. [That it is standard does not of course mean that it is right and the authors may be able to give a convincing argument as to why their approach is better.]

      Also curious is that they don't report anything about glutamine concentrations. As far as I can tell from the methods these are measured (fully accepting that I could be misreading what they have done). There has been a lot of conflation of glutamate with glutamate+glutamine (Glx) in the literature as not all methods allow these to be reliably separated. The two things are not equivalent however. Some clarification along these lines in the case of this paper would be nice.

      MRS is probably best described as a trait measure. There are some so-called "functional MRS" studies that find changes in metabolite (generally glutamate or lactate) concentrations when a task is performed. Normally, though, measurements are made "at rest" over a period of 10-20 minutes.

      The test-retest reliability of MRS varies between different acquisition/analysis methods, regions, and metabolites. For Glx the CoV seems to be around 7% and for GABA 10%. For creatine it seems to be closer to 5% (relevant where this is taken as the reference substance). Glutamine has poor reproducibility (CoV over 20%), which is worrying when trying to separate this signal from glutamate. These numbers are from adults though and might be different in children.

      Related to the difference between children and adults, the issue of head movement is one that has not yet been properly addressed in the MRS literature. These is some evidence that choline concentrations are particularly affected by head motion (Andrews-Shigaki BC, 2011). It could therefore be the case that the children towards one end or the other of the reading score distribution tend to move more (differences in boredom thresholds or suchlike?).

      Metabolite concentrations do differ between different parts of the brain but the question is whether they are correlated with each other or not. I'm not aware of anybody specifically testing this yet. It is thus possible that the occipital cortex is acting as a correlated "proxy region" for a different region that is more directly linked to reading. This question of specificity is, I believe, an important one for MRS studies. Alternatively, my naive first take would be that doesn't seem so unlikely that metabolite levels in the visual system are related to "higher-level" functions such as reading.

      A few papers that interested people might find useful are:

      • An excellent overview of the metabolites being measured with MRS (Rae CD, 2014).
      • A review of MRS techniques for measuring Glx (Ramadan S, 2013).
      • A review of MRS techniques for measuring GABA (Puts NA, 2012).
      • A discussion of how to combine MRS with other measures (Duncan NW, 2014).
      • A review of the multimodal imaging (i.e., combining MRS with other measures) literature to date (Duncan NW, 2014).

      [Disclaimer - the last two are by colleagues and me.]


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2014 May 05, Dorothy V M Bishop commented:

      The findings reported in this study suggest that the study of neurometabolites could help us understand the neural basis of individual differences in reading ability. It is good to see this novel approach to neurobiological bases of reading. There were, however, places where the paper was unclear on certain details, and I would be grateful if the authors could clarify these points – and/or make the whole dataset publicly available.

      1. On p 4083, we are told that poor readers are identified on the basis of a composite score on TOWRE of 85 or less, and good readers with score of 100 or more. Later, though, data are presented for a reading composite score which is based on the two TOWRE subtests and reading comprehension from WJ-III. Why was WJ-III LWID not included?

      2. I had assumed that in forming a composite, age-scaled scores would be used, yet on p 4085, it is stated that the analyses used a principal component based on raw scores, unadjusted for age. Specifically, it is stated that "Standard scores were not used because our focus is on the relation between neurometabolite levels and raw skill differences, not skill differences standardized to an age or grade norm." (p. 4085). Yet the focus of the paper in the Abstract, Introduction and Discussion is on reading disability – which can only be identified if we have age-adjusted reading scores.

      3. This is key to interpreting the scatterplots in Figure 2. It seemed odd that some good/intermediate readers had lower reading composite scores than poor readers. I initially thought these might be children with poor reading comprehension, who had done well on TOWRE but got very low scores on reading comprehension. But if these scores are not age-adjusted, then these could be children whose low scores are less abnormal because they are young. Is that the case? Given the focus on reading disability, we do need to see the data for age-adjusted reading ability relative to glutamate levels. An association between raw reading scores and glutamate levels could simply mean that both measures show maturational changes.

      4. The legend of Table 2 is confusing: it is stated that p-values indicated with * indicate significance after correction for multiple comparisons. With 4 neurometabolites and 4 dependent variables, a Bonferroni correction would treat as significant correlations with associated p-values less than .05/16 = .003. The p-values in Table 2 are uncorrected p-values for the correlations, and none met that criterion. It could be argued that a Bonferroni correction is too conservative here, but if so, further information is needed about the 'conservative correction for multiple comparisons' referred to on p 4085.

      5. The partial correlations after removing linear effects of age and grey matter volume are presented in brackets in Table 2 and are not interpreted statistically. (They are generally lower than the raw correlations and would not be regarded as statistically significant if a Bonferroni correction were applied). It thus seems that the main results that form the focus of the discussion and conclusions are not age-adjusted and hence could reflect the fact that both reading and neurometabolite levels change with age. Is that correct?

      6. It would be good to see fuller data from time 2 testing. A correlation between Cho and RC that had been significant (using an uncorrected p-value) at time 1 was no longer significant. This could be because the reading composite was not very reliable, because there is a genuine change in the strength of association, and/or because of reduced power in the smaller sample, but we cannot tell on the basis of the data that is provided. What is the correlation between RC at time 1 and time 2? How many children who were RD at time 1 still met criteria for RD at time 2? If we focus just on the 45 children tested both times, what were the correlations with Glu and Cho at time 1?

      7. On p 4086 ANOVA is used to compare TD and RD groups. It is argued that age and gray matter volume need not be controlled because they do not differ between these groups. However, insofar as both variables are correlated with metabolite levels, it would be good to include them as covariates, as this would improve the power of the analysis by removing extraneous sources of variance. It is possible that the authors are underestimating the effect size here. In addition, the performance of the 'middle' group, with scores intermediate between reading disabled and typical development, is not shown, yet is of interest. More generally, converting quantitative data to categories using cutoffs and then excluding a middle group is problematic, because it creates 'researcher degrees of freedom' if cutoffs are not pre-specified - see Simmons, J. P., et al (2011). Psychological Science, 22(11), 1359-1366. doi: 10.1177/0956797611417632

      8. It is good to see the authors attempting to replicate the Cho result with data from the NIH MRI study of normal brain development, but they do not compare like with like. The NIH study used two reading measures from WJ-III, both of which were also used in the Pugh et al study. We need to see a parallel analysis of the two studies with the same measures treated the same way, as well as a fuller report of the NIH sample data that would make it possible to compare the means and ranges of scores between the two samples.

      9. Finally, as someone unfamiliar with MRS, I would be very interested to know more about the nature of this measure, in particular, how consistent is it within individuals, and how consistent is it across brain voxels? Regarding the individual consistency, I found myself wondering if it should be seen more as a measure of trait or state: Is there data on test-retest reliability of MRS values that could give insights into this? Alternatively, can neurometabolite levels be manipulated by behavioural interventions? As regards consistency across the brain, is it the case that glutamate/choline levels tend to be similar across the whole brain, so that sampling any region would give similar findings? I had assumed not, but in that case, it is surprising that the midline occipital voxel should show correlations with reading, since this brain region is not among those usually regarded as forming part of the reading network.

      I appreciate that length constraints in journals often preclude full reporting of results, and I am aware that the Journal of Neuroscience has compounded this problem by ceasing to accept supplemental material. In my view this is bad for science as it means that readers are left with many questions about details of results, such as those that I have noted here. It is likely that future studies will aim to replicate and extend this line of work and these findings could then be incorporated into a meta-analysis – but as it stands, the level of detail is insufficient for this purpose. There are, however, options open to authors who wish to report fuller data, including FigShare or Open Science Framework, and perhaps this can provide a solution for authors whose complex datasets cannot be easily condensed into a brief report.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Mar 10, Oliver Gillie commented:

      Dr Autier has misunderstood my affairs which are explained in a Corrigendum to the scientific journal, Public Health Nutrition (see reference below). I have always reported conflict of interest accurately. The editor of PHN published the Corrigendum to clarify the situation while agreeing that I had no financial conflict of interest to declare. Dr Autier raises the issue once more but I have already explained my position in the Corrigendum and am surprised he has not seen it.

      I worked with the Vitamin D Company Ltd helping them to sell their 5,000 IU tablet in the UK at a time when high dose tablets were difficult to get in the UK. I felt that availability of the 5000 tablet could make an important contribution to our national health. However I never received any sales commission or any money at all from the Vitamin D Company and so have no financial conflict of interest to declare. In fact my 11 year campaign to inform the public about the facts of vitamin D and sunshine has cost me several thousand pounds in publishing and distributing, free of charge, reports on vitamin D (please see below for details). These reports have done much to publicise scientific and health issues surrounding sunshine and vitamin D which have been widely misunderstood by influential bodies such as Cancer Research UK. Over these 11 years I have made no earnings other than from occasional newspaper and magazine articles.

      Dr Autier is a staff member of the International Prevention Research Institute (IPRI), a body which is funded by industry and undertakes custom research for industry. This financial security allows Dr Autier the luxury of adopting a fallacious statistical argument, which may be welcomed by the pharmaceutical industry, but does them no credit because it is poorly thought out scientifically and poorly argued. Autier et al have presented lengthy statistical results which incorporate this scientific error, an error well understood by most experimentalists yet denied here by Autier. His response below tells us what we know already but fails to address the scientific issue raised by my article in Public Health Nutrition (ref below).

      REFERENCES:

      The CORRIGENDUM in PHE:

      http://journals.cambridge.org/download.php?file=/PHN/S1368980015000130a.pdf&code=a0e2984cd8793a37d36eebf438633b36

      ARTICLE ON AUTIER et al’s ERROR:

      “Controlled trials of vitamin D, causality and type 2 statistical error.” Public Health Nutrition 2014

      TWO REPORTS on vitamin D written by Gillie (summaries below) are available free from: www.healthresearchforum.org.uk :

      “SUNLIGHT ROBBERY - Health benefits of sunlight are denied by current public health policy in the UK.” (2004), drew attention to the health benefits of sunbathing and vitamin D and inadequate public understanding of the issues. Alternative advice for the public on safe sun exposure, given in the book, differs in important ways from advice given by Cancer Research-UK, which has encouraged sun avoidance so risking serious vitamin D insufficiency and increasing overall risks of cancer. Sir Richard Doll commented on the book: “I am most impressed with the way Gillie has collected and presented the evidence”.

      “SCOTLAND'S HEALTH DEFICIT - an explanation and a plan (2008)”, has drawn attention to vitamin D insufficiency in Scotland and the link with chronic disease. The central hypothesis of the book is that a substantial and significant portion of the excess mortality of Scotland compared with England is caused by vitamin D insufficiency. This excess mortality, known as “the Scottish effect”, cannot be explained by smoking, alcohol, diet or poverty. A subsidiary hypothesis is that major chronic diseases that are caused at least in part by D insufficiency (e.g. cancer, heart disease, MS etc) have a higher incidence in Scotland than in England. Evidence for these ideas is examined in the book and general support is found for them. Sir Muir Gray commented: “I was very impressed by the strength of evidence and by the conclusion…The work is of importance and a great achievement.”


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2015 Mar 09, William Grant commented:

      Comments on why vitamin D observational and intervention studies disagree

      In the paper by Autier et al. [1], it was stated that "The discrepancy between observational and intervention studies suggests that low 25(OH)D is a marker of ill health. Inflammatory processes involved in disease occurrence and clinical course would reduce 25(OH)D, which would explain why low vitamin D status is reported in a wide range of disorders." In my comment I will argue that the primary reason for the discrepancy between observational and intervention studies lies in the design of the intervention studies and that there is evidence that vitamin D3 reduces biomarkers of inflammation.

      If intervention trials are intended to evaluate observational studies, i.e., determine whether vitamin D has a beneficial role in health, they should start with an understanding of the 25-hydroxyvitamin D [25(OH)D] concentration-health outcome relation [2]. They should then seek to enroll people in the studies with sufficiently low 25(OH)D concentrations that the vitamin D dose used in the study will significantly change the position along the relation [3]. If this is not done, the intervention study then becomes an exercise in determining whether supplementing the average population with a given amount of vitamin D has any impact on health outcomes, which is a different question.

      A typical 25(OH)D concentration-health outcome relation was recently published for breast cancer incidence from case-control studies [4]. The power law fit to the data from 11 studies goes from an odds ratio of 1.77 at 15 nmol/L, to 1.04 at 30 nmol/L, 0.70 at 50 nmol/L, 0.52 at 75 nmol/L, and 0.41 at 100 nmol/L. The 25(OH)D concentration-PTH relation has a similar form, extending to 185 nmol/L [5]. Thus, there is a large change in odds ratio for a small change in 25(OH)D for low 25(OH)D concentrations, with the change decreasing with increasing 25(OH)D concentration. In addition, 25(OH)D concentration changes in a nonlinear fashion with respect to baseline 25(OH)D concentration for a given vitamin D3 oral intake [6], so it takes higher vitamin D3 doses to have an impact at higher baseline concentrations.

      Some examples from the journal literature are instructive. A vitamin D3 trial regarding respiratory infections in adults conducted in New Zealand had baseline 25(OH)D concentration of 73 nmol/L and achieved 25(OH)D concentration above 120 nmol/L but found no reduction [7]. In contrast, a related study in Mongolia involving school children with baseline 25(OH)D concentrations of 18 nmol/L given 300 IU/d vitamin D3 found a 50% reduction in acute respiratory infections [8].

      It is noted that the history of the understanding of the health benefits of vitamin A went through some of the same concerns as the understanding of those for vitamin D is today. About half of the intervention studies in the 1930s found beneficial effects in reducing infections [9]. In that paper "As suggested by Hess et al. (1933), no therapeutic benefit of vitamin A was noted for infants who were already sufficient in vitamin A." [10]. Now vitamin A supplementation prevents blindness and saves many lives from death due to infections in developing countries [11].

      Regarding inflammation, a paper was recently published in response to Ref. 1 in which all vitamin D randomized controlled trials found were examined for beneficial effects on biomarkers of inflammation along with baseline and achieved 25(OH)D concentration [12]. For those trials using vitamin D3 with baseline 25(OH)D concentration between 17 and 47 nmol/L, 49% found a beneficial effect. For those trials with baseline 25(OH)D concentration between 49 and 77 nmol/L, only 27% did. In addition, 55% those with achieved 25(OH)D concentrations between 35 and 82 nmol/L had a beneficial finding contrasted with 29% for those with achieved concentrations between 83 and 187 nmol/L. Thus, baseline 25(OH)D concentration was much more important than achieved 25(OH)D concentration.

      Well-designed vitamin D3 intervention studies should confirm many of the findings from observational studies. However, for now, observational studies remain the best guide to optimal 25(OH)D concentrations [13].

      References 1. Autier P, Boniol M, Pizot C, Mullie, P. Vitamin D status and ill health: a systematic review. Lancet Diabetes & Endocrinology. 2014;2(1):76-89. 2. Grant WB. Using findings from observational studies to guide vitamin D randomized controlled trials. J Intern Med. 2015;277(1):83-6. 3. Heaney RP. Guidelines for optimizing design and analysis of clinical studies of nutrient effects. Nutr Rev. 2014;72(1):48-54. 4. Grant WB. 25-Hydroxyvitamin D and breast cancer, colorectal cancer, and colorectal adenomas: case–control versus nested case–control studies, Anticancer Res. 2015;35(2):1153-60. 5. Valcour A, Blocki F, Hawkins DM, Rao SD. Effects of age and serum 25-OH-vitamin D on serum parathyroid hormone levels. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2012;97(11):3989-95. 6. Garland CF, French CB, Baggerly LL, Heaney RP. Vitamin D supplement doses and serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D in the range associated with cancer prevention. Anticancer Res 2011;31:617-22. 7. Murdoch DR, Slow S, Chambers ST, Jennings LC, Stewart AW, Priest PC, Florkowski CM, Livesey JH, Camargo CA, Scragg R. Effect of vitamin D3 supplementation on upper respiratory tract infections in healthy adults: the VIDARIS randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2012;308(13):1333-9. 8. Camargo CA Jr, Ganmaa D, Frazier AL, Kirchberg FF, Stuart JJ, Kleinman K, Sumberzul N, Rich-Edwards JW. Randomized trial of vitamin D supplementation and risk of acute respiratory infection in Mongolia. Pediatrics. 2012;130(3):e561-7. 9. Semba RD. Vitamin A as "anti-infective" therapy, 1920-1940. J Nutr. 1999;129(4):783-91 10. Hess AF, Lewis, JM, Barenberg LH. Does our dietary require vitamin A supplement? J. Am. Med. Assoc. 1933;101:657–63. 11. Sommer A. Preventing blindness and saving lives: the centenary of vitamin A. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2014;132(1):115-7. 12. Cannell JJ, Grant WB, Holick MF. Vitamin D and inflammation. Dermatoendocrinol. 2014;6(1): e983401-1-10. Published online Jan. 29, 2015 http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.4161/19381980.2014.983401 13. Grant WB, Wimalawansa SJ, Holick MF, Cannell JJ, Pludowski P, Lappe JM, Pittaway M, May P. Emphasizing the health benefits of vitamin D for those with neurodevelopmental disorders and intellectual disabilities. Nutrients. 2015;7:1538-64.

      Disclosure My organization, Sunlight, Nutrition and Health Research Center, receives funding from Bio-Tech Pharmacal, Inc. (Fayetteville, AR) and the Vitamin D Council (San Luis Obispo, CA). In the past it also received funding from), the UV Foundation (McLean, VA), the Vitamin D Society (Woodstock, Ontario), the Sunlight Research Forum (Veldhoven), and MediSun Technology (Highland Park, IL.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2015 Mar 09, Philippe Autier commented:

      In our systematic review, we concluded that the association between low vitamin D status (as assessed by serum 25(OH)D concentration) and ill health found in most observational studies is not causal and could just reflect the influence of inflammatory processes present in many diseases (1). Our conclusion was essentially based on the finding that in the vast majority of randomized trials, vitamin D supplementation had no impact on health outcomes. O Gillie refutes our conclusions on the basis that type 2 errors could underlie the absence of trial result compatible with health benefit of vitamin D supplementation. This refutation is wrong because it equates to the common mistake of post-hoc power calculation (2). A type 2 error consists in rejecting the alternative hypothesis (i.e., vitamin D supplementation can prevent or cure diseases) and accepting the null hypothesis (i.e., vitamin D supplementation cannot prevent or cure diseases) when ultimately, the alternative hypothesis would turn out to be true. The avoidance of type 2 errors in randomized trials largely governs the size of trials, because the more subjects are included in a trial, the lower the probability of a type 2 error. This concept is termed the “power” of a trial that is the capacity of a trial to demonstrate the likelihood of the alternative hypothesis. The power is computed prior to randomization as the probability that a trial of a given size could confirm the possibility of a truly existing association between the intervention and the outcome. But once a trial has been conducted, the interpretation of results should be solely driven by the probability of type 1 error to avoid fallacious interpretation based on post-hoc power calculation.

      For example, observational studies on cardiovascular diseases have documented risk reductions of about 40% for high versus low quantile of serum 25(OH)D concentration. At inception of their meta-analysis of randomized trials on vitamin D supplements and the risk of cardiovascular death, given the numbers of subjects and of cardiovascular events reported by trials, Bjelakovic et al. (3) had a 96.1% power to detect a 20% reduction in the risk of cardiovascular death (bilateral test with an alpha risk of 5%). By usual standards, a 96.1% power is huge. Once the meta-analysis was done, the observed relative risk was of 1.02 (95%CI 0.91-1.13). The probability that a significant real risk reduction of 20% could have been missed by the meta-analysis is a type 1 error whose probability can be estimated as 1 out of 58548.

      In our review, many randomized trials on vitamin D supplementation and their meta-analyses were sufficiently powered for identifying risk reductions of 20% or more. But the flat results of most trials indicate that the probability of type 1 error for missing risk reductions of 20% is negligible. In lay terms, this can be referred as an extremely small chance to have missed a true relationship between vitamin D supplementation and an outcome.

      O Gillie insists much on vitamin D status in children. Probably that because growing evidence reveals the unsoundness of the hypothesis of an “epidemic of vitamin D deficiency” in adults, “vitamin D deficiency” in children is taken as the substitute culprit for ill health during adult life. However, in many countries where bright sunshine is not habitual, vitamin D supplementation for small children and pregnant women is common practice since 50 years or more. If occasional cases of rickets occur in latitudes remote from the equator, these cases concentrate in children with naturally dark skin. It is therefore highly unlikely that a large proportion of ill health in adults would be due to rampant vitamin D deficiency in children.

      O Gillie alludes to physiological mechanisms by which a lack of vitamin D would harm individuals, including cardiac remodeling and auto-immune diseases. But the scientific literature is replete with speculations on biological mechanism evoked by believers for promoting the putative healthy virtues of antioxidants, vitamins, and many other micro or macro nutrients. Unfortunately, ugly facts, often under the form of results of randomized trials (e.g., ref. 4 and 5), practically always demonstrate the inanity of these speculations.<br> We were surprised that O Gillie declared to have no conflict of interest (http://www.vitdco.com/pages/about-us).

      Philippe Autier MD, Mathieu Boniol PhD, Patrick Mullie PhD

      1. Autier P, Boniol M, Pizot C, Mullie P. Vitamin D status and ill health: a systematic review. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol 2014;2(1):76-89.
      2. Hoenig, John M. and Heisey, Dennis M. (2001), The Abuse of Power: The Pervasive Fallacy of Power Calculations for Data Analysis, The American Statistician, 55, 19-24.
      3. Bjelakovic G, Gluud LL, Nikolova D, Whitfield K, Wetterslev J, Simonetti RG, Bjelakovic M, Gluud C. Vitamin D supplementation for prevention of mortality in adults. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2011; (7):CD007470.
      4. Miller ER 3rd, Pastor-Barriuso R, Dalal D, Riemersma RA, Appel LJ, Guallar E. Meta-analysis: high-dosage vitamin E supplementation may increase all-cause mortality. Ann Intern Med. 2005 Jan 4;142(1):37-46.
      5. Lawlor DA, Davey Smith G, Kundu D, Bruckdorfer KR, Ebrahim S. Those confounded vitamins: what can we learn from the differences between observational versus randomised trial evidence? Lancet. 2004 May 22;363(9422):1724-7.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    4. On 2015 Feb 25, Oliver Gillie commented:

      There is a serious flaw in the scientific reasoning of Autier et al 1,2. A negative result in a clinical trial of a vitamin in adult disease does not prove that the vitamin cannot have caused the disease. A vitamin deficiency may have occurred earlier in life causing irreversible metabolic damage.

      Title: Clinical trials, causation and type 2 error. By Oliver Gillie PhD Health Research Forum, 68 Whitehall Park, London N19 3TN Tel: ++44 20 7561 9677

      I have pointed out Autier’s mistake which may be classified as a “type 2 statistical error” 2. Regrettably The Lancet (diabetes and endocrinology) has refused to publish a correction even though the error may be seen as one of editing as much as of original research.

      Autier et al point out that raising the blood level of 25(OH)D (the standard reference measure of vitamin D) with a vitamin D supplement in clinical trials has not generally been found to modify the occurrence or clinical course of diseases associated with low D in observational studies. And they conclude: “Hence, associations between 25(OH)D and health disorders reported by investigators of observational studies are not causal.” 1

      Autier is even more forthright in a Lancet press release where he is quoted as saying: "If the health benefits of high vitamin D concentrations shown by data from observational studies are not reproduced in randomised trials (the gold standard method for assessing a causal relation between an exposure and an outcome) then the relation between vitamin D status and disorders are probably the result of confounding or physiological events involved in these disorders… What this discrepancy suggests is that decreases in vitamin D levels are a marker of deteriorating health." 3

      However, Autier et al have misunderstood the literature on trials and causation. They reference Byar et al.4 as referring to RCTs as a “gold standard” in establishing causation. However Byar et al. do not use the term “gold standard” and only consider causation very briefly. Causation may be proved in a clinical trial when supplementation succeeds in correcting a defect, but not when it fails to do so. A null result may be obtained simply because the trial took place too long after an irreversible insult occurring at a much earlier time.

      A “type 2 error” mistakenly accepts the null hypothesis, when an alternative hypothesis is or could be the true state of nature. That is: Autier et al find no significant difference between treated and untreated subjects in adult trials of vitamin D supplements and use this finding to support a null hypothesis. In doing so they wrongly rule out an alternative hypothesis that treatment at an earlier stage, e.g. during growth and development, might be effective. Much more extensive trials in all age groups and in pregnancy are required before a null conclusion could be safely reached.

      Rickets is the classical example of a disease which may be cured in early life but not in adulthood.5 It causes alteration of normal bone formation and deformation of limbs which may be corrected in childhood by supplementation with vitamin D. If however the deformations, whether gross or minor, are not corrected by vitamin D while the bones are growing, the bones become set in a pathological form that cannot be corrected by later supplementation.

      Cardiac structure and some cardiac diseases such as hypertension may be associated causally with low 25(OH)D levels. The Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging has found that 25(OH)D levels are positively correlated with left ventricle wall thickness and there is a relationship between 25(OH)D and left-ventricle concentric remodelling.6 Hypertension in this study was also linked to left ventricle hypertrophy and low 25(OH)D. Experiments with young rats show that deprivation of vitamin D causes specific cardiac abnormalities similar to those found in the Baltimore study: cardiac hypertrophy, left-chamber alterations and systolic dysfunction, which follow on from cardiac inflammation, fibrosis and apoptosis.6 This strongly suggests that the association of low 25(OH)D with cardiac pathology is the result of lifelong low vitamin D levels which caused heart abnormalities during early growth. These observations can reasonably be regarded as proof that heart anatomy, and diseases arising from pathological changes in heart anatomy, can be caused by early vitamin D deficiency.

      However, if we follow the reasoning of Autier et al. failure of vitamin D to induce beneficial heart remodeling in adults with abnormal heart anatomy would lead us to misleading conclusions i.e. that low vitamin D associated with abnormal heart anatomy in the Baltimore study is the result of reverse causation.

      Autoimmune and other diseases are associated in significantly elevated rates with hospital admission for vitamin D deficiency, osteomalacia and rickets.8 Failure in determination of the immune system early in life by escape of T cells from thymic deletion could explain this common association of low vitamin D with autoimmune disease. Vitamin D is well known to be involved in immune processes.9

      Many of the molecular mechanisms involving vitamin D are well known and provide a substantial basis for further exploration with clinical trials.9 However clinical trials of vitamin D must be conducted bearing in mind the additional difficulties presented when testing a nutrient rather than a drug.10 It is very difficult and enormously expensive to undertake clinical trials which continue for 20 or 30 years. Indeed it may not be possible. Other approaches are therefore required such as the animal experiments which complement the Baltimore study.

      Further examples showing how vitamin D deficiency may cause disease at various stages of life are given in an article2 by the author together with examples of the serious harms which may ensue from literal or dogmatic adherence to policy advice provided by Autier and colleagues. Conflicts of interest: I have no conflicts of interest.

      References 1. Autier PBM, Boniol M, Pizot C & Mullie P (2013) Vitamin D status and ill health: a systematic review. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol 2, 76–89. 2. Gillie O. Controlled trials of vitamin D, causality and type 2 statistical error. Public Health Nutrition 2014. 3. Anon. (2013) Further doubt cast on benefit of vitamin D supplementation for disease prevention. Press release promoting Autier et al. (2013); available at : http://www.eurekalert.org/pub_releases/2013-12/l-fdc120313.php 4. Byar DP, Simon RM, Friedewald WT et al. (1976) Randomized clinical trials. Perspectives on some recent ideas. N Engl J Med 95, 74–80. 5. Holick MF (2006) Resurrection of vitamin D deficiency and rickets. J Clin Invest 116, 2062–2072. 6. Ameri P, Canepa M, Milaneschi Y et al. (2013) Relationship between vitamin D status and left ventricular geometry in a healthy population: results from the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. J Intern Med 273, 253–262. 7. Assalin HB, Rafacho BP, dos Santos PP et al. (2013) Impact of the length of vitamin D deficiency on cardiac remodeling. Circ Heart Fail 6, 809–816. 8. Ramagopalan SV, Maugeri NJ, Handunnetthi L et al. (2013) Hospital admissions for vitamin D related conditions and subsequent immune-mediated disease: record-linkage studies. BMC Med 11, 171. 9. Pludowski P, Holick MF, Pilz S et al. Vitamin D effects on mitochondrial health, immunity, autoimmunity, cardiovascular disease, cancer, fertility, pregnancy, dementia and mortality – a review of recent evidence. Autoimmune Rev 2013; 12: 976-89 10. Grant WB. Using findings from observational studies to guide vitamin D randomized controlled trials. J Int Med. Published online: 5 May 2014 DOI: 10.1111/joim.12260


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 26, Guillaume Filion commented:

      This article is one of the "CISCOM meta-analyses", which are very similar papers written by different authors. For more information about the CISCOM meta-analyses, check the blog post "A flurry of copycast on PubMed" at the following link http://blog.thegrandlocus.com/2014/10/a-flurry-of-copycats-on-pubmed


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jun 18, Ryan Radecki commented:

      Post-publication commentary:

      "Abscess Management in the Era of MRSA"

      Every so often, it’s good to circle back from the esoteric to the basics, and remind ourselves how to provide the best, evidence-based treatment for some of the most common diseases – in this case, abscesses....

      http://www.emlitofnote.com/2014/06/abscess-management-in-era-of-mrsa_16.html


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Jun 02, thomas samaras commented:

      Additional research on height, body size and longevity is available from the following publications.

      Samaras TT. Evidence from eight different types of studies showing that smaller body size is related to greater longevity. Journal of Scientific Research & Reports. 2014: 3 (16): 2150-2160, 2014; article no. JSRR.2014.16.003.

      Samaras TT. Human Scaling and Body Mass Index. In: Samaras TT (ed): Human Body Size and the Laws of Scaling: Physiological Performance, Growth, Longevity and Ecological Ramifications. New York: Nova Science Pub; 2007: pp 17-32.

      He Q, Morris BJ, Grove JS, Petrovitch H, Ross W, Masaki KH, et al. Shorter men live longer: Association of height with longevity and FOXO3 genotype in American men of Japanese ancestry. Plos ONE 9(5): e94385. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0094385.

      Salaris L, Poulain M, Samaras TT. Height and survival at older ages among men born in an inland village in Sardinia (Italy), 1866-2006. Biodemography and Social Biology, 58:1, 1-13.

      Bartke A. Healthy Aging: Is Smaller better? A mini-review. Gerontology 2012; 58:337-43.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Mar 27, Gustav van Niekerk commented:

      So called ‘sickens associated anorexia’ (SAA) is one of the major manifestations of an infection. I am very curious regarding the extent to which this response is evolutionary conserved: I have come across articles on describing a SAA in vertebrate [1-4] and invertebrates [5-8] (sorry, not using the NCBI PMID codes as I am cut-and pasting from a draft) such as Drosophila and African army worm but not primitive animals such as corals and sea anemone. In your manuscript, you make reference that “no feeding occurred after the infection” as well as the “retraction of tentacles”. Is this because you stopped feeding, or because the animal stopped feeding? More explicitly, would you consider the sea anemone enacting a form of SAA? Also, is there any similar study describing a decrees in feeding when infected by other primitive animals such as see sponges?

      References 1. Crespi EJ, Denver RJ: Roles of stress hormones in food intake regulation in anuran amphibians throughout the life cycle. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology 2005, 141(4):381-390. 2. De Voe RS: Nutritional support of reptile patients. Vet Clin North Am Exot Anim Pract 2014, 17(2):249-261. 3. Islam AN, Woo PT: Anorexia in goldfish Carassius auratus infected with Trypanosoma danilewskyi. Dis Aquat Org 1991, 11(1):45-48. 4. Johnson R, Curtis S, Dantzer R, Bahr J, Kelley K: Sickness behavior in birds caused by peripheral or central injection of endotoxin. Physiol Behav 1993, 53(2):343-348. 5. Povey S, Cotter SC, Simpson SJ, Lee KP, Wilson K: Can the protein costs of bacterial resistance be offset by altered feeding behaviour? J Anim Ecol 2009, 78(2):437-446. 6. Povey S, Cotter SC, Simpson SJ, Wilson K: Dynamics of macronutrient self‐medication and illness‐induced anorexia in virally infected insects. J Anim Ecol 2014, 83(1):245-255. 7. Adamo SA, Fidler TL, Forestell CA: Illness-induced anorexia and its possible function in the caterpillar, Manduca sexta. Brain Behav Immun 2007, 21(3):292-300. 8. Ayres JS, Schneider DS: The role of anorexia in resistance and tolerance to infections in Drosophila. PLoS Biol 2009, 7(7):e1000150.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Aug 26, Anders von Heijne commented:

      PML in the posterior fossa remain a clinical and radiological challenge. It is important to keep in mind that JCV infections come in different forms. There are some mutations of JCV that seem to target the cerebellar granule cells specifically, causing granular cell neuronopathy, that at least in early stages only show rapid cerebellar atrophy but with obvious cerebellar symtoms. See for example a recent report on JCV GCN in natalizumab treated MS: Agnihotri et al Neurology. 2014 Aug 19;83(8):727-32


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Aug 12, Miguel Lopez-Lazaro commented:

      Most of the patients with advanced cancers die because the drugs used in their treatment have a low ability to kill their cancer cells at concentrations that do not significantly affect their normal cells. Although these patients need drugs that kill their cancer cells selectively, we typically look for drugs that kill cancer cells at low concentrations or that have specific mechanisms of action. These strategies do not reliably predict the ability of a drug to kill cancer cells selectively and many times result in the selection of compounds with low therapeutic potential and in the failure to detect compounds with therapeutic potential.

      In vitro therapeutic potential can be easily and efficiently assessed answering the following question:

      Can my drugs improve the ability of the standard drugs to kill cancer cells without significantly affecting nonmalignant cells from a variety of appropriate tissues? (1)

      In this article, the authors demonstrate that organoiridium complexes improve the ability of platinum complexes to kill cancer cells at low concentrations. They also show that their cytotoxic activity is mediated by a pro-oxidant mechanism of action. One of their complexes could also match the selectivity of cisplatin when tested in ovarian cancer cells versus lung nonmalignant cells.

      In my opinion, additional nonmalignant cell lines (or primary cells) from appropriate tissues should be used to assess the therapeutic potential of these compounds further. If one only uses one cancer cell line and one non-malignant cell line originated from different tissues, the observed selective anticancer effect could be caused by tissue differences in sensitivity. In addition, cells from other tissues commonly affected by chemotherapy could be highly sensitive to the cytotoxic activity of organoiridium complexes. In view of the possible relevance of the results presented by the authors, I think that these additional experiments are worth considering.

      Dr. Lopez-Lazaro

      (1) Lopez-Lazaro, M. Experimental Cancer Pharmacology for Researchers: At What Concentration Should my Drug Kill Cancer Cells so that it has Potential for Cancer Therapy? 2014, ASIN: B00MMO25NM http://www.amazon.com/Experimental-Cancer-Pharmacology-Researchers-Concentration-ebook/dp/B00MMO25NM/ref=sr_1_1?ie=UTF8&qid=1407829198&sr=8-1&keywords="at+what+concentrations+should"


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Apr 24, BSH Cancer Screening, Help-Seeking and Prevention Journal Club commented:

      The HBRC journal club read Scherer et al’s paper with interest. While flu vaccination is not the focus of our work, using metaphors as a manipulation to increase the likelihood of a behaviour and the methods used to test the efficacy of doing so, resonated with our research team. Most of the group were unfamiliar with the metaphor literature and found the introduction to provide a useful summary of the field. The authors present a discussion of the role of risk perceptions and affect, but a more detailed discussion of how the two interact and their complexities might have provided a truer representation of the field.

      The group liked that the authors attempt to measure whether participants were likely to move beyond intention (measuring participants’ desire to receive a reminder email to get a flu vaccination), without having to measure behaviour (which is difficult to do objectively). However, contrary to the authors, the finding that individuals who occasionally get a flu vaccine were more likely to request an email reminder was unsurprising to us because individuals who always get a flu vaccination seemingly do not need reminding. A further strength of the paper is that non-emotive metaphors were considered (the flu as a weed), as this helped dispel the suggestion that the effect was due to vividity or violence (as might be the case with metaphors such as ‘beast’ and ‘riot’). The group wondered if a virus could also be considered to be a metaphor, given its use in computing. Additionally, it may have been of interest if the vaccination itself was also part of the metaphor, for example the flu virus being described as a ‘weed’ and the flu vaccine as ‘weed killer’. In Hauser DJ, 2015 using congruent metaphors to describe an illness and the measure to prevent the illness increased behavioural intentions compared to just using a metaphor to describe the illness.

      While metaphors may increase the vividness of the flu and encourage individuals to engage, the group were concerned about how use of metaphors to manipulate behaviour may not be congruent with informed decision making, instead being considered coercive. We disagreed with the authors suggestion that metaphors might have a use in decision aids, which we believe have a role in helping individuals make informed decisions and not swaying their opinion. We suggested that metaphors might be useful when the aim is to increase individuals’ understanding, rather than increasing intentions to engage in a behaviour. It may also be important to consider the unintended consequences of using metaphors, for example in the cancer field (the focus of our work), describing cancer as a battle may lead to suggestions that people who do not survive the disease did not fight hard enough. However, we acknowledge that metaphors are used ubiquitously in the media, which is difficult to control.

      The group felt that flu vaccination was a complex example to choose to conduct these studies. Flu is a fairly common illness, which might result in participants having an accurate estimate of their risk of contracting the illness or how serious it is. This might explain why the manipulation did not affect the mediators in the main analysis. Individuals are likely to have existing beliefs about vaccination (for example, beliefs about side effects, effectiveness) and the benefits of vaccination might not always be obvious (the individual does not contract the flu - a non event and herd immunity benefiting the population). It would have been helpful to have been informed about the flu vaccination recommendations in the USA where the study was conducted. Cultural differences in how the flu is appraised, treated and prevented between countries might alter the effect of metaphors. For example, in the group’s opinion, the metaphor ‘beast’ was an exaggerated conceptualisation of the flu and felt removed from the actual consequences of the virus.

      Study 3 was perceived by the group to be the most useful study of the paper as it used a validated measure of affect and a larger sample than Study 1. The ecological validity of studies 2 and 3 could have been increased if Study 1 had been a ‘think aloud’ study, whereby the authors could have gained a justification for the mediators of flu vaccination that were tested. The group thought that a strength of the studies is that the authors did consider the possible mediators of any effect of the metaphor. Other mediators that could have been considered include attitudes and a measurement of arousal (engagement). A ‘think aloud’ study might also help to ensure that metaphors are used appropriately, for example in Hauser DJ, 2015, use of an enemy metaphor reduced intentions for self-limiting cancer prevention behaviours (such as stopping smoking or limiting alcohol intake). The group also wondered how novel the ‘novel’ metaphors used in the studies actually were, something that pilot work could have investigated.

      The group thought it was interesting that the findings were not wholly consistent across the studies reported and considered that this could have been a product of differences in the sample. The authors do not describe whether participants were randomised to each condition, and no baseline measurements were reported, both of these factors leave readers unclear about whether the sample was similar across each of the studies. It would also have been helpful to have a justification for the sample size chosen for each of the studies.

      Scherer et al. present a novel paper, which has provided readers with examples of how to measure the impact of using metaphors to increase intentions to receive a flu vaccination. Future work in this field should consider conducting pilot work to ensure the topic, manipulation and measures used are relevant to the population of interest. We caution readers to consider the unintended consequences of using metaphors to manipulate behaviour, including concern about the ethical implications this might have.

      Conflicts of interest We report no conflict of interests and note that the comments produced by the group are collective and not the opinion of any one individual.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 May 17, Maya Guglin commented:

      It is true that patients with advanced HF typically have low blood pressure. That is why almost none of them are tolerating thiazide diuretics. In fact, not a single patient from our cohort was on thiazides, so their contribution to the reported effect of LVADs can be completely excluded. Admittedly, we did not record the doses of ACE inhibitors before and after LVADs, although cardiologists are known for neglecting uptitration of HF meds after LVAD implant.

      However, other publications on the same topic can shed some light on ACE doses, because I am sure some authors did record them. We summarized their findings in our recent paper "What did we learn about VADs in 2014?" published in our newborn "The VAD Journal". The text below is the excerpt from this paper.

      Several reports, including ours, unanimously confirmed the discovery made by Uriel et al.in 2011: diabetes improves after LVAD. Choudhary et al. also found that fasting blood glucose improved from 136 +/- 35 to 108 +/- 29 mg/dl post-LVAD (p < 0.001), and daily insulin dose decreased from 43 +/- 37 to 29 +/- 24 units (p = 0.02). Mohamedali et al. presented similar findings and added that some patients were able to completely discontinue oral hypoglycemics. Other groups published similar findings. The nature of this phenomenon is unclear; however, Koerner et al. measured cortisol and plasma catecholamine levels and found that both decreased after the LVAD implant. (6). This may mean that reduction of the systemic inflammatory and stress response may play a role. Otherwise, improved hemodynamics in either pancreas or peripheral tissues or both may be the cause of improvement of glucose metabolism. In any case, diabetes should not be considered a contraindication to LVAD.

      Uriel N, Naka Y, Colombo PC et al. Improved diabetic control in advanced heart failure patients treated with left ventricular assist devices. European journal of heart failure 2011;13:195-9.

      Choudhary N, Chen L, Kotyra L, Wittlin SD, Alexis JD. Improvement in glycemic control after left ventricular assist device implantation in advanced heart failure patients with diabetes mellitus. ASAIO journal (American Society for Artificial Internal Organs : 1992) 2014;60:675-80.

      Mohamedali B, Yost G, Bhat G. Mechanical circulatory support improves diabetic control in patients with advanced heart failure. European journal of heart failure 2014;16:1120-4.

      Subauste AR, Esfandiari NH, Qu Y et al. Impact of left ventricular assist device on diabetes management: an evaluation through case analysis and clinical impact. Hospital practice (1995) 2014;42:116-22.

      Koerner MM, El-Banayosy A, Eleuteri K et al. Neurohormonal regulation and improvement in blood glucose control: reduction of insulin requirement in patients with a nonpulsatile ventricular assist device. The heart surgery forum 2014;17:E98-102.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2015 May 17, David Keller commented:

      Reduced diuretic doses & increased ACE-inhibitor doses account for some of the improvement in blood sugars after LVAD

      Implanting a left ventricular assist device (LVAD) should reduce the doses of diuretics required to treat congestive heart failure (CHF). Thiazide diuretics are well-known to promote insulin resistance, and are often used with loop diuretics to treat CHF. Therefore, the reduced need for diuretics after LVAD implantation could account for some or all of the observed decrease in blood sugars.

      Also, CHF patients often do not tolerate maximal doses of ACE-inhibitors, due to hypotension. After LVAD implantation, these patients should be able to tolerate a higher dose of ACE inhibitor, which would also have the side effect of improving insulin sensitivity.

      How much of the benefit in blood sugar control in diabetic CHF patients who received LVAD implantation was due to their decreased need for diuretics (especially thiazides) and increased tolerance of ACE-inhibitors? The results of this study should be corrected for these well-known medication side-effects. Only the residual improvements in blood sugar control which are not explained by these medication effects should be attributed to improvement in the CHF itself.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Sep 29, Jan Egger commented:

      Videos demonstrating the interactive real-time segmentation can be found under the following YouTube channel: https://www.youtube.com/c/JanEgger/videos

      Best wishes, Jan

      http://www.janegger.de/


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Mar 19, Salvatore Chirumbolo commented:

      This interesting paper refers to previous mouse models of anxiety and developed a molecular investigation on human neuroblastoma cell line SH-SY5Y to explain mouse behaviour. Why do not use a mouse neuroblastoma model? The authors refer to a paper by Li et al (2009) which is in Chinese and does not deal with the related issue mentioned within the manuscript. probably, the correct ref should be a paper by Li et al on Neuropsychopharmacology 2009. Authors should consider an ERRATUM for their paper, to make it more readable.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 26, Guillaume Filion commented:

      This article is one of the "CISCOM meta-analyses", which are very similar papers written by different authors. For more information about the CISCOM meta-analyses, check the blog post "A flurry of copycast on PubMed" at the following link http://blog.thegrandlocus.com/2014/10/a-flurry-of-copycats-on-pubmed


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 26, Guillaume Filion commented:

      This article is one of the "CISCOM meta-analyses", which are very similar papers written by different authors. For more information about the CISCOM meta-analyses, check the blog post "A flurry of copycast on PubMed" at the following link http://blog.thegrandlocus.com/2014/10/a-flurry-of-copycats-on-pubmed


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 26, Guillaume Filion commented:

      This article is one of the "CISCOM meta-analyses", which are very similar papers written by different authors. For more information about the CISCOM meta-analyses, check the blog post "A flurry of copycast on PubMed" at the following link http://blog.thegrandlocus.com/2014/10/a-flurry-of-copycats-on-pubmed


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 26, Guillaume Filion commented:

      This article is one of the "CISCOM meta-analyses", which are very similar papers written by different authors. For more information about the CISCOM meta-analyses, check the blog post "A flurry of copycast on PubMed" at the following link http://blog.thegrandlocus.com/2014/10/a-flurry-of-copycats-on-pubmed


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Feb 27, Geriatric Medicine Journal Club commented:

      This pharmaceutical company sponsored trial of Mirabegron for treatment of overactive bladder symptoms in older people was discussed at the September 2014 Geriatric Medicine Journal Club (follow #GeriMedJC on Twitter). The full transcript of the conversation can be found here: http://gerimedjc.blogspot.com/2014/09/to-two-articles-critically-appraised.html?spref=tw Highlights include concern for lack of reporting on cognitive outcomes.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 10, Patrick Hiepe commented:

      Congratulations, really nice paper! RPBM-offered information are quite impressive!


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 26, Guillaume Filion commented:

      This article is one of the "CISCOM meta-analyses", which are very similar papers written by different authors. For more information about the CISCOM meta-analyses, check the blog post "A flurry of copycast on PubMed" at the following link http://blog.thegrandlocus.com/2014/10/a-flurry-of-copycats-on-pubmed


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jul 11, Mirko Spiroski commented:

      Retraction

      The article published by Ilankovic et al., 2013 [1] has been retracted by Editor-in-Chief because corresponding author published the similar paper in Psychiatria Danubina in 2014 [2]. An internal investigation has raised sufficient evidence of the originality in the first paper [1] and self plagiarism in the second paper [2]; as such, we retract this article from the literature on request by corresponding author and in accordance with guidelines and best editorial practices from the Committee on Publication Ethics. We apologize to our audience about this unfortunate situation.

      References

      [1] Ilankovic A, Damjanovic A, Ilankovic V, Milovanovic S, Petrovic D, Ilankovic N. Sleep Organisation in Depression and Schizophrenia: Index of Endogenous Periodicity of Sleep as a State Marker. Maced J Med Sci. 2013 Dec 15; 6(4):408-413.

      [2] Ilanković A, Damjanović A, Ilanković V, Filipović B, Janković S, Ilanković N. Polysomnographic sleep patterns in depressive, schizophrenic and healthy subjects. Psychiatr Danub. 2014;26(1):20-6.

      The retraction is located here: http://www.mjms.mk/Online/MJMS_2014_7_2/MJMS.1857-5773.2014-0380.pdf and here: http://www.degruyter.com/view/j/mjms.2014.7.issue-2/mjms.1857-5773.2014.0380/mjms.1857-5773.2014.0380.xml?format=INT

      The retracted paper is located here: http://www.mjms.mk/Online/MJMS_2013_6_4/MJMS.1857-5773.2013-0335.pdf


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Jul 03, Jeffrey Beall commented:

      The text in this article very closely matches the text in an article published in Volume 6, Issue 4 (Dec 2013) of the Macedonian Journal of Medical Sciences. That article is entitled "Sleep Organisation in Depression and Schizophrenia: Index of Endogenous Periodicity of Sleep as a State Marker."

      The other article is located here: http://www.degruyter.com/view/j/mjms.2013.6.issue-4/mjms.1857-5773.2013.0335/mjms.1857-5773.2013.0335.xml?format=INT

      Both articles have six authors. There are four authors common to both papers. Each article also has two authors that are not listed as an author on the counterpart paper.

      I cannot tell which was published first; they both came out at about the same time. I request that both journals investigate this possible case of duplicate publication / plagiarism.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Mar 31, Sergio Stagnaro commented:

      None


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Mar 30, Allison Stelling commented:

      The accuracy isn't totally surprising, mass spec would be quite sensitive to pathogenic phenotype markers like those phosopholipids. These smaller biological molecules- lipids, sugars, etc- are linked to the mechanisms of pathogenesis. As biomarkers go, they're pretty solid.

      What I worry about is that the algorithm they use to sort the data may have a contaminated "healthy vs normal" training set. I see this in a lot of the spectral diagnostic literature. It is making the assumption that the "gold standard" test for the disease is in fact accurate and reliable itself.

      I also see a lot of use of non-open source software in the field. This may hinder our efforts to establish ourselves in the medical realms. We must make these analyses reproducible and reliable across many, many sites for medical work; and that means having many eyes on the code.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2014 Mar 27, Robert Tibshirani commented:

      This paper shows some impressive results, predicting phenoconversion to either amnestic mild cognitive impairment or Alzheimer's disease, from ten lipids in peripheral blood, with 90-95% accuracy over a 2-3 time frame. The research appears to be well done, but as is often the case, it is impossible to judge the strength of the results without digging into the details. The statistical analysis has multiple steps and is quite complicated. The raw data needs to be made available, and ideally, an R script that carries out the calculations in full.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 May 22, University of Kansas School of Nursing Journal Club commented:

      Team 9: Sage Peterson, Jessica Joslin, Dahnika Sachs, Melissa Ryan, Erin Ekholm, Brittney DuBois, Halie McCombs (Class of 2015)

      Background Introduction

      This article focuses on the transnational migration of nurses worldwide and the uneven distribution of nurse labor in a current nursing shortage (Prescott, 2014). This plays in directly with our class discussion on global nurse migration. Our team chose this article because it ties in the reasoning for migration of nurses to the US from other countries with our class discussion of integrating these nurses into our American healthcare system.

      The article brings up the issue of our current nursing shortage and how it is not evenly distributed. There is more demand for nurses in less developed countries because the desire to work there is not as high as in the more developed countries such as the US or UK (Prescott, 2014). In class, we discussed the role of migrating nurses in our own culture and health care system and how important it is to be culturally open and competent. We are seeing a lot of internationally educated nurses migrating to the US to work because we do have a shortage and we are a more desirable place to work than other less wealthy countries. A lot of the nurses that we see migrating into America are from the Philippines and India. These foreign educated nurses help cut down on the nursing shortage here, but they could be potentially causing shortages in the home countries. The article also discusses this and other political “push and pull” that leads to nurse migration.

      Methods

      We were able to find this article by searching “global nursing” in PubMed. The article reviews the literature on nurse migration and its current and future impact on healthcare systems worldwide. There is a focus on the “push/pull” of economic logic and the cause and effect of nurse migration (Prescott, 2014). The concepts of political-economic, historical, and cultural factors are all covered in the article and their impact on nurse migration. The data was collected from reviews of nursing literature in order to analyze and provide directions for future anthropologic studies. The target population that this issue of nursing shortage and migration has the greatest impact on is the global healthcare systems.

      Findings

      What was mainly discussed and found in the article is that we have a big problem with an uneven distribution of nurses globally. This is causing inadequate migration to some countries and an abundance of migration to others. An example of this would be the migration of Philippine educated nurses to the US. Because of this, we are seeing the need to integrate them properly to our healthcare system in order for them to be successful. What also needs to be considered is the impact this has on the sending countries (like the Philippines) that are seeing more and more nurses leaving to work in more developed countries with better jobs and pay (Prescott, 2014). This unbalanced labor is creating problems globally and this study brings up these issues in order to bring attention to the matter for future studies.

      Implications

      The article is relevant to our nursing practice in many ways. It gives us an understanding and reasoning behind nurse migration around the world and gets us thinking about what we can do to solve this issue. Also, since there is so much transnational migration of nurses, it is important for us to be culturally competent and open about providing other culturally different nurses the opportunity to assimilate to our healthcare system. By better understanding the reasoning and method that many foreign nurses have for migrating out of their home countries to others, we are able to assimilate them to the local health care system better.

      References Prescott, M., & Nichter, M. (2014). Transnational nurse migration: Future directions for medical anthropological research. Social Science & Medicine, 107113-123. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.02.026


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Aug 13, Jim Woodgett commented:

      This is an open access article (just click on the full text link). I don't know why this isn't indicated by PubMed.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Mar 13, Jonathan Eisen commented:

      The first author of the paper, Rachel Adams, has a post on the "microbiology of the Built Environment network" blog discussing some background to this study. See Bacteria in a university housing complex


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Mar 26, Dan Laks commented:

      This study ignores the dramatic rise in blood inorganic mercury over that same time period. Why? Inorganic mercury may be a better bioindicator of chronic mercury exposure than organic mercury which is really a bioindicator of recent exposure. Please see this article for a full description of a more accurate assessment of chronic mercury exposure:

      http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24368746


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Mar 23, Adrian Heilbut commented:

      A supplementary website for easily searching and browsing the data presented in this paper is accessible at http://pd.sciencespace.org

      Source code for statistical analyses and plots is available at https://github.com/aheilbut/pdmouse/


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Sep 06, Hilda Bastian commented:

      The authors of this paper state: “Our own findings as well as research by others show that the effect of children on women’s academic careers is so remarkable that it eclipses other factors in contributing to women’s underrepresentation in academic science”.

      This paper fails to support this contention in 5 ways:

      1. Addressing only a subset of the range of factors that potentially contribute to women’s underrepresentation.

      2. Relying on a selected set of literature that fails to discount alternative explanations, in particular that there is no one single factor that accounts for the phenomenon of women’s underrepresentation in science. Multiple contributing factors, even small ones, can contribute to cumulative advantage for men in science (National Academy of Sciences (US), National Academy of Engineering (US), and Institute of Medicine (US) Committee on Maximizing the Potential of Women in Academic Science and Engineering, 2007).

      3. No method to quantify and comparatively weigh contributing factors that could underpin the single remarkable factor hypothesis.

      4. Not satisfactorily demonstrating that motherhood consistently results in high levels of underrepresentation across disciplines of academic science, and not in all other academic careers.

      5. It generalizes to all of academic science, based exclusively on American data of family responsibilities and science careers.

      The authors rely heavily on their previous work: Ceci SJ, 2011. I have addressed that in a PubMed Commons comment (link to comment). That paper also does not contain adequate evidence to sustain the contention of the claim about the motherhood hypothesis presented here.

      The only data sets presented in support of this hypothesis are (in order of appearance):

      • A study including 586 graduate students in 1992 in the US, surveyed again in 2003 and 2004 (Lubinski D, 2006).

      • A figure of the number of ovarian follicles women have by age from birth to 51, overlaid with key scientists’ career stages.

      • A national faculty survey on career and family in 1998 (with over 10,116 respondents across scientific and non-scientific disciplines) (Jacobs, 2004).

      • 2 selected examples of studies from their previous review chosen to illustrate their argument that there is a level playing field for women in the science workforce, along with a blanket claim that I do not believe the evidence in their review supports (Ceci SJ, 2011).

      • A study that included 2 major components (Goulden, 2009):

        (a) Modeling of data from the Survey of Doctorate Recipients (SDR), which had limited data on potential contributing factors to women’s careers (see for example (Bentley 2004). Women with young children had a 4-13% lower odds of achieving tenure than women without, which is not a considerably higher contribution to gender differences than has been in other studies. (Note that age of children is one of the areas with relatively high missing data in the SDR (Hoffer 2002.)

        (b) A survey of 45 female doctoral and postdoctoral at the University of California, including 16 “new mothers”.

      • A survey with 2,503 respondents from 2008/2009 which found that women were more likely than men to wish they had more children (Ecklund EH, 2011) (although it is not included in the article’s list of references, the study was readily identifiable). Williams and Ceci report “Often this regret is associated with leaving the academy”. However, Ecklund and Lincoln report that there was no gender difference in the desire to leave academic science among these respondents. Further, they conclude, “the effect on life satisfaction of having fewer children than desired is more pronounced for male than female faculty, with life satisfaction strongly related to career satisfaction”.

      • A study of people early in their careers, graduating with MBAs from a single US business school between 1990 and 2006. It had a low response rate (31%) and including 629 women (Bertrand, 2010).

      This data basis is inadequate to support the paper’s conclusions and presents highly selected data. The article included a separate extended bibliography, but the basis for the identification and selection of the studies in the bibliography and in the article is not given. In relation to the major review on which they rely (Ceci SJ, 2011), an unsystematic approach and lack of methods to minimize bias has resulted in a very misleading sample of data, and biased reporting and interpretation of that data (see my comment in PubMed Commons).

      Finally, central to the argument presented here is the hypothesis that as societal and policy changes have reduced the impact of blatant and conscious discrimination, the salience of motherhood as a relative barrier to the progression of women’s scientific careers has assumed greater significance.

      However, those same societal changes have also been affecting how people manage and accommodate family responsibilities and careers. For example, later childbirth and fewer children is an ongoing trend in the US (Matthews TJ, 2009, Matthews TJ, 2014), which partially results from, and contributes to, changing attitudes to motherhood and parenting over time. Similarly, increasing workforce participation by women has been changing, and continues to rapidly change, men’s roles in parenting Cabrera NJ, 2000. The authors acknowledge that there has been some accommodation by academic institutions, but their analysis remains largely one-sided.

      For example, this statement is made with neither current nor longitudinal data cited in support: “Men more often have stay-at-home spouses or spouses in flexible careers who bear and raise children while the men are free to focus on academic work”. Indeed, a study they cite in another context found that both men and women scientists with children worked fewer hours than those without children, but similar hours to each other (Ecklund EH, 2011).

      I agree with the authors that much remains to be done to accommodate family responsibilities of all types, not just motherhood. But that will not be a single magic bullet that counteracts the cumulative impact of biases and barriers affecting women related to gender, race, and more as well as family responsibilities. These authors have not made their case for the claim that, “It is when academic scientists choose to be mothers that their real problems start”.

      In addition to comments here on PubMed Commons on the previous review by these authors that supports this paper, I have discussed it on my blog

      Disclosures: I work at the National Institutes of Health (NIH), but not in the granting or women in science policy spheres. The views I express are personal, and do not necessarily reflect those of the NIH. I am an academic editor at PLOS Medicine and on the human ethics advisory group for PLOS One. I am undertaking research in various aspects of publication ethics.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Apr 08, Lydia Maniatis commented:

      This article is a follow-up to Gheiratmand, Meese & Mullen (2013) and is vulnerable to the same criticisms, which can be found here: https://pubpeer.com/publications/23283693

      Other comments: In the first sentence of the article the authors state that: "The processing shape and form begins with the encoding of local orientation information in the visual scene by arrays of neural mechanisms selective for different orientations."

      What is the basis for this statement? The local orientation of what? Of points? Of horizontal rows of points, vertical rows of points, diagonal rows of points, points having identical luminance, points varying in their luminance? How many points per locally oriented item? On what principle are the points linked into rows, so that they can be "detected"? How about subjective contours, or curves? How about orientations in 3D? How about amodal contours?

      Given the well-established fact that non-local, high level principles mediate the percept, the notion that the percept can reveal "low-level" detector mechanisms and their "tuning" lacks, as Teller (1984) would put it, face validity. Even threshold effects have been clearly shown to be stimulus-dependent, i.e. the results of "tried and true" Gabor patches don't generalise.

      In other words, the authors foundational claim is as obsolete and invalid as it could possibly be. But they assert it, and proceed accordingly.

      The absence of a defensible rationale is (as is typical in this category of studies) complemented by a casual approach to assumptions in general (caps mine). Thus, "a von Mises function is used in part because "it IS THOUGHT TO BE the best function to fit to neurophysiological orientation tuning data. (Swindale, 1998)." So basically, Swindale thought this 1998, and Gheiratmand and Mullen apparently think it because Swindale thought it. Nuff said. Similarly: "m is fixed at 4, which has been CONSISTENTLY USED in the literature for approximation of the probability summation rule." I'm sure those other people had a good reason to use it. And: "we use a model involving arrays of orientation and spatial frequency tuned filters whose outputs are combined across the visual extent of the stimulus using a Minkowski summation rule. This method HAS BEEN USED previously to determine orientation and spatial frequency tuning for achromatic stimuli."Well, as long as its been used before. Some people must have thought it was good enough. Summing up: "We have used the classical psychophysical method of subthreshold summation to measure orientation tuning of the visual detectors underlying human colour vision at different spatial frequencies." Because old (classic) is best, e.g. some people think classical music is better than modern, or that old Coke tastes better than new. And if our: "visual detectors at different spatial frequencies" assumption is invalid (which it certainly is), our experiments will never reveal it, thanks to our other insulating, arbitrary (though confirmed by popularity) assumptions and restricted choice of stimulus features, that allow us to interpret our results in terms of those features (whether they are, in fact, relevant or not).


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 26, Guillaume Filion commented:

      This article is one of the "CISCOM meta-analyses", which are very similar papers written by different authors. For more information about the CISCOM meta-analyses, check the blog post "A flurry of copycast on PubMed" at the following link http://blog.thegrandlocus.com/2014/10/a-flurry-of-copycats-on-pubmed


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 26, Guillaume Filion commented:

      This article is one of the "CISCOM meta-analyses", which are very similar papers written by different authors. For more information about the CISCOM meta-analyses, check the blog post "A flurry of copycast on PubMed" at the following link http://blog.thegrandlocus.com/2014/10/a-flurry-of-copycats-on-pubmed


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 16, Claudiu Bandea commented:

      What if we have totally missed the true nature of viruses? (Part II)

      (Due to size limitations, this comment was entered as two parts)

      Another way to react to the critical question raised by Wolkowicz and Schaechter (24) is to invoke the notion that viruses are not even living entities, and come up with “Ten reasons to exclude viruses from the tree of life” (26). As discussed in more detail elsewhere (8), many of the reasons presented by David Moreira and Purificación López-García (26) were rationalized within the framework of the misleading conventional paradigms about their nature and evolution, so their scientific validity is compromised. Interestingly, Moreira and López-García were well aware of the problems with the dogma of viruses as virus particles, as they wrote: “Claverie recently proposed a provocative redefinition of the viral identity wherein the true nature of a virus is not the virion (the infective viral particle)” (26). However, they dismissed the entire issue by justifiably arguing that the solution to the problems associated with the dogma of viruses as virus particles as proposed by Claverie, “The virus factory should be considered the actual virus organism when referring to a virus” (4), is nonsensical, as the identity of an organism should rely solely on its components and properties, not those of its environment (i.e. the host cell; for more discussion on this issue see Ref. 9); incidentally, this rationale also questions the ‘virocell concept,’ which was developed by Patrick Forterre as a novel solution to the misleading dogma of viruses as virus particles (11, 27). Moreover, in their response to a flurry of comments prompted by their article (see the published correspondence associated with Ref. 26 in the journal Nature Rev. Microbiol.), Moreira and López-García explained the reasons for much of the scientific confusion afflicting the current paradigms on the evolution of viruses: “We realize that much of this confusion comes from the fact that many virologists and other biologists are not familiar with the theory and practice of molecular phylogeny.” That might be true. However, generating correct data and observations is only half of the scientific process, the other half is their interpretation; and, as previously discussed (9), the interpretation of their own (presumably valid) molecular phylogeny data by Moreira and López-García was compromised by the current misleading paradigms on their nature and evolutionary origin.

      The allegations outlined here against the conventional paradigms on the nature and evolution of viruses are strong and implicating, climaxing with the assertion that the life and welfare of tens of millions of people suffering from neurodegenerative diseases might have been affected by the constrains imposed by the dogma of viruses as virus particles on understanding the true etiology of these devastating diseases and on the development of preventive and treatment approaches (14-16). It should be expected, therefore, that scientists working in these basic and applied biomedical fields would timely and openly refute or embrace these allegations. That might not happen, though, as it is well recognized by the historians and philosophers of science (28, 29) that the scientific theories, paradigms and dogmas, even if blatantly wrong, have a life of their own, which doesn’t necessarily follow Peter Medawar’s sensible recipe for conducting science: “The scientific method is a potentiation of common sense, exercised with a specially firm determination not to persist in error” (30). However, in this particular case, there is hope that some of the millions of patients affected by neurodegenerative diseases, as well as their family members and friends, might put some pressure on the scientists working these fields to either dismiss or to embrace these allegations.

      References:

      (1) Edwards, RA, Rohwer F. Viral metagenomics. 2005. Nat. Rev. Microbiol. 3:504-510. Edwards RA, 2005

      (2) Suttle C.A. 2007. Marine viruses--major players in the global ecosystem. Nat Rev Microbiol. 5:801-12. Suttle CA, 2007

      (3) Forterre P. 2006. The origin of viruses and their possible roles in major evolutionary transitions. Virus Res. 117:5-16. Forterre P, 2006

      (4) Claverie JM. 2006. Viruses take center stage in cellular evolution. Genome Biol. 7, 110. Claverie JM, 2006

      (5) Koonin EV, Senkevich TG, Dolja VV. 2006. The ancient Virus World and evolution of cells. Biol Direct. 1-27. Koonin EV, 2006

      (6) Bandea CI. 2009. A Unifying Scenario on the Origin and Evolution of Cellular and Viral Domains. Nature Precedings; http://precedings.nature.com/documents/3888/version/1

      (7) Rohwer F, Barott K. 2013. Viral information. Biol Philos. 28:283-297. Rohwer F, 2013

      (8) Bandea CI. 1983. A new theory on the origin and the nature of viruses. Journal of Theoretical Biology 105:591-602. Bândea CI, 1983

      (9) Bandea CI. 2009. The origin and evolution of viruses as molecular organisms. Nature Precedings; http://precedings.nature.com/documents/3886/version/1

      (10) Watson, JD. 1976. Molecular Biology of the Gene. Benjamin-Cummings, Menlo Park.

      (11) Forterre P. 2010. Giant viruses: conflicts in revisiting the virus concept. Intervirology. 53:362-78. Forterre P, 2010

      (12) Legendre M et al. 2014. Thirty-thousand-year-old distant relative of giant icosahedral DNA viruses with a pandoravirus morphology. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 111:4274-9. Legendre M, 2014

      (13) Zimmer C. 2011. A Planet of Viruses. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

      (14) Bandea CI. 1986. From prions to prionic viruses. Med Hypotheses. 20:139-142.Bândea CI, 1986

      (15) Bandea CI. 2009 Endogenous viral etiology of prion diseases. Nature Precedings; http://precedings.nature.com/documents/3887/version/1

      (16) Bandea CI. 2013. Aβ, tau, α-synuclein, huntingtin, TDP-43, PrP and AA are members of the innate immune system: a unifying hypothesis on the etiology of AD, PD, HD, ALS, CJD and RSA as innate immunity disorders. bioRxiv; http://biorxiv.org/content/early/2013/11/18/000604

      (17) Prusiner, SB. 1998. Prions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 95:13363-13383. Prusiner SB, 1998

      (18) Raoult D et al. 2004. The 1.2-megabase genome sequence of Mimivirus. Science. 306:1344-50. Raoult D, 2004

      (19) Philippe N et al. 2013. Pandoraviruses: amoeba viruses with genomes up to 2.5 Mb reaching that of parasitic eukaryotes. Science 341:281-6. Philippe N, 2013

      (20) Hoffmann R et al. 1998. Archaea-like endocytobiotic organisms isolated from Acanthamoeba sp. (Gr II). Endocytobiosis & Cell Res.12, 185.

      (21) Michel R et al. 2003. Endocytobiont KC5/2 induces transformation into sol-like cytoplasm of its host Acanthamoeba sp. as substrate for its own development. Parasitol Res. 90:52-6. Michel R, 2003

      (22) Scheid P et al. 2008. An extraordinary endocytobiont in Acanthamoeba sp. isolated from a patient with keratitis. Parasitol. Res.102, 945, (2008). Scheid P, 2008

      (23) Scheid P, Hauröder B, Michel R. 2010. Investigations of an extraordinary endocytobiont in Acanthamoeba sp.: development and replication. Parasitol Res.106:1371-7. Scheid P, 2010

      (24) Wolkowicz R, Schaechter M. 2008.What makes a virus a virus? Nat Rev Microbiol. 6:643. Wolkowicz R, 2008

      (25) Raoult D, Forterre P. 2008. Redefining viruses: lessons from Mimivirus. Nat Rev Microbiol. 6:315-9. Raoult D, 2008

      (26) Moreira D. & López-García P. 2009.Ten reasons to exclude viruses from the tree of life. Nature Rev. Microbiol. 7:306–311. Moreira D, 2009

      (27) Forterre P. 2013. The virocell concept and environmental microbiology.ISME J. 7:233-6. Forterre P, 2013

      (28) Kuhn TS. 1962. The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

      (29) Popper K. 1963. Conjectures and Refutations: The Growth of Scientific Knowledge. Routledge, London

      (30) Medawar PB. 1969. Induction and Intuition in Scientific Thought. Methuen, London.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Apr 16, Claudiu Bandea commented:

      What if we have totally missed the true nature of viruses? (Part I)

      (Due to size limitations, this comment was entered as two parts)

      Viruses are the most abundant life forms on Earth and the repertoire of their genes is greater than that of cellular organisms (reviewed in 1, 2). Considering also their extraordinary medical and ecological significance as well as their critical role in shaping the evolution of cellular species and their genome (3-7), it would be expected that the paradigms about the nature and evolution of viruses were on solid scientific grounds.

      However, in what might be the most enduring misconception in biology, ever since they were discovered more than a century ago, viruses have been conceptually misidentified with the virus particles and defined based on the physical, biochemical, and biological properties of these particles (8, 9). Because of the dogma of viruses as virus particles, thousands of scientific articles and books about viruses contain errors that border the pseudo-science realm. Take, for example, the following quote, which is representative of the scientific description of viruses: “all viruses differ fundamentally from cells, which have both DNA and RNA, in that viruses contain only one type of nucleic acid, which may be either DNA or RNA” (italics added to all quotes; 10). Despite common knowledge that during their life cycle many viruses have both nucleic acids, even James Watson, the eminent scientist, who arguably knows nucleic acids better than anyone, has fallen victim to the dogma of viruses as virus particles (for additional examples and quotes outlining this dogma see Ref. 9).

      Although the scientific flaws associated with the dogma of viruses as virus particles were outlined more than three decades ago (8), along with a new hypothesis on their nature and evolutionary origin, these flaws have only become acute after the discovery of giant viruses (reviewed in Ref. 11). Perhaps no one has questioned the dogma of viruses as virus particles more explicitly, and in stronger terms, than Jean-Michel Claverie, the senior author of the article by Legendre et al. (12) and one of the leading researchers in the field of giant viruses, who asked: “what if we have totally missed the true nature of (at least some) viruses?” (4). Claverie answered this intriguing question in a rather revealing way: identifying viruses with the virus particles, he wrote, might “be a case of ‘when the finger points to the stars, the fool looks at the finger.”

      As previously emphasized, however, questioning the dogma of viruses as virus particles is challenging (9). After all, it can be argued that, even if scientifically flawed, the dogma has ‘guided’ several generations of scientists to extraordinary discoveries in virology and in related biomedical fields. By analogy, it is also true that many scientific studies about our planet, which is increasingly thought of as “A Planet of Viruses” (13; see also Ref. 7), can be successfully performed in context of the theory that Earth is flat. So, why question the dogma of viruses as virus particles? First, this dogma is scientifically flawed and, therefore, academically unacceptable. More importantly, though, it has constrained progress not only in basic research, such as that on the origin and evolution of viruses and cells, but also in some applied biomedical fields, in which the life and welfare of many people is at stake.

      For example, in the field of neurodegenerative diseases, which affect tens of millions of people and have a negative economic impact that dwarfs the entire global investment in biomedical research, the misleading dogma of viruses as virus particles led to the formulation of the prion hypothesis, which has apparently misdirected much of the thinking and research for decades (14-16). Briefly, in the context of dogma of viruses as virus particles, in order to prove or disprove the viral etiology of Transmissible Spongiform Encephalopathies (TSEs), the scientists searched for viruses (i.e. virus particles containing the viral genome) in the material used for the transmission of the disease. Hundreds of studies using this experimental approach were performed, but no TSE virus was found, which led to the triumph of the prion hypothesis and the dismissal of the virus hypothesis: “the 50-year quest for a virus has failed because it does not exist!” it was exclaimed (17). However, a decade earlier, it was proposed that the scientific rationale and the experimental approach that were used to investigate the etiology of TSEs were misleading; specifically, it was proposed that the so called ‘prion protein’ was encoded by an endogenous virus, and that this virus could not be identified by the experimental approaches that were employed or in the context of the scientific rationale imposed by the dogma of viruses as virus particles; indeed, none of the thousands of endogenous viruses can be identified using these scientifically flawed approaches. Nevertheless, the pseudoscientific aberration imposed by conventional view about the nature and identity of viruses did not stay in the way of making the prion hypothesis one of the top novelties in modern biology and promoted and rewarded accordingly (17).

      Regarding P. sibericum and other giant viruses, the current dogma about the nature and evolution of viruses has delayed their identification for many years, if not decades, and have confused the interpretation of the experimental data and observations in the field. For example, the original mimivirus isolate was classified as a small Gram-positive bacterium for a decade before realized that it was a virus (18). Since then, several viruses with large genomes size have been identified, culminating with the remarkable reports on the discovery of pandoraviruses (19) and P. sibericum (12) by Claverie and his colleagues. Interestingly, it appears that some of these ‘novel’ giant viruses have also been isolated and characterized many years ago, but classified as cellular organisms. Indeed, two free-living amoeba intracellular parasites, labeled KC5/2 and KLaHel, which were discovered by a German research group in samples collected form a water-treatment-plant sample (20, 21) and the inflamed eye of a patient with keratitis (22, 23), respectively, are highly similar P. sibericum and pandoraviruses.

      So, “What makes a virus a virus?” (24). Incited by a new proposal by Didier Raoult and Patrick Forterre for defining the nature of viruses (25), Roland Wolkowicz and Moselio Schaechter (24) have come to realize that the identity of viruses, as traditionally defined (i.e. virus particles), or as proposed by Raoult and Forterre (i.e. capsid-, or virus particle-encoding organisms), is missing the “the most fundamental aspect of what makes a virus a virus: it breaks up and loses its bodily integrity, with its progeny becoming reconstituted after replication from newly synthesized parts.” And, reflecting more broadly on the nature of viruses and on the field of virology, Wolkowicz and Schaechter wrote: “We are surprised from our own experience that the world of virology has not fully embraced this outlook” (24). However, this outlook, along with a comprehensive theory on their evolutionary origin, nature, and identity, has been presented more than three decade ago (8, 9), but until recently (see, for example, Ref. 11) it has received little attention.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 26, Guillaume Filion commented:

      This article is one of the "CISCOM meta-analyses", which are very similar papers written by different authors. For more information about the CISCOM meta-analyses, check the blog post "A flurry of copycast on PubMed" at the following link http://blog.thegrandlocus.com/2014/10/a-flurry-of-copycats-on-pubmed


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 15, Ronald Perez commented:

      Good review on checkpoint inhibitor immuno-oncology, however, I was hoping the authors had mentioned cancer vaccines. I believe cancer vaccines (i.e. T-VEC and MAGE-A3) are also included in the realm of immuno-oncology and deserved to be mentioned in a future review.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Jul 21, Judy Slome Cohain commented:

      The term, avulsion means ‘forcibly detached from its normal point of insertion by either trauma or surgery’. It is unusual nowadays to have a medical term that by definition blames the practitioner for the problem. Probably this term is going to be replaced with cord snapping asap. But whatever the terminology, Yes, cord snapping or avulsion is caused by force. The cord only snaps if you pull too hard on it. If the cord is only 10 -14 cm, it is hard not to pull on it, so even if you dont intend to pull hard, if the cord is very short, you will have a cord snap if you do enough births. If you pay attention and have your clamp readily accessible (which is what you are being paid for) you clamp it right away and lose very little blood. To be perfectly clear, cord snapping after the baby is born is never a problem except when the practitioner is not acting expediently as they should be or if there is no birth attendant present or if the water is so murky that one cannot see what is going on or if the woman is in the middle of a large pool and inaccessible.

      It was not pointed out that after coming out of the uterus, cords keep pulsing nicely in hot water. This is a great advantage when delivering breech babies because the cord keeps pulsing, even after half the baby is out, if it is under hot water, the fetus continues to be well supplied with oxygen. However, since hot water causes cords to keep pulsing, even the ones that snapped, perhaps that is why there are more cases described of babies who delivered into water, who had a snapped cord, who also lost significant amount of blood. But maybe it is not waterbirth, but pool birth. In most pool births, the midwife is not in the pool with the woman, so would not be able to expediently clamp the cord if it snaps. This is not true for bathtub births. This is another reason to recommend bathtub over pools. In a bathtub waterbirth, if the cord snaps, the midwife can easily clamp it. The other advantages of bathtubs over pools are: it is possible to get rid of the feces by draining the bathtub, it is much easier to keep hot, it is easier to get the woman out in the event of shoulder dystocia, and it enables the husband more time to bond with the baby instead of having to empty the pool.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 26, Guillaume Filion commented:

      This article is one of the "CISCOM meta-analyses", which are very similar papers written by different authors. For more information about the CISCOM meta-analyses, check the blog post "A flurry of copycast on PubMed" at the following link http://blog.thegrandlocus.com/2014/10/a-flurry-of-copycats-on-pubmed


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Mar 05, Dale D O Martin commented:

      The authors state that the protein has multiple predicted N-myristoylation sites that probably plays a role in telomere maintenance. However, myristoylation can only occur on N-terminal Glycines, hence the name N-myristoylation. I've commented on this on two other papers by these authors (here http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18071584 & here http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23007995 and the same comment I made is pasted below). I have also contacted the senior author and the editor of the journal regarding the second paper (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23007995), but they have not changed it despite agreeing with me that it was incorrect. So, I am highlighting it here once again.

      Previous comment: It should be noted that N-myristoylation can only occur on N-terminal Glycines, hence the name N-myristoylation. This occurs either co-translationally on the nascent polypeptide following the removal of the initiator Met or it can occur posttranslationally following proteolysis, which exposes a new N-terminal Gly. The latter has only been shown to occur in caspase-cleaved proteins. In this case, the Gly follows an Asp residue where caspase will cleave. The authors here predict internal myristoylation at very unlikely positions. Furthermore, the general consensus sequence for myristoylation is GXXXS/C/T where X is any amino acid, except for large bulky residues, and S/C/T are preferred in position 5 (counting from Gly). The first site they predict is GAAPP and is very unlikely to be myristoylated. Caution should be taken when predicting internal myristoylation sites. Unless it is predicted to be cleaved to expose an N-terminal Gly.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Mar 11, Daniel Kripke commented:

      These wonderful data add important additions to our understanding of long and short sleep. There are some additional issues to which the data might contribute.

      1) It appears that like the Cancer Prevention Studies and many others, this study suggests that the longest survival was associated with sleep durations slightly below the mean. According to Table 5, among females, the risk was consistently just a bit less with -1 hour adjusted sleep than with mean sleep. (Incidentally, perhaps the squared age-adjusted sleep regressions for females would be significant if centered at the minimum risk.) Among males, Table 5 indicated that death risk rose much more rapidly above the mean age-adjusted sleep duration than below it. Long sleep was associated with more risk than short sleep. Would the lowest risk for males be associated with sleep durations about 30 min. below the mean, as was the case in the adult data?

      2) Increased mortality risk has been found associated with delayed sleep phase disorder, that is, with late bedtimes. Is a late bedtime a mortality risk factor in data from these children?

      3) Several studies of risk factors among adults have indicated that use of hypnotic drugs (sleeping pills) is an independent risk factor for increased mortality, though sometimes confounded with short sleep or insomnia. Does this remarkable data set offer any information about mortality risks associated with hypnotics?


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Mar 10, LEE ELLIS commented:

      can the authors or editors please provide an explanation for the retraction. I cannot see the link to any article describing the reasons for the retraction.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jun 19, Sacchetti Maria Luisa commented:

      This study demonstrate a high prevalence of the nerve dysfunction and a significant correlation with the severity of SDB, in acute stroke patients. Due to the recording technique limitations, the Authors do not classify patients' SDB as central or obstructive. As cases affected by previous peripheral neuropathy or previous OSA were excluded, what might have caused the deficit? Hypoglossal nerve function - and more in general upper airways patency- is regulated by serotonin (5HT). Several studies have demonstrated that altered central networks, particularly serotoninergic, can contribute to OSA Brown RE, 2012 as well as to central sleep apnea Buchanan GF, 2010. Moreover, Sunderram et al.Sunderram J, 2000 observed that a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI), paroxetine hydrochloride,may activate motor neurons in the hypoglossal nucleus and increase genioglossal electromyographic (EMG) activity. Therefore we can hypothesize that the hypoglossal nerve dysfunction observed in the study is -at least in part- the direct consequence of stroke on the serotoninergic network. The incidence of depression after stroke<PMID: 18728805 >, together with effectiveness of its treatment independently to the clinical relevance depressive symptoms Mead GE, 2012, partially support this hypothesis.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Oct 01, Anthony Tweedale commented:

      PubMed soon should be adding the published Erratum to this Letter to the Editor (responding to academics' research important to risk assessment (Krauth D, 2013), but as I prompted the Erratum, I wish to add that it, concerning the declared interests (DoI) of two of these industry and industry-affiliated authors (Pr.'s Leist & Boobis), even now fails to say 'industry' or a synonym; whereas my investigations revealed that these two also work regularly for industries.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Apr 05, Marko Premzl commented:

      The third party data gene data set of eutherian Mas-related G protein-coupled receptor genes HG426065-HG426183 was deposited in European Nucleotide Archive under research project "Comparative genomic analysis of eutherian genes" (https://www.ebi.ac.uk/ena/data/view/HG426065-HG426183). The 119 complete coding sequences were curated using tests of reliability of eutherian public genomic sequences included in eutherian comparative genomic analysis protocol including gene annotations, phylogenetic analysis and protein molecular evolution analysis (RRID:SCR_014401).

      Project leader: Marko Premzl PhD, ANU Alumni, 4 Kninski trg Sq., Zagreb, Croatia

      E-mail address: Marko.Premzl@alumni.anu.edu.au

      Internet: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/myncbi/mpremzl/cv/130205/


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jun 16, S Sundar commented:

      Medical paternalism in prostate radiotherapy.

      Authors: Dr S.Sundar & colleagues (E.F, G.W, R.T)

      The MRC RT01 randomised trial has shown no overall survival benefit for escalated doses of radiation in prostate cancer( 1). This complete lack of overall survival benefit for higher doses of radiation is a consistent finding across many well designed randomised trials (2). It is a fundamental radiobiological fact that radiation toxicity is directly proportional to total dose of radiation administered. So, not surprisingly, escalated doses of radiation has been associated with higher toxicity across the randomised trials (2).

      In spite of the lack of overall survival benefit coupled with higher radiation toxicity, escalated doses of radiation have inexplicably become the international standard of care. Various guidelines including National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE) guideline (CG 175) recommend escalated doses of radiation with the NICE guideline making a specific recommendation of at least 74 Grays of radiation.

      If escalated radiation dose is treated in the same way as a pharmacological drug, it certainly would not have been accepted as a universal standard of care due to higher toxicity and lack of overall survival benefit. Medical paternalism seems rife in prostate radiotherapy. A patient preference study from Netherlands has demonstrated that prostate patients prefer lower toxicity and are happy to trade off efficacy for better quality of life. However,more importantly, physicians were poor in predicting this patient preference(3)(4).

      Radiation oncologists need to shed medical paternalism and engage patients. They should explain the pros and cons of escalated doses of radiation, may be with the us of decision aids, and help patients make an informed choice. Radiation oncologists need to demonstrate evidence based medicine particularly when there is an ongoing debate about appropriate use of prostate radiotherapy in urologist owned radiation facilities (5).

      References: 1 Dearnaley DP, Jovic G, Syndikus I, et al. Escalated-dose versus control-dose conformal radiotherapy for prostate cancer: long-term results from the MRC RT01 randomised controlled trial. Lancet Oncol 2014. doi:10.1016/S1470-2045(14)70040-3.

      2 Viani GA, Stefano EJ, Afonso SL. Higher-than-conventional radiation doses in localized prostate cancer treatment: a meta-analysis of randomized, controlled trials. Int J Radiat Oncol Biol Phys 2009; 74: 1405–18.

      3 Van Tol-Geerdink JJ, Stalmeier PFM, van Lin ENJT, et al. Do patients with localized prostate cancer treatment really want more aggressive treatment? J Clin Oncol Off J Am Soc Clin Oncol 2006; 24: 4581–6.

      4 Stalmeier PFM, van Tol-Geerdink JJ, van Lin ENJT, et al. Doctors’ and patients’ preferences for participation and treatment in curative prostate cancer radiotherapy. J Clin Oncol Off J Am Soc Clin Oncol 2007; 25: 3096–100.

      5 Mitchell JM. Urologists’ use of intensity-modulated radiation therapy for prostate cancer. N Engl J Med 2013; 369: 1629–37.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Mar 19, Alexander Lerchl commented:

      The symbol "*" in Fig. 1 is misplaced. According to the text, the "Total time carrying phone/week (hrs)" was not significantly different.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Aug 23, Ben Goldacre commented:

      This trial has the wrong trial registry ID associated with it on PubMed: both in the XML on PubMed, and in the originating journal article. The ID given is NCT0104446. We believe the correct ID, which we have found by hand searching, is NCT01044446.

      This comment is being posted as part of the OpenTrials.net project<sup>[1]</sup> , an open database threading together all publicly accessible documents and data on each trial, globally. In the course of creating the database, and matching documents and data sources about trials from different locations, we have identified various anomalies in datasets such as PubMed, and in published papers. Alongside documenting the prevalence of problems, we are also attempting to correct these errors and anomalies wherever possible, by feeding back to the originators. We have corrected this data in the OpenTrials.net database; we hope that this trial’s text and metadata can also be corrected at source, in PubMed and in the accompanying paper.

      Many thanks,

      Jessica Fleminger, Ben Goldacre*

      [1] Goldacre, B., Gray, J., 2016. OpenTrials: towards a collaborative open database of all available information on all clinical trials. Trials 17. doi:10.1186/s13063-016-1290-8 PMID: 27056367

      * Dr Ben Goldacre BA MA MSc MBBS MRCPsych<br> Senior Clinical Research Fellow<br> ben.goldacre@phc.ox.ac.uk<br> www.ebmDataLab.net<br> Centre for Evidence Based Medicine<br> Department of Primary Care Health Sciences<br> University of Oxford<br> Radcliffe Observatory Quarter<br> Woodstock Road<br> Oxford OX2 6GG


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Sep 01, Francois Cachat commented:

      In this study, the authors demonstrated that the outcome of children with a single kidney is not as good as it was thought previously. As they pointed out rightfully, poor outcome (proteinuria, hypertension, chronic kidney disease (CKD)) is (mostly) related to the addition of other injury to the single kidney such as (recurrent) pyelonephritis or obstruction. That was the case in 4 of 5 patients they described with CKD. Genetics might play a role as well (as with their 5th patient with hypodysplasia). Other studies also reported chemotherapy after Wilms tumor and nephrectomy as a risk factor for poor outcome. In addition to CKD, it would be interesting to know if hypertension (3 patients) and proteinuria (2 patients) was found only in the "high risk" group of patients with a single kidney. It is my understanding that a child with a congenital single kidney, with no other anomalies, with renal length above 95th percentile, and no other aggression such as hypertension or diabetes, is probably not at higher risk than the general population of developping renal disease (but that remains to be demonstrated in long-term follow up cohort studies)(hyperfiltration might be deleterious as well on a long-term basis). Thank you and congratulation to the authors for their work.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Apr 08, Steve Herman commented:

      This abstract, and the authors' press releases, fail to adequately clarify the fact that the hazard ratios shown in the abstract are "adjusted", and do not represent the real risks to real offspring in the population. Some of the "adjustments" are staggering. For example, the actual population risks that were found in this study can be correctly summarized as follows:

      Compared with offspring born to fathers 20 to 24 years old, offspring of fathers 45 years and older were 1.5 times more likely to have autism, .7 times less likely to have ADHD, 1.3 times more likely to have a psychotic disorder, 1.5 times more likely to have bipolar disorder, .9 times less likely to have suicidal behavior, .8 times less likely to have a substance abuse problem, .7 times less likely to have failing grades in school, .9 times less likely to have low educational attainment, .8 times less likely to have a low IQ; and 1.5 times more likely to have some higher education.

      Please see http://ow.ly/v6ehS for more information.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Sep 21, Bernard Baars commented:

      Dear Leonard, --- I admire your range of interesting papers on fundamental questions.

      In regard to esthetic pleasure and behavior, I would call your attention to a sizable evolutionary anthropology literature on the cost of social display of primary and secondary sexual signals --- the classical case being the male peacock.

      Dan Zahavi called this the Handicap Principle in 1975, and the idea is essentially that sexual selection for mating with the fittest available other-gender mates is so important as an evolutionary driver (only comparable to individual survival itself) that hominins like us, and all of our ancestors among primates, mammals and vertebrates, dedicated a great percentage of biological resources to it. The male peacock posing for sexual selection (by the well-camouflaged females) is endangering his life by attracting predators (by blatant visual, auditory, and presumably olfactory signaling.) The female peahen takes no such chances. Thus the male handicaps himself to look beautiful, and interestingly, humans have used peacock feathers historically to decorate themselves as well.

      It is precisely the apparently inutility of esthetic enjoyment that is evolutionarily important, along the lines of Thorstein Veblen's "conspicuous consumption." Biologically, the male peacock is signaling "looking how strong and fertile I am!!!" I can even afford to waste immense personal risk of predation, great metabolic energy, attacks from competing males in heat, the strength to shiver my tail feathers and preen for hours, simply to attract the best female! What healthy offspring we shall have! The easy analogy would be to men who buy muscle cars or Cadillacs when they could drive a mini-car instead. Among recent entertainment stars, Kim Kardashian and her many imitators among women spending hugely on breast and buttocks enlargements imitates varieties of H sapiens sapiens morphology among peoples who evolved large breasts, steatopigous buttocks, and large stomachs in evolutionarily recent times. Fat storage is a great advantage in certain climates (Siberian-descended Inuits and Amerindians are a good example), but encounter handicaps to survival in the face of massive droughts and famine conditions. Since human ancestors are known to have encountered massive drought conditions in the desertification of the Sahara in the millennia prior to the "African Exodus," when the Hss population is thought to have collapsed to 5,000 individuals in North East Africa, famine-adaptibility is plausibly a major Darwinian constraint on human survival.

      (Note that the term "African Exodus" does not apply to African humans who escaped the desertification of the Sahara by migrating southwards, and who never left Africa. Nor does it apply to the peoples who remained sub-Saharan, such as plausibly the Khoi San of the Kalahari Desert. Khoi San body morphology is gracile rather than robust, as befits a desert-dwelling people, and their cultural and personal knowledge of semi-arid survival tactics is vast.)

      Nicholas Wade has also pointed out the fact that tribal peoples very often perform frequent, vigorous and long-lasting community dancing, and universally harbor other-worldly religious beliefs, which are thought to enhance group harmony and therefore survival. (Mating in tribal peoples tends to obey strict kinship rules, either within the birth group, or between allied groups). The very wide distribution of these human cultural habits has been very well studied since the publication of Stephen Brown's Human Universals (1992) (also called Cultural Universals today).

      In human evolution the earliest evidence for artificial body decoration comes from human-related diggings in South Africa of ancient colored clay deposits, thought to have been used for spectacular body decoration by men and women, especially during and after puberty. (At least 200KYA) Personal jewelry involving trading over sizable distances, such as seashells found far from their origins in North Africa, are also ancient. In more recent years mating-related body decoration, hair styles, special clothing, vigorous dancing, music-making, singing and use of instruments (!), seductive movements and gestures, open competition within genders, verbal facility, display of cooking and hunting skills, and an essentially unlimited number of novel attention-catching behaviors can be related to sexual display. Among the American Sioux the male warriors showed their physical size (often 6' or taller), and created new clothing fashions each year (while the women took a more modest role). "Counting coup" --- rushing into an enemy village, physically touching a fierce enemy warrior, and rushing out again to safety was a quantitative measure of masculine heroics. Precisely analogous behavior can be seen today in ever-changing female fashions, in male body building, and in military uniforms for men, including medals and honor ribbons displayed on the left chest, reflecting both combat experience, military skills, and rank in the male hierarchy. The bodily posture of "pride" is also on display (see palace guards throughout Europe, including the Kremlin in Russia). Mammalian positions of pride are anti-gravity postures (head back, body erect, goose-stepping high) which require great physical training, and which oppose the body posures of social defeat, depression and surrender (head down, bowing low, slow non-threatening approach to the victors, etc.) Notice that we instantly recognize those body postures in lions, horses, and humans --- the standard Napoleonic pride statue in European capitals is a proud-looking man on a horse, bearing a sword. The upward direction of the sword, spear or rifle in heroic sculptures may hark back in evolution to the upward-pointing position of the penis during courtship display in our primate relatives. In classical art this is highly visible in 19th century paintings of Napoleon on a white horse, surrounded by battle. The link between male heroics, female fashions, secondary and primary sexual signals, music and the arts is unavoidable in the art of the Romantic period in Europe.

      Notice that this bio-anthropological hypothesis accounts for a number of features of esthetics you have raised in your interesting article.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Aug 30, Jorge H Ramírez commented:

      Ramirez, Jorge H (2014). A story about oxygen nanobubbles (RNS60). figshare. http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.1135796

      No further comments.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 26, Guillaume Filion commented:

      This article is one of the "CISCOM meta-analyses", which are very similar papers written by different authors. For more information about the CISCOM meta-analyses, check the blog post "A flurry of copycast on PubMed" at the following link http://blog.thegrandlocus.com/2014/10/a-flurry-of-copycats-on-pubmed


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jun 19, Swapnil Hiremath commented:

      This guideline was discussed on June 10th 2014 on the open online nephrology journal club, #NephJC, on twitter. Introductory comments are available on PBFluids and at the NephJC website. It was quite a spirited discussion, with participation from nephrologists, clinical pharmacologists, internists, and more. A transcript and three different curated (i.e. Storified) versions of the tweetchat are available at the same NephJC link.

      On June17th 2014, we conducted a video chat via Google Hangout, among the NephJC editors, Dr Richard Sterns and Dr Hatim Hassan, an archived version of which can be viewed on Youtube.

      The highlights of the tweetchat and the hangout were: 1. These guidelines are extensive and exhaustive and will serve as an extremely useful resource for students, residents and practicing physicians. 2. There was widespread agreement that 'asymptomatic' hyponatremia is rarely asymptomatic, and doing away with that qualifier is a good move. 3. The recommendation against use of vasopressin antagonists in chronic hyponatremia is appropriate given lack of superiority in comparison against standard treatment, and possibility of neurological sequelae from rapid correction (and the high cost of these agents remains a concern). 4. The empiric treatment with hypertonic saline in hyponatremic patients with moderate to severe symptoms will be quite handy, particularly since the intricate calculations otherwise needed are often found to be daunting. 5. The lack of strong evidence (made especially apparent by the use of the GRADE methodology) is disappointing, especially given how common hyponatremia is, and highlights a need for future research.

      Interested individuals can track and join in the conversation by following @NephJC or #NephJC, or visit the webpage at NephJC.com.

      This comment is cross-posted at the other two versions of the guidelines also.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jun 19, Swapnil Hiremath commented:

      This guideline was discussed on June 10th 2014 on the open online nephrology journal club, #NephJC, on twitter. Introductory comments are available on PBFluids and at the NephJC website. It was quite a spirited discussion, with participation from nephrologists, clinical pharmacologists, internists, and more. A transcript and three different curated (i.e. Storified) versions of the tweetchat are available at the same NephJC link.

      On June17th 2014, we conducted a video chat via Google Hangout, among the NephJC editors, Dr Richard Sterns and Dr Hatim Hassan, an archived version of which can be viewed on Youtube.

      The highlights of the tweetchat and the hangout were: 1. These guidelines are extensive and exhaustive and will serve as an extremely useful resource for students, residents and practicing physicians. 2. There was widespread agreement that 'asymptomatic' hyponatremia is rarely asymptomatic, and doing away with that qualifier is a good move. 3. The recommendation against use of vasopressin antagonists in chronic hyponatremia is appropriate given lack of superiority in comparison against standard treatment, and possibility of neurological sequelae from rapid correction (and the high cost of these agents remains a concern). 4. The empiric treatment with hypertonic saline in hyponatremic patients with moderate to severe symptoms will be quite handy, particularly since the intricate calculations otherwise needed are often found to be daunting. 5. The lack of strong evidence (made especially apparent by the use of the GRADE methodology) is disappointing, especially given how common hyponatremia is, and highlights a need for future research.

      Interested individuals can track and join in the conversation by following @NephJC or #NephJC, or visit the webpage at NephJC.com.

      This comment is cross-posted at the other two versions of the guidelines also.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 May 14, David Keller commented:

      Still waiting for anyone to answer my criticisms of this USPSTF report

      When I first read the 2014 USPSTF update on vitamins for disease prevention, I expected, based on the headlines, to find evidence that there is no reason to take a multivitamin. Instead, I became convinced by the data presented by the USPSTF that the evidence of benefits versus harms favors men over 50 taking a multivitamin to prevent cancer and possibly reduce overall mortality. At the very least, the USPSTF would be fully justified in recommending that men over 50 who do not consume a diet rich in vegetables and fruits should consider adding a multivitamin. For some reason, most editorials and comments have completely ignored the significant reductions in cancer for men randomized to multivitamins in the 2 studies cited by USPSTF, and the significant reduction in overall mortality for men randomized to the high dose multivitamin tested in the French study. Instead, we saw headlines and editorials stating or implying that we now have proof that multivitamins are useless. I request that an expert in this area reply to my comments, giving good reasons why you are not convinced by the data we have, and what it would take to convince you. If you are knowledgeable in this area, and especially if you are a member of the USPSTF, I would greatly appreciate your pointing out where my thinking on this issue is wrong or even debatable. For details, data and references, please see my Open Letter to the USPSTF on the following PubMed Commons web page:

      http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24566474#cm24566474_4093

      Lastly, it is not helpful to tag a comment as "not helpful" without specifying why. PubMed Commons should foster meaningful debate, not merely anonymous unexplained contradiction of each other.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Apr 23, David Keller commented:

      Open letter to the USPSTF: the evidence shows multivitamins reduce cancers in men

      The United States Preventative Services Task Force (USPSTF) recently updated their report on multivitamin supplements, and again found "insufficient evidence" to recommend their general use. The USPSTF report dismissed the significant reduction in cancers seen in men in both the SU.VI.MAX and Physicians' Health Study (PHS II), stating: "The lack of effect in women and the use of different supplement formulations in the 2 trials make extrapolating these findings to the general population difficult."(1) However, since men constitute approximately half of the general population, there is no need to extrapolate any benefit which men obtain from supplements to women. A benefit proven to accrue to men would justify a recommendation that men should take supplements, and the USPSTF could state that the issue was still unresolved for women. After all, the USPSTF makes other sex-specific recommendations, such as aortic ultrasound screening for male smokers but not females. Alternatively, the USPSTF may have doubted the biological plausibility of supplements preventing cancers in men but not in women. The SU.VI.MAX authors explained that difference by pointing out that women have a higher baseline nutritional status than men (2), and thus have less to gain by adding a multivitamin supplement. The USPSTF failed to mention or rebut that seemingly reasonable explanation.

      The second reason USPSTF gave for not being able to interpret the results of the PHS II and SU.VI.MAX studies was that these two clinical trials tested 2 different multivitamin supplement formulations. However, the 5 antioxidants used in the SU.VI.MAX supplement are a subset of the micronutrients in the Centrum Silver administered in PHS II. I have pointed out in PubMed Commons (3) that there is evidence of a dose-response effect when one compares the effects of the Centrum Silver supplement versus the higher-dose SU.VI.MAX supplement, with regard to the significant reduction in cancer seen in men in both studies. Each of the 5 ingredients of the SU.VI.MAX supplement (vitamin C, vitamin E, beta carotene, selenium and zinc) is present in a higher dose than in Centrum Silver, yielding a correspondingly higher reduction in the risk of cancer in men (see Table 1). A dose-response effect tends to corroborate the findings of the individual studies, and it also suggests the need for a dose-ranging study of the SU.VI.MAX supplement. Would increasing the doses of these 5 antioxidant nutrients reduce cancer rates and mortality even further in men? Would a significant effect in women become evident?

      Lastly, the updated USPSTF report completely omitted any mention of the significant reduction in all-cause mortality which benefitted the men taking the SU.VI.MAX supplement (2).

      At this time, there is consistent evidence from 2 large, prospective, randomized, placebo-controlled trials that the low-dose multivitamin supplement used in PHS II significantly reduces the incidence of cancer in men, and that the higher-dose SU.VI.MAX supplement (consisting of 5 antioxidant nutrients in higher doses than in Centrum Silver) reduces cancer rates even more in men, and adds a significant reduction in all-cause mortality. The USPSTF report did not indicate how many more studies must replicate these results before they find the data persuasive. Until such trials are completed, the evidence we have indicates that men will continue to suffer cancers and deaths which are preventable by taking a multivitamin. If future studies fail to replicate these results (perhaps as a result of improved nutritional status among men) then the only known harm or cost would be the 6 cents per pill retail price of the Centrum multivitamin used in PHS II, or the inexpensive combination of supplements which duplicates the SU.VI.MAX formula.

      The USPSTF should modify their assessment of multivitamin supplements to reflect the significant dose-related benefits multivitamins have demonstrated for men over the age of 50, who can benefit from reduced cancer rates and overall mortality, according to the best evidence we have. The USPSTF should call for dose-ranging studies to determine whether the benefits of the five antioxidants administered in SU.VI.MAX can be increased by increasing their doses. If the USPSTF has evidence or reasons other than those I have refuted above for not recommending multivitamins for men over age 50, they should make them public.

      At the very least, the USPSTF report should be amended to include the fact that overall mortality was significantly decreased in men taking the SU.VI.MAX supplement. The absence of that data from their report was a disservice to men who are deciding whether or not to take a supplement.

      Table 1: Dose-Response Effect? Vitamin doses versus relative risk of cancer:

      Centrum Silver 50+............SU.VI.MAX multivitamin....................................................

      Beta-Carotene 1000 IU.......Beta-Carotene 6mg = 9960 IU...............................................

      Vitamin C 60 mg.................Vitamin C 120 mg..........................................................

      Vitamin E 50 IU...................Vitamin E 30 mg = 67 IU ..................................................

      Zinc 11 mg..........................Zinc 20 mg................................................................

      Selenium 55 mcg..................Selenium 100 mcg..........................................................

      RR of cancer = 0.93............RR of cancer = 0.69.......................................................

      References

      1: Moyer VA. Vitamin, Mineral, and Multivitamin Supplements for the Primary Prevention of Cardiovascular Disease and Cancer: U.S. Preventive Services Task Force Recommendation Statement. Ann Intern Med. 2014 Feb 25. doi: 10.7326/M14-0198. [Epub ahead of print] PubMed PMID: 24566474.

      2: Hercberg S, Galan P, Preziosi P, Bertrais S, Mennen L, Malvy D, Roussel AM, Favier A, Briançon S. The SU.VI.MAX Study: a randomized, placebo-controlled trial of the health effects of antioxidant vitamins and minerals. Arch Intern Med. 2004 Nov 22;164(21):2335-42. Erratum in: Arch Intern Med. 2005 Feb 14;165(3):286. PubMed PMID: 15557412.

      3: Keller DL. Multivitamins: "Expensive Urine" or inexpensive cancer prevention? Comment posted on PubMed Commons, last edit dated 3/29/2014. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24566474#cm24566474_3700


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2014 Apr 19, David Keller commented:

      Men taking multivitamins had significantly lower overall mortality and lower cancer risk

      The USPSTF report did not disclose that men randomized to antioxidant supplements in the large prospective SU.VI.MAX trial had significantly lower risk of death, in addition to significantly fewer overall cancers than men assigned to placebo (1). This important information should be considered by any man over age 50 who is deciding whether or not to take multivitamins. In addition, please see my accompanying comments regarding the apparent dose-response effect of the SU.VI.MAX supplement with regard to cancer prevention in men.

      (1) Hercberg S, Kesse-Guyot E, Druesne-Pecollo N, Touvier M, Favier A, Latino-Martel P, Briançon S, Galan P. Incidence of cancers, ischemic cardiovascular diseases and mortality during 5-year follow-up after stopping antioxidant vitamins and minerals supplements: a postintervention follow-up in the SU.VI.MAX Study. Int J Cancer. 2010 Oct 15;127(8):1875-81. doi: 10.1002/ijc.25201. PubMed PMID: 20104528.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    4. On 2014 Mar 29, David Keller commented:

      Multivitamins: "Expensive Urine" or inexpensive cancer prevention?

      The USPSTF guideline statement on multivitamins and cancer risk (1) includes the following statements:

      Statement 1: “Two large trials, the Physicians' Health Study II (PHS II) and the SU.VI.MAX (Supplementation in Vitamins and Mineral Antioxidants) study, showed a decrease in overall cancer incidence in men (pooled unadjusted relative risk, 0.93 [95% CI, 0.87 to 0.99])“

      Statement 2: “Use of dietary supplements is common in the U.S. adult population. Forty-nine percent of adults used at least 1 dietary supplement between 2007 and 2010, and 32% reported using a multivitamin–multimineral supplement. Supplement use is more common among women and older adults than men and younger adults.”

      Statement 3: “The lack of effect in women and the use of different supplement formulations in the 2 trials make extrapolating these findings to the general population difficult.”

      The lack of benefit of multivitamins and mineral supplements (MVMS) in women might have been due to the higher background use of MVMS by women (Statement 2). Intention-to-treat analysis would count women in control groups who took MVMS in violation of experimental protocol as if they were not taking MVMS; this would tend to reduce the apparent benefit of MVMS in women, perhaps explaining Statement 3. A hypothesis-generating per-protocol analysis of these trials is warranted; if an anti-cancer effect of MVMS is thereby discerned in women, a more rigorous follow-up study would be justified.

      As a male physician, I will continue to take a MVMS, based on Statement 1, unless evidence emerges which disproves the results of these 2 large trials. While awaiting further information, and considering the minimal potential harms and cost of multivitamins, and the possible benefits, I see no reason to dissuade women from taking a MVMS.

      The USPSTF report also states that "the use of different supplement formulations in the 2 trials makes extrapolating these findings to the general population difficult", which refers to the fact that the Physician's Health Study tested "a commercially available multivitamin that contained 30 ingredients" (which was Centrum Silver), while the SU.VI.MAX Study (2) tested a supplement which "included nutritional doses of vitamins C and E plus β-carotene, selenium, and zinc".

      In fact, the 5 nutrients included in the SU.VI.MAX supplement are a subset of the 30 nutrients included in Centrum Silver, and each of these 5 nutrients is present at a substantially higher dose in the SU.VI.MAX supplement than in Centrum Silver (3). Furthermore, use of the SU.VI.MAX supplement led to a larger reduction in the average relative risk of cancer than did the use of Centrum Silver, suggesting a possible dose-response effect for the ingredients of the SU.VI.MAX supplement with regard to lowering the relative risk of cancer. See Table 1:

      Table 1: Dose-Response Effect? Vitamin doses versus relative risk of cancer:

      Centrum Silver 50+............SU.VI.MAX multivitamin....................................................

      Beta-Carotene 1000 IU.......Beta-Carotene 6mg = 9960 IU...............................................

      Vitamin C 60 mg.................Vitamin C 120 mg..........................................................

      Vitamin E 50 IU...................Vitamin E 30 mg = 67 IU ..................................................

      Zinc 11 mg..........................Zinc 20 mg................................................................

      Selenium 55 mcg..................Selenium 100 mcg..........................................................

      RR of cancer = 0.93............RR of cancer = 0.69.......................................................

      A dose-response effect, if present, would tend to support the hypothesis that the decrease in cancers observed in these 2 studies was real and not due to the play of chance. Proving a dose-response effect requires more than just two data points taken from 2 different studies on 2 different populations. However, this observed trend suggests the need for a follow-up study to determine whether further increases in the doses of the ingredients of the SU.VI.MAX supplement will lead to further declines in the average relative risk of cancer. Of course, all relevant cautions must be taken, such as not administering beta-carotene to persons with a smoking history.

      References

      1: Moyer VA. Vitamin, Mineral, and Multivitamin Supplements for the Primary Prevention of Cardiovascular Disease and Cancer: U.S. Preventive Services Task Force Recommendation Statement. Ann Intern Med. 2014 Feb 25. doi: 10.7326/M14-0198. [Epub ahead of print] PubMed PMID: 24566474.

      2: Hercberg S, Galan P, Preziosi P, Bertrais S, Mennen L, Malvy D, Roussel AM, Favier A, Briançon S. The SU.VI.MAX Study: a randomized, placebo-controlled trial of the health effects of antioxidant vitamins and minerals. Arch Intern Med. 2004 Nov 22;164(21):2335-42. Erratum in: Arch Intern Med. 2005 Feb 14;165(3):286. PubMed PMID: 15557412.

      3: Centrum Silver 50+ website, accessed on 3/24/2014:<br> http://www.centrum.com/centrum-silver-adults-50-plus#tablets

      4: The following websites were referenced for converting vitamin doses from mg to IU, accessed on 3/24/2014: http://ods.od.nih.gov/factsheets/VitaminE-HealthProfessional/

      http://ods.od.nih.gov/factsheets/VitaminA-HealthProfessional/

      http://dietarysupplementdatabase.usda.nih.gov/ingredient_calculator/help.php


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.